Hostname: page-component-5b777bbd6c-w9n4q Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-06-20T09:53:31.522Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perspective integration capability: A valid and reliable measurement instrument for assessing knowledge integration readiness in interdisciplinary collaborations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 April 2025

Maritza Salazar Campo*
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor, Organizations & Management, UCI Paul Merage School of Business, Irvine, CA, USA
Theresa K. Lant
Affiliation:
Emeritus Distinguished Professor of Management, Pace University, New York, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: M. Salazar Campo; Email: maritza.salazar@uci.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Work in science, medicine, and engineering increasingly relies on collaborations among diverse experts to solve complex problems. Despite the importance of interprofessional training and practice to enhance collaboration and knowledge integration, there is a lack of a conceptually meaningful, valid, and reliable measure of individual capacity for interdisciplinary knowledge integration. This study contributes a conceptual framework and empirical tool to facilitate both research and practice of interdisciplinary collaborations.

Methods:

We conduct a three-phase, five-study investigation to develop and validate a measure of individual perspective integration capability (PIC), which assesses individual willingness and ability to integrate knowledge with others during collaborative work. Phase 1 includes item generation and reduction in three studies using different samples of respondents. Phase 2 demonstrates convergent and discriminant validity with conceptually related and unrelated constructs, using a separate sample of respondents. Phase 3 tests criterion-related validity and mediation by examining the multilevel relationships between PIC and key antecedents and outcomes, using data from a unique sample of research scientists in interdisciplinary medical research teams.

Results:

Across the three phases of our study, the results demonstrate support for the PIC instrument’s factor structure, reliability, and validity. We also demonstrated that the PIC construct has important implications for individuals engaged in interdisciplinary collaborations.

Conclusions:

Having a conceptually meaningful, valid, reliable, and easily administered survey instrument will facilitate further study of interdisciplinary collaboration, and the development and evaluation of integration efforts of teams engaged in convergent and translational initiatives.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Association for Clinical and Translational Science

Introduction

Knowledge-based work in domains such as science, medicine, and engineering increasingly relies on collaboration among diverse experts to solve complex problems. Knowledge experts often collaborate in temporary teams on multiple projects simultaneously [Reference Mistry, Kirkman and Moore1,Reference O’Leary, Mortensen and Wooly2]. These collaborations require more than simply sharing expertise; they necessitate deep integration across different knowledge areas [Reference Sanders3Reference Petersen, Ahmed and Pavlidis5]. Failure to integrate knowledge across specialties can hinder innovation, including the development of diagnostics and treatments for complex diseases. Despite the importance of interprofessional training and practice, collaboration and knowledge integration remain challenging, in large part due to the nature of developing and practicing professional expertise [Reference Kumarasamy and Sanfillippo6,Reference Rider, Chou and Abraham7].

Pursuing professional expertise is a continuous journey of learning and improvement, leading to specialization [Reference Snell and Fyfe8]. Acquiring expertise is rigorous and involves delving into specialized language, thinking, and assessment methods [Reference Hall, Vogel and Stipelman9]. Experts learn to demonstrate and defend their status and knowledge, serving as role models for novices. Collaborating across professional areas can be hindered by biases that develop along with the development of specialized knowledge [Reference Rider, Chou and Abraham7]. Becoming a disciplinary expert can lead to a fixed mindset, wherein experts view the standards of their profession as superior to different ways of thinking [Reference Lombardo, Ehlers and Lutz10]. While specialized expertise is essential, it is also crucial to maintain awareness and openness to knowledge outside one’s area of expertise. Highly trained professionals tend to frame problems in terms of their own expertise. This phenomenon, known as the myopia of learning [Reference Levinthal and March11], can create cognitive barriers that make it less likely for one to consider perspectives from different areas of specialization [Reference Clark12].

An understanding of how individual cognitive processing can facilitate or impede knowledge utilization and integration is crucial to the success of interdisciplinary collaborations. Cognitive processing is a critical determinant of knowledge integration among diverse collaborators. Sharing expertise is not sufficient for integration to emerge. Knowledge integration requires deep listening, openness to the contributions of others, thoughtful reflection, and a willingness to update one’s current understanding [Reference Harvey13]. It is likely that collaborative knowledge integration begins with a cognitive shift at the individual level, where one contributor recognizes a potential link between their expertise and that of the other. This curiosity about the connection between previously disconnected knowledge unlocks innovative potential, allowing the emergence of new ideas and breakthrough solutions.

Scholars have assumed that the individual-level skill of perspective-taking contributes to forming collective understanding [Reference Hoever, van Knippenberg and van Ginkel14]. However, there is still a lack of clarity regarding how considering others’ perspectives [Reference Parker and Axtell15] leads to a shift in an individual’s cognitive structure. Being receptive to and contemplating the contributions of others is a cognitive capacity that is often overlooked. We refer to this aptitude as a person’s perspective integration capability (PIC). In developing this construct, we drew upon existing theories of collective cognition [Reference Gibson16] and cognitive processing [Reference Homan, van Knippenberg and Van Kleef17]. This conceptualization enables an assessment of the degree to which an individual considers inputs from others and how they adjust their current understanding based on the new knowledge received. Building on the conceptual [Reference Falk-Krzesinski, Borner and Contractor18,Reference Salazar, Lant and Fiore19] and empirical [Reference Lotrecchiano, Schwartz and Falk-Krzesinski20,Reference Misra, Stokols and Cheng21] literature on interdisciplinary collaboration, this study contributes a conceptual framework and empirical tool to facilitate both research and practice of interdisciplinary collaborations. The instrument we develop assesses an individual’s cognitive openness to the contributions of others and their capacity to integrate new ideas. Conceptually, we set out to develop an instrument that assesses three aspects of an individual’s ability to integrate knowledge with others. First, it measures the extent to which an individual considers the merits of the ideas contributed by collaborators. Once these ideas have been evaluated, the second aspect assesses the individual’s ability to assimilate aspects of those ideas into their task-relevant cognitive schemas. Finally, the third aspect evaluates the individual’s capacity to accommodate aspects of inputs by adjusting task schemas to be more consistent with or complementary to those of collaborators. Our approach addresses the need for practical and reliable measurement tools to assess an individual ability to integrate knowledge with others during interdisciplinary collaborations.

Materials and methods

We conducted a three-phase, five-study investigation to develop an easily used survey instrument of perspective integration capability (PIC) with desirable psychometric properties. We followed conventional steps for scale development and validation. A large pool of items relevant to knowledge consideration, sharing, elaboration, assimilation, and accommodation, were drawn from the literature on team cognition [Reference Gibson16, Reference Homan, van Knippenberg and Van Kleef17]. The process of scale development and validation typically begins with a large pool of relevant items, which are then reduced and refined using exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, followed by assessment of convergent and discriminant validity. Details of this process are provided in the Supplement.

Phase 1 includes three studies to develop and validate the PIC scale through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. Results show that PIC has two components: Knowledge Consideration (KC) and Knowledge Assimilation/Accommodation (KAA). Although existing literature [Reference Gibson16] suggests that knowledge assimilation and knowledge accommodation are two distinct concepts, our results suggest that these concepts overlap significantly. Phase 2 provides evidence for the psychometric properties of PIC by assessing convergent and discriminant validity. In Phase 3, we examine the criterion validity of the PIC instrument. Criterion validity is the last step of a full development and assessment of an instrument’s psychometric properties. It is assessed by analyzing how an instrument behaves in a conceptual model. The PIC instrument developed in this paper focuses on the cognitive aspects of knowledge integration. We use the broader conceptual framework of integrative capacity [Reference Salazar, Lant and Fiore19] to explore antecedents and outcomes of PIC using structural equation modeling. The integrative capacity framework suggests how social factors and team emergent states influence knowledge integration in interdisciplinary collaborations. The PIC scale is intended to assess the knowledge integration component of the integrative capacity framework.

Phase 1: item generation and reduction

In Phase 1, we conducted three studies to develop and validate the PIC scale through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. The Phase 1 process involved generating a large list of items, modified from existing measures in the literature on cognitive processes [Reference Salazar, Lant and Fiore19], and reduced this number to a feasible set with high content validity [Reference Floyd and Widaman22]. To generate the initial item pool, broad item inclusion criteria were used to circumvent a potential attenuation paradox [Reference Loevinger23] and used a deductive approach based on existing theory [Reference Hinkin24]. Details of this process are described in the Supplement. This process yielded 19 items relevant to KC, and 8 items relevant to KAA. All items use 7-point Likert scales with response anchors ranging from “Strongly Disagree” (1) to “Strongly Agree” (7). The pool of initial items and additional details and references describing the methodology are provided in the Supplement.

Study 1. Exploratory Factor Analysis. The Study 1 sample was recruited using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk). MTurk was selected based on the high diversity of its respondent pool [Reference Buhrmester, Kwang and Gosling26], given that participant diversity is highly desirable in the initial phase of scale construction [Reference Clark and Watson27]. Respondents were selected for participation if they reported having previously collaborated with team members with diverse, difficult-to-integrate perspectives. They were asked to recall this experience when answering the survey questions. Further data screening filters included answering the entire survey and responding to a neutral language “human intelligence task” that ensured the respondent was not a robot and obscured both the purpose of the research and the respondent qualifications sought by the researchers, thus minimizing self-selection, social desirability, and reactance biases [Reference Schulze, Krug and Schader25]. Study 1 also included items that served as participant screening (4 items), attention checks for data quality (1 item), or were qualitative or demographic items (2 and 5 items, respectively). Two hundred respondents were deemed suitable for inclusion in statistical analyses (53% male, 47% female, mean age 34.6). Iterative exploratory factor analyses (EFA) were conducted to create a cohesive, feasible set of PIC items and to identify factors with demonstrable structural validity and high reliability [Reference Comrey28].

Table 1 shows the results of the EFA for Study 1. Principal axis factoring extraction method was used. The rotation method used was Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. The rotation converged in three iterations. It shows a two-factor solution, with six KC items (α = .89) and three KAA items (α = .70), for a total of 9 items (α = .87). Alpha reliabilities show adequate internal consistency for each factor and the overall PIC scale. Communalities ranged from 0.39 to 0.78, implying that items were adequately associated with their corresponding factors and were correlated, r = .61. The KMO (.89) and Barlett’s Test of Sphericity (χ2 (36) = 816.15, p > .001) suggest that the items can be grouped into underlying factors. Based on the Kaiser rule, the factors explain 54.15% of variance in 9 PIC items.

Table 1. Study 1 Exploratory factor analysis pattern matrix

KC = Knowledge Consideration factor; KAA = Knowledge Accommodation/Assimilation factor.

Study 2: Exploratory Factor Analysis. To verify the factor structure of our measure, an EFA was conducted on the 9-item scale resulting from the Study 1 EFA. Undergraduate and graduate students were recruited based on their enrollment in interdisciplinary courses at two private US universities. These courses engaged students in interdisciplinary teamwork by explicitly requiring team composition and outputs to exemplify expertise diversity. Examples of course topics include Environmental Policy Clinic, Crisis Management, e-Learning, and Civic Participation. The nature of the collaborative work assigned to students made them ideal for inclusion in the next step of scale development. The students were participating in interdisciplinary teams at the time that they took the survey, thus providing a closer approximation of actual collaboration than the MTurk sample participants who were asked to recall working in such a team. Student teams were participating in a six-month longitudinal study of interdisciplinary teamwork employing a pretest-posttest design and administration of team training interventions. Data for Study 2 are taken from the pretest stage to prevent any confounds to the structural validity of the measure that might result from differential exposure to the training condition in the study sample. The students were compensated $25.00 per person. The sample included 179 students, 171 of which completed the surveys. Of these respondents, 91 were undergraduate students (63% female, average age 20 years old). Eighty respondents were graduate students (of respondents indicating age and gender, 51% were female, with average age of 33). Additional items included tracking, demographics, and substantive items related to the long-term class project students.

As shown in Table 2, two factors emerged, consistent with Study 1 and verified by parallel analysis in R, explaining 65.30% of the variance in PIC items. Principal axis factoring extraction method was used. The rotation method used was Oblimin with Kaiser Normalization. The rotation converged in four iterations. Data showed high factorability, KMO = 0.91, Barlett’s χ2(36) = 971.67, p > .001. Communalities ranged from 0.53 to 0.77, and the two factors were correlated, r = 0.61, with no meaningful cross-loadings observed between them. Alpha reliabilities show adequate internal consistency (KC α = .92; KAA α = .86; Composite PIC α = .89).

Table 2. Study 2 Exploratory factor analysis pattern matrix

KC = Knowledge Consideration factor; KAA = Knowledge Accommodation/Assimilation factor.

Study 3: Confirmatory Factor Analysis. In a third study, we conducted an analysis to assess the factor structure of the 9-item PIC Index with a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), using a conservative SEM approach. The respondents were 182 research faculty from five universities participating in a six-month investigation of work interdisciplinary teams. As in Study 2, data used for analysis were collected before any team intervention took place to avoid the potentially confounding effects of receiving different types of training. As incentive, participants were given $250.00 per team to be donated to a charity of their choice. Of the 159 respondents who indicated gender, 44% were male.

Given the challenges of administering surveys to this sample and having an adequate response rate, we had some missing data. Simply deleting the cases with missing information would have made using SEM on the sample inappropriate. Multiple imputation [Reference Rubin29] was needed to generate complete responses to satisfy SEM’s high sample size requirements [Reference Wolf, Harrington and Clark30]. Analyses were conducted by pooling the results of the SEM model across ten imputed datasets in Mplus v. 7.3. Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were conducted for the measurement model [Reference Hu, Bentler and Hoyle31,Reference Hu and Bentler32] and for the PIC structural model. Each measurement model tested revealed good fit (Knowledge Consideration: χ 2 = 14.13, χ 2/ /df = 1.57, CFI = 0.98, TLI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.06, βs from 0.78 to .90; Accommodation/Assimilation: χ 2 = 0.45, χ 2/ /df = 0.45, CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00, βs from 0.78 to .86), as did the overall PIC index (χ 2 = 44.41, χ 2/ /df = 1.71, p < 0.05, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.98, RMSEA = .06). The high fit indices observed are not unusual for measurement models close to saturation, and equivalence between χ 2 and χ 2/ /df indicates a single model degree of freedom [Reference Mulaik, James, Van Alstine, Bennett, Lind and Stilwell33]. No post hoc adjustments were made.

Summary of Phase 1. Following recommended procedures for developing valid, reliable, and generalizable survey scales, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were conducted in three different samples and results supported a two-factor model to measure PIC. The scale measuring Knowledge Consideration consists of six items, and the scale measuring Knowledge Assimilation/ Accommodation consists of three items.

Phase 2: the construct validity of perspective integration capability

To establish a nomological network to validate the PIC measure, we use structural equation modeling to assess convergent and discriminant. We collected a new set of data using MTurk. One hundred twenty-seven respondents were chosen who reported having experience on interdisciplinary teams.

Study 4: Convergent and Discriminant Validity. The purpose of Study 4 was to cross-validate the PIC scale with similar constructs in the literature to assess its convergent and discriminant validity. Convergent validity for a focal measure is established when there are significant correlations between the focal measure and measures of similar or dissimilar constructs [Reference Schwab34].

Based on prior literature, we chose four conceptually related measures. Each concept was measured with an established scale, which are available in the Supplement. Alpha reliabilities are provided in parentheses. 1) Bringing Expertise to Bear (α = .88) involves sharing information that is relevant for task completion [Reference Faraj and Sproull35]; 2) Interpersonal Conflict (α = .72) can reduce willingness to consider another person’s point of view [Reference Knight and Smith36]; 3) Knowledge Hiding (α > .70) is the intentional attempt of an individual to withhold knowledge [Reference Connelly, Zweig and Webster37]; and 4) Openness to Experience (α = .72) is the extent to which an individual is imaginative, amenable to new ideas and experiences [Reference Hislop38]. Hypotheses for the theoretical predictions of the relationship between PIC and the four concepts are listed below.

H1: Bringing Expertise to Bear will be positively related to PIC and its components.

H2: Interpersonal Conflict will be negatively related to PIC and its components.

H3: Knowledge Hiding will be negatively related to PIC and its components.

H4: Openness to Experience will be positively related to PIC and its components

Correlational tests of construct validity [Reference Morizot39,Reference Cronbach and Meehl40] revealed the PIC Index to be positively related to Bringing Expertise to Bear and negatively related to Interpersonal Conflict and Knowledge Hiding. For PIC subscales: Knowledge Consideration showed a positive relation with Bring Expertise to Bear and Openness to Experience, and a negative relationship with Knowledge Hiding and Interpersonal Conflict. Knowledge Accommodation/Assimilation was positively related to Bringing Expertise to Bear and negatively related to Interpersonal Conflict and Knowledge Hiding. Table 3 provides descriptive statistics and correlations. To summarize, study 4 demonstrates convergent and discriminant validity of PIC through its significant but distinct relationships to theoretically derived measures of knowledge processing using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM with MPlus).

Table 3. Study 4 Descriptive statistics & correlations of PIC index, PIC factors, & related concepts

Note. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. All measures use team as respondent point of reference.

Phase 3: criterion validity – antecedents and outcomes of PIC

Criterion validity can be assessed by analyzing how a construct behaves in a conceptual model. We tested a model of antecedents and outcomes of PIC using variables proposed in the conceptual model of integrative capacity [Reference Salazar, Lant and Fiore19]. The sample we employed for study 5 was 100 members of 26 interdisciplinary teams of scientists. The scientists in this sample all had doctoral training plus several years of postdoctoral education, which emphasized their deep specialization and heightened the barriers to effective coordination of expertise across their specialized areas. Data were collected at two points in time three months apart. The survey at T1 provides data on the antecedent variables in the model as well as the control variable Interdisciplinary Collaboration Years of Experience. The survey at T2 provides data on PIC and the outcome variables.

Study 5: Structural Equation Modeling. The conceptual model is illustrated in Figure 1. Drawing from prior research and the model of integrative capacity we identify four antecedents of PIC that foster an orientation to consider, accommodate, or assimilate another’s knowledge. Project Vision is a problem conceptualization that characterizes a shared understanding of how unique knowledge of diverse collaborators contributes to the shared task [Reference van Ginkel and van Knippenberg41]. Trust is the trustor’s evaluation of the trustee’s competence, kindness, and honesty, as well as the trustor’s inclination to trust [Reference Mayer, Davis and Schoorman42]. Perspective Seeking refers to actively seeking the perspective of collaborators and indicates a willingness to consider the information received [Reference Davis, Conklin and Smith43,Reference Grant and Berry44]. Creative Self-Efficacy is an individual’s beliefs about their creative potential and that of collaborators [Reference Shin and Zhou45]. It motivates participation and is typically a product of a history of member interactions [Reference Ford46].

Figure 1. Study 5 Structural equation model.

H1. Project vision is positively related to PIC.

H2. Trust in the competence of collaborators is positively related to PIC.

H3. Perspective-seeking is positively related to PIC.

H4. Creative self-efficacy is positively related to PIC.

Consistent with the integrative capacity model [Reference Salazar, Lant and Fiore19], we also examine two important outcomes that PIC will likely enhance. Knowing who knows what (KWKW), a subcomponent of transactive memory, refers to being aware of what your collaborators know [Reference Lewis47]. Higher levels of PIC are expected to enhance the understanding of where expertise is located among collaborators. Transdisciplinary orientation behavior (TDOB) measures a willingness to consider ideas from collaborators outside your own area of expertise by engaging in specific behaviors to learn more about their fields [Reference Misra, Stokols and Cheng21]. As PIC increases, individuals will become more open to the insights, tools, and approaches possessed by their collaborators.

H5: PIC enhances the knowledge of where expertise resides among collaborators (KWKW).

H6. PIC enhances the willingness to draw on knowledge, methods, and approaches from other disciplines (TDOB).

Lastly, we hypothesize that PIC mediates the relationships between the proposed sets of antecedents and outcomes.

H7. PIC mediates the relationship between the theoretically relevant antecedents (vision, trust, perspective seeking, and creative self-efficacy) and outcomes (KWKW and TDOB).

Table 4 provides descriptive statistics and a correlation matrix. To test the relationship between the antecedents, PIC, and outcomes, we used maximum likelihood estimation in MPlus version 8.3 [Reference Muthén and Muthén48] to consider the longitudinal nature of the data. The analysis made use of 2,000 bootstrapped samples of indirect (a*b), direct (path c’), and total effects [Reference Baron and Kenny49]. The model fit the data well (χ 2 /df = 1.82, CFI = 0.91, RMSEA = .09).

Table 4. Study 5. Descriptive statistics & correlations of PIC index with antecedents and outcomes

Note. ** p < .001.

As shown in Figure 1, the control variable years of experience is positively related to PIC. Hypotheses 2 and 3 are supported, as antecedents trust and perspective seeking are positively related to PIC. Hypotheses 5 and 6 are supported, with outcomes KWKW and TDOB being positively associated with PIC. Hypothesis 7 predicts a mediation relationship between antecedents and outcomes through PIC. The strength and direction of the path from the antecedent to the mediator and from the mediator to the outcome are used to assess this indirect effect. For the relationships between the antecedent and outcome variables as mediated by PIC, the indirect effects were significant for perspective-seeking to TDOB (B = 0.14, p < .05; .95% CI [−0.02, .30]), and marginally significant for trust to KWKW (B = 0.18, p < .10; 95% CI [−0.01, .43])

Discussion and conclusion

The set of studies in this manuscript aimed to create a reliable and valid measure of the perspective integration capability of individuals working in expertise diverse collaborations. We conducted a three-phase, five-study investigation to develop and validate a measure of individual perspective integration capability (PIC), which assesses individual ability to integrate knowledge with others during collaborative work. The results demonstrate the scale’s validity, reliability, and generalizability across differing populations and showed that PIC is related to but distinct from other constructs.

In Phase 1, the results of the exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis in three studies using different samples of respondents yielded a nine-item, two-factor measurement scale. These two factors are Knowledge Consideration and Knowledge Accommodation/Assimilation. Phase 2 established convergent and discriminant validity using conceptually related and unrelated constructs. The results of structural equation modeling using data from an independent sample of respondents support the two-factor PIC measure. Our results indicated that PIC was positively related to bringing knowledge to bear, whereas it had a negative association with knowledge hiding and interpersonal conflict. Phase 3 tested criterion-related validity and mediation by examining the multilevel relationships between PIC and theoretically relevant antecedents and outcomes. Using data from a unique sample of research scientists in interdisciplinary medical research teams at two points in time, structural equation modeling established criterion and predictive validity of the scale. This analysis provides further insight into how the perspective integration construct can add to our understanding of individual cognitive capability to utilize and integrate diverse perspectives in collaborative work. Our analysis revealed support for two antecedents, perspective seeking and trust, and two outcomes, namely knowing who knows what and transdisciplinary orientation behaviors.

This manuscript makes several contributions to research and practice concerning diverse collaborations that require knowledge integration. Such collaborations require more than simply sharing expertise; they necessitate deep integration across different knowledge areas. Despite the importance of interprofessional training and practice, collaboration and knowledge integration remain challenging. Collaborating on knowledge-intensive tasks requires the ability to evaluate and utilize knowledge effectively [Reference Harvey13,Reference Gibson16]. The conceptual framework of integrative capacity in diverse collaborations [Reference Salazar, Lant and Fiore19] suggests that perspective integration is essential for navigating the complexities of diverse knowledge collaboration. The initial step in joint knowledge creation involves assessing and sharing each other’s knowledge through knowledge consideration. Cognitive integration entails the incorporation of new knowledge into existing frameworks. Assimilation occurs when individuals encounter information that aligns with their current understanding, whereas accommodation occurs when individuals encounter information that challenges or contradicts their existing beliefs or knowledge. Knowledge integration through assimilation, therefore, consists of the incorporation of novel information into current understanding and accommodation requires adjusting mental schemas to incorporate novel information. This dynamic between assimilation and accommodation is essential to the active and evolving nature of cognition. It highlights how individuals continually shape and refine their mental models in response to the ever-expanding landscape of learning and experience.

We suggest that one barrier to successful interdisciplinary collaboration is the lack of a psychometrically valid and easily administered measurement instrument to provide data to collaborators, team leaders, and research development professionals about a team’s readiness to integrate their diverse expertise. Across the three phases of our study, the results demonstrate support for the PIC instrument’s factor structure, reliability, and validity. We also demonstrate that the construct of PIC has important implications for individuals engaged in interdisciplinary collaboration. Having a conceptually meaningful, valid, reliable, and easily administered survey instrument will facilitate further study of interdisciplinary collaboration and the development and evaluation of integration efforts of teams engaged in convergent and translational initiatives. This set of studies demonstrates that the PIC measurement instrument is a valid, reliable, and conceptually meaningful for studying collaborations.

Finding solutions to important problems like identifying cures for complex disease or enhancing the effectiveness of robotics in manufacturing, requires drawing expertise from disparate fields. In these problem-focused teams, experts, each with their unique and specialized knowledge, are brought together and charged with the difficult task of working together to arrive at a joint solution. This requires the integration of their knowledge and engaging in processes assessed in the PIC scale including knowledge consideration and knowledge assimilation/accommodation. The PIC scale can be leveraged as a diagnostic tool to assess the extent to which knowledge integration is occurring within interdisciplinary and translational teams. Assessing knowledge integration over time has become an important metric to government funding agencies. Having a tool to rapidly and accurately assess team member readiness for cognitive integration can be a competitive advantage for teams seeking federal funding. Moreover, our final study demonstrates how the scale can be used to assess change in knowledge integration capability over time, which can be useful to track team development.

In summary, our aim was to develop and validate a measure of individual-member perspective integration capability, which can assess individuals’ willingness and ability to integrate knowledge with others as part of their work. This scale focuses on evaluating whether an individual is open to the contributions of others and whether they can adopt new ideas or insights they receive. We provide a reliable scale to measure one’s ability to integrate knowledge, which we tested for validity in different contexts and samples. Our results demonstrated the scale’s generalizability across populations and show that PIC is related to but distinct from other constructs. Third, we are the first to empirically show the importance of PIC in collaborative and cross-boundary work environments. PIC can predict individuals’ knowledge of who knows what and their willingness to draw upon and use expertise from other disciplines.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

The strength of this set of studies is the application of well-established standards in creating valid and reliable survey instruments to team science, interdisciplinary and translational research. The scale developed fills a critical need in both research and practice to have a psychometrically established instrument that is easy to use. The limitations of the study are that the aggregate traits of the samples or our choice of outcome variables may have biased the results. For example, all individuals across the five studies shared a characteristic of having interdisciplinary education or work experience. The omission of respondents without this characteristic, while important to sample selection, eliminates the possibility of identifying boundary conditions on the application of the PIC instrument. The PIC scale items are self-reports, which can lead to bias based on the Dunning-Kruger effect [Reference Dunning50] where less skilled individuals tend to overestimate their ability more than highly skilled individuals. Future research is needed to further explore the validity and applicability of the PIC scale. Although it is challenging to study individuals and teams in the field, we encourage more research on interdisciplinary and translational teams in action.

A practical implication of our work is that information gathered using this tool can be used to assess the initial conditions of a collaborative team, to guide professional development activities, and to assess the growth of a team’s integrative capacity over time. Organizations could develop training to increase perspective integration capacity. Efforts to build trust among collaborators and willingness to seek others’ perspectives should increase PIC, which, in turn, could increase the likelihood of knowledge integration through an increase in transdisciplinary orientation behaviors and knowledge of who knows what in a collaboration. Moreover, leadership training programs that develop leaders who promote and reinforce behaviors that foster a perspective integrative capability will be impactful for teams and organizations who want to improve utilization and integration of available knowledge.

Conclusion

Our research on perspective integration offers a solid foundation for building upon prior theoretical and empirical work and creates new avenues for future investigation. Perspective integration is a process that occurs at the individual level and influences how knowledge is shared between individuals and larger groups such as teams or departments, which has excellent potential for future research. We anticipate studies that build on the conceptual framework developed in this paper and empirically validated measurement for perspective integration. We also expect the easily administered questionnaire instrument will be used in practice to enhance interdisciplinary collaboration.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/cts.2025.54.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank study participants for their participation and numerous colleagues for their insights.

Author contributions

Both authors contributed to the conception and design of the work, data collection, and drafting of the manuscript. Dr Salazar also contributed analysis and interpretation of results. Dr Salazar takes responsibility for the manuscript as a whole.

Funding statement

Research reported in this paper was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Science of the National Institutes of Health under the UCLA Clinical and Translational Science Institute grant number UL1TR001881, the UCI Institute for Clinical Translational Science grant number UL1 TR001414, and the National Science Foundation, Grant Number 1262754.

Competing interests

Neither author has competing interests.

Informed consent

All survey respondents completed informed consent forms. The studies were approved by each author’s university IRB.

References

Mistry, S, Kirkman, BL, Moore, O, et al. Too many teams? Examining the impact of multiple team memberships and permanent team identification on employees’ identity strain, cognitive depletion, and turnover. Pers Psychol. 2023;76(3):885912. doi: 10.1111/peps.12515.Google Scholar
O’Leary, MB, Mortensen, M, Wooly, AW. Multiple team membership: a theoretical model of its effects on productivity and learning for individuals and teams. Acad Manage Rev. 2011;36(3):461478. doi: 10.5465/amr.2009.0275.Google Scholar
Sanders, S. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Measuring convergence in science and engineering: Proceedings of a workshop. The National Academies Press, 2021. doi: 10.17226/26040.Google Scholar
The National Academies Keck Futures Initiative. National Research Council. Collaborations of consequence: NAKFI’s 15 years igniting innovation at the intersections of disciplines. The National Academies Press, 2018. doi: 10.17226/25239.Google Scholar
Petersen, AM, Ahmed, ME, Pavlidis, I. Grand challenges and emergent modes of convergence science. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. 2021;8(194):115. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00869-9.Google Scholar
Kumarasamy, MA, Sanfillippo, FP. Breaking down silos: engaging students to help fix the US healthcare system. J Multi Health. 2015;8:101108. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S79384.Google Scholar
Rider, EA, Chou, C, Abraham, C, et al. Longitudinal faculty development to improve interprofessional collaboration and practice: a multisite qualitative study at five US academic health centers. BMJ Open. 2023;13(4):e069466. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/37076167/.Google Scholar
Snell, R, Fyfe, SF, et al. Development of professional identity and professional socialization in allied health students: a scoping review. Focus Health Prof Edu. 2020;21(1):2956.Google Scholar
Hall, K, Vogel, A, Stipelman, B, et al. A four-phase model of transdisciplinary team-based research: goals, team processes and strategies. Trans Behav Med. 2012;2(4):415430.Google Scholar
Lombardo, L, Ehlers, J, Lutz, G. Mindset and reflection - how to sustainably improve intra-and interpersonal competencies in medical education. Healthcare. 2023;11(6):859. doi: 10.3390/healthcare11060859.Google Scholar
Levinthal, DA, March, JG. The myopia of learning. Strategic Manage J. 1993;14(52):95112. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250141009.Google Scholar
Clark, KM. Interprofessional education: making our way out of the silos. Resp Care. 2018;63(5):637639. doi: 10.4187/respcare.06234.Google Scholar
Harvey, S. A different perspective: the multiple effects of deep level diversity on group creativity. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2013;49(5):822832. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.004.Google Scholar
Hoever, IJ, van Knippenberg, D, van Ginkel, WP, et al. Fostering team creativity: perspective taking as key to unlocking diversity’s potential. J Appl Psychol. 2012;97(5):982996. doi: 10.1037/a0029159.Google Scholar
Parker, SK, Axtell, CM. Seeing another viewpoint: antecedents and outcomes of employee perspective taking. Acad Manag J. 2001;44(6):10851100. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3069390.Google Scholar
Gibson, CB. From knowledge accumulation to accommodation: cycles of collective cognition in work groups. J Organ Behav. 2001;22(2):121134. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3649586.Google Scholar
Homan, AC, van Knippenberg, D, Van Kleef, GA, et al. Interacting dimensions of diversity: cross-categorization and the functioning of diverse work groups. Group Dyn - Theor Res. 2007;11(2):7994. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.11.2.79.Google Scholar
Falk-Krzesinski, H, Borner, K, Contractor, N, et al. Advancing the science of team sciences. Clin Transl Sci. 2010;3(5):263266.Google Scholar
Salazar, MR, Lant, TK, Fiore, SM, et al. Facilitating innovation in diverse science teams through integrative capacity. Small Gr Res. 2012;43(5):527558. doi: 10.1177/1046496412453622.Google Scholar
Lotrecchiano, GR, Schwartz, L, Falk-Krzesinski, HJ. Measuring motivation for team science collaboration in health teams. Clin Transl Sci. 2020;5(e84):16. doi: 10.1017/cts.2020.567.Google Scholar
Misra, S, Stokols, D, Cheng, L. The transdisciplinary orientation scale: factor structure and relation to the integrative quality and scope of scientific publications. J Trans Med Epid. 2015;3(2):1042.Google Scholar
Floyd, FJ, Widaman, KF. Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psych Assess. 1995;7(3):286299. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.286.Google Scholar
Loevinger, J. The attenuation paradox in test theory. Psych Bul. 1954;51(5):493504. doi: 10.1037/h0058543.Google Scholar
Hinkin, TR. A brief tutorial on the development of measures for use in survey questionnaires. Organ Res Meth. 1998;1(1):104121. doi: 10.1177/109442819800100106.Google Scholar
Schulze, T, Krug, S, Schader, M. Workers’ task choice in crowdsourcing and human computation markets. In: ICIS. 2012. Proceedings: 33rd International Conference on Information Systems Google Scholar
Buhrmester, M, Kwang, T, Gosling, SD. Amazon’s mechanical turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psych Sci. 2011;6(1):35. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980.Google Scholar
Clark, LA, Watson, D. Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psych Assess. 1995;7(3):309319. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309.Google Scholar
Comrey, AL. Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and clinical psychology. J Consult Clin Psych. 1988;56(5):754761. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.5.754.Google Scholar
Rubin, DB. Multiple imputation after 18+ years. J Am Stat Assoc. 1996;91(434):473489. https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/2291635.pdf.Google Scholar
Wolf, EJ, Harrington, KM, Clark, SL, et al. Sample size requirements for structural equation models: an evaluation of power, bias, and solution propriety. Educ Psych Measur. 2013;73(6):913934. doi: 10.1177/0013164413495237.Google Scholar
Hu, L, Bentler, PM. Evaluating model fit. In: Hoyle, RH, ed. Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Sage Publications, Inc, 1995: 7699.Google Scholar
Hu, L, Bentler, PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equat Model. 1999;6(1):155. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118.Google Scholar
Mulaik, SA, James, LR, Van Alstine, J, Bennett, N, Lind, S, Stilwell, CD. Evaluation of goodness-of-fit indices for structural equation models. Psych Bul. 1989;105(3):430445. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.430.Google Scholar
Schwab, DP. Research methods for organizational studies. Psychology Press, 2013.Google Scholar
Faraj, S, Sproull, L. Coordinating expertise in software development teams. Manage Sci. 2000;46(12):15541568. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2661533.Google Scholar
Knight, DP, Smith, CL, et al. Top management team diversity, group process, and strategic consensus. Strat Manag J. 1999;20:445465.Google Scholar
Connelly, CE, Zweig, D, Webster, J, et al. Knowledge hiding in organizations. J Organ Beh. 2012;33(1):6488. https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/41415737.pdf.Google Scholar
Hislop, D. Knowledge integration processes and the appropriation of innovations. Europ J Innov Manag. 2003;6(3):159172. doi: 10.1108/14601060310486235.Google Scholar
Morizot, J. Construct validity of adolescents’ self-reported big five personality traits: importance of conceptual breadth and initial validation of a short measure. Assess. 2014;21(5):580606. doi: 10.1177/1073191114524015.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L, Meehl, PE. Construct validity in psychological tests. Psych Bul. 1955;52(4):281302. doi: 10.1037/h0040957.Google Scholar
van Ginkel, WP, van Knippenberg, D. Group leadership and shared task representations in decision making groups. Leader Quar. 2012;23(1):94106. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.11.008.Google Scholar
Mayer, RC, Davis, JH, Schoorman, FD. An integrative model of organizational trust. Acad Manag Rev. 1995;20(3):709734. doi: 10.2307/258792.Google Scholar
Davis, MH, Conklin, L, Smith, A, et al. Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: a merging of self and other. J Pers Soc Psych. 1996;70(4):713726. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.713.Google Scholar
Grant, AM, Berry, J. The necessity of others is the mother of invention: intrinsic and prosocial motivations, perspective taking, and creativity. Acad Manag J. 2003;54(1):7396. doi: 10.5465/AMJ.2011.59215085.Google Scholar
Shin, SJ, Zhou, J. When is educational specialization heterogeneity related to creativity in research and development teams? Transformational leadership as a moderator. J Appl Psych. 2007;92(6):17091721. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.6.1709.Google Scholar
Ford, CM. A theory of individual creative action in multiple social domains. Acad Manag Rev. 1996;21:11121142. doi: 10.2307/259166.Google Scholar
Lewis, K. Measuring transactive memory systems in the field: scale development and validation. J Appl Psych. 2003;88(4):587604.Google Scholar
Muthén, LK, Muthén, BO. Mplus User’s Guide, 8th Edition ed. 1998-2017. https://www.statmodel.com/download/usersguide/MplusUserGuideVer_8.pdf.Google Scholar
Baron, RM, Kenny, DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psych. 1986;51(6):11731182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173.Google Scholar
Dunning, D. The dunning-kruger effect: on being ignorant of one’s own ignorance. In: eds. Advances in experimental social psychology. Academic Press, 2011;44:247296.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Study 1 Exploratory factor analysis pattern matrix

Figure 1

Table 2. Study 2 Exploratory factor analysis pattern matrix

Figure 2

Table 3. Study 4 Descriptive statistics & correlations of PIC index, PIC factors, & related concepts

Figure 3

Figure 1. Study 5 Structural equation model.

Figure 4

Table 4. Study 5. Descriptive statistics & correlations of PIC index with antecedents and outcomes

Supplementary material: File

Salazar Campo and Lant supplementary material

Salazar Campo and Lant supplementary material
Download Salazar Campo and Lant supplementary material(File)
File 35.2 KB