Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Grossmann, Tobias
Lloyd-Fox, Sarah
and
Johnson, Mark H.
2013.
Brain responses reveal young infants’ sensitivity to when a social partner follows their gaze.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,
Vol. 6,
Issue. ,
p.
155.
Reddy, Vasudevi
Markova, Gabriela
Wallot, Sebastian
and
Tsakiris, Manos
2013.
Anticipatory Adjustments to Being Picked Up in Infancy.
PLoS ONE,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 6,
p.
e65289.
Kourtis, Dimitrios
Knoblich, Günther
and
Sebanz, Natalie
2013.
History of interaction and task distribution modulate action simulation.
Neuropsychologia,
Vol. 51,
Issue. 7,
p.
1240.
Northoff, Georg
2013.
What is culture? Culture is context-dependence!.
Culture and Brain,
Vol. 1,
Issue. 2-4,
p.
77.
Jack, Anthony I.
Dawson, Abigail J.
Begany, Katelyn L.
Leckie, Regina L.
Barry, Kevin P.
Ciccia, Angela H.
and
Snyder, Abraham Z.
2013.
fMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive domains.
NeuroImage,
Vol. 66,
Issue. ,
p.
385.
Froese, Tom
Gershenson, Carlos
and
Rosenblueth, David A.
2013.
The dynamically extended mind.
p.
1419.
Stanley, Damian A.
and
Adolphs, Ralph
2013.
Toward a Neural Basis for Social Behavior.
Neuron,
Vol. 80,
Issue. 3,
p.
816.
Gallese, Vittorio
2013.
Mirror neurons, embodied simulation and a second-person approach to mindreading.
Cortex,
Vol. 49,
Issue. 10,
p.
2954.
Krämer, Katharina
Bente, Gary
Luo, Siyang
Pfeiffer, Ulrich J.
Han, Shihui
Vogeley, Kai
and
Zalla, Tiziana
2013.
Influence of Ethnic Group-Membership and Gaze Direction on the Perception of Emotions. A Cross-Cultural Study between Germany and China.
PLoS ONE,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 6,
p.
e66335.
Gianelli, Claudia
Lugli, Luisa
Baroni, Giulia
Nicoletti, Roberto
Borghi, Anna M.
and
Paterson, Kevin
2013.
The Impact of Social Context and Language Comprehension on Behaviour: A Kinematic Investigation.
PLoS ONE,
Vol. 8,
Issue. 12,
p.
e85151.
Wiltshire, Travis J.
and
Fiore, Stephen M.
2014.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience in Human–Machine Systems: A Roadmap for Improving Training, Human–Robot Interaction, and Team Performance.
IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems,
Vol. 44,
Issue. 6,
p.
779.
Ansuini, Caterina
Cavallo, Andrea
Bertone, Cesare
and
Becchio, Cristina
2014.
The visible face of intention: why kinematics matters.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 5,
Issue. ,
Kuzmanovic, Bojana
Schilbach, Leonhard
Georgescu, Alexandra L.
Kockler, Hanna
Santos, Natacha S.
Shah, N. Jon
Bente, Gary
Fink, Gereon R.
and
Vogeley, Kai
2014.
Dissociating animacy processing in high-functioning autism: Neural correlates of stimulus properties and subjective ratings.
Social Neuroscience,
Vol. 9,
Issue. 3,
p.
309.
Poulsen, Andreas Trier
Kamronn, Simon
Parra, Lucas C.
and
Hansen, Lars Kai
2014.
Bayesian correlated component analysis for inference of joint EEG activation.
p.
1.
Williamson, Kellie
and
Sutton, John
2014.
Brain Theory.
p.
107.
Wiese, Eva
Müller, Hermann J.
and
Wykowska, Agnieszka
2014.
Social Robotics.
Vol. 8755,
Issue. ,
p.
370.
Bhui, Kamaldeep
2014.
From the Editor's desk.
British Journal of Psychiatry,
Vol. 205,
Issue. 4,
p.
336.
Huis in ‘t Veld, Elisabeth M. J.
Van Boxtel, Geert J. M.
and
de Gelder, Beatrice
2014.
The Body Action Coding System I: Muscle activations during the perception and expression of emotion.
Social Neuroscience,
Vol. 9,
Issue. 3,
p.
249.
Schnell, Knut
2014.
Mentalizing Functions Provide a Conceptual Link of Brain Function and Social Cognition in Major Mental Disorders.
Psychopathology,
Vol. 47,
Issue. 6,
p.
408.
Sevdalis, Vassilis
and
Keller, Peter E.
2014.
Know thy sound: Perceiving self and others in musical contexts.
Acta Psychologica,
Vol. 152,
Issue. ,
p.
67.
Target article
Toward a second-person neuroscience1
Related commentaries (28)
A mature second-person neuroscience needs a first-person (plural) developmental foundation
A second-person approach cannot explain intentionality in social understanding
Advancing the neuroscience of social emotions with social immersion
Brain games: Toward a neuroecology of social behavior
From synthetic modeling of social interaction to dynamic theories of brain–body–environment–body–brain systems
From the bottom up: The roots of social neuroscience at risk of running dry?
Further steps toward a second-person neuroscience
Interaction versus observation: A finer look at this distinction and its importance to autism
It takes two to talk: A second-person neuroscience approach to language learning
Merging second-person and first-person neuroscience
Mirror neurons are central for a second-person neuroscience: Insights from developmental studies
On projecting grammatical persons into social neurocognition: A view from linguistics
Parameterising ecological validity and integrating individual differences within second-person neuroscience
Reciprocity between second-person neuroscience and cognitive robotics
Second person neuroscience needs theories as well as methods
Second-person neuroscience: Implications for Wittgensteinian and Vygotskyan approaches to psychology
Second-person social neuroscience: Connections to past and future theories, methods, and findings
Social affordances in context: What is it that we are bodily responsive to?
Social affordances: Is the mirror neuron system involved?
Social cognition is not a special case, and the dark matter is more extensive than recognized
Social perception and “spectator theories” of other minds
Talking to each other and talking together: Joint language tasks and degrees of interactivity
The brain as part of an enactive system
The second person in “I”-“you”-“it” triadic interactions
The use of non-interactive scenarios in social neuroscience
Toward a neuroscience of interactive parent–infant dyad empathy
What we can learn from second animal neuroscience
Why not the first-person plural in social cognition?
Author response
A second-person neuroscience in interaction1