Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-9knnw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-31T18:20:11.915Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-cultural variation in understanding of animal welfare principles and animal management practices among veterinary and animal welfare professionals in the UK and Japan

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 August 2025

Yuki Otani*
Affiliation:
Jeanne Marchig International Centre for Animal Welfare Education, Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK International Affairs Office, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, https://ror.org/02e16g702 Hokkaido University , Sapporo, Japan Social Cooperation Unit, One Health Research Centre, https://ror.org/02e16g702 Hokkaido University , Sapporo, Japan
Mariko Kanamori
Affiliation:
Institute for the Future of Human Society, https://ror.org/02kpeqv85 Kyoto University , Kyoto, Japan Department of Public Health Sciences, https://ror.org/05f0yaq80 Stockholm University , Stockholm, Sweden
Hiromi Kato
Affiliation:
Animal Welfare and Wildlife Damage Management Group, National Agriculture and Food Research Organisation, Tsukuba, Japan
Cathy M. Dwyer
Affiliation:
Jeanne Marchig International Centre for Animal Welfare Education, Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK Animal Behaviour and Welfare, Department of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, Central Faculty, Scotland’s Rural College (SRUC), Edinburgh, UK
*
Corresponding author: Yuki Otani; Email: yukiotani35@vetmed.hokudai.ac.jp
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The World Organisation for Animal Health describes animal welfare as a “complex and multi-faceted subject with scientific, ethical, economic, cultural, social, religious and political dimensions.” In this study, an online survey in English and Japanese was developed based on the Five Freedoms, with the aim of investigating attitudes of veterinarians and behaviour/welfare scientists in the United Kingdom (UK) and Japan toward management of companion, farmed, experimental, zoo and wildlife animals. Respondents from the UK (n = 212) were more familiar with the Five Freedoms than those from Japan (n = 321) but both countries tended to prioritise ‘survival-related’ attributes (health and nutrition) over ‘situation-related’ attributes (behaviour) and the environmental impacts (discomfort). In Japan, however, fewer respondents recognised the ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ as important for domesticated animals compared to UK respondents. When considering vignettes with practical situations of cat management and dog euthanasia, UK respondents considered the provision of outdoor access to represent better management for cat welfare while most Japanese respondents thought cats should be managed entirely indoors, although the benefits and risks of going outdoors were similarly recognised in both countries. For the vignette of dog pain relating to an incurable tumour, severe pain and the dog’s mental stress motivated respondents from both countries to consider euthanasia. However, for Japanese respondents, the data suggested a perception that mental stress did not have an association with the dog’s inabilities to express normal behaviour. These data highlighted the importance of understanding the manner in which people perceive animals in different contexts and the value of considering different cultural approaches.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

The concept of animal welfare is increasingly recognised as being of global importance in the context of managing animals. The World Organisation for Animal Health (WOAH) defines animal welfare as “the physical and mental state of an animal in relation to the conditions in which it lives and dies.” Animal welfare science has been applied to attempt to quantify the physical and mental state of animals, focusing on physical, physiological, behavioural and neurological evidence. In the European Union, evidence-based legislation has been developed considering the physical state of the animal, and ethological, physiological and psychological evidence concerning the animal’s behavioural motivations. WOAH also describes animal welfare as “a complex and multi-faceted subject with scientific, ethical, economic, cultural, social, religious and political dimensions.” This emphasises that there can be differing views as to what constitutes good animal welfare. Fraser et al. (Reference Fraser, Weary, Pajor and Milligan1997) argues that there are at least three overlapping ethical concerns when considering the quality of animals’ lives: natural lives (which can include behaviour in the context of natural adaptations to environment); biological functioning (such as biological responses and health) and feelings (which may also include behaviours in the context of understanding emotional states); and the application or weight given to these different ethical concerns may vary across cultures and between individuals. Fraser (Reference Fraser2008) further suggests that what people consider more or less important for animals to have good lives would be influenced by the different worldviews they have, which sometimes could lead to opposing conclusions relating to welfare issues (Fraser Reference Fraser2008). In this study, we explore these ideas by comparing the views of veterinarians and behaviour/welfare scientists living in two culturally distinct regions: the United Kingdom (UK) and Japan.

In the UK, animal welfare is a matter of great social concern and legislation relevant to animal protection and animal welfare has been developed. The first legislation for protecting animals in the UK was established in 1822 (Cruel Treatment of Cattle Act), which aimed to prevent cruelty towards farm animals. The impetus for the introduction of the Act was, however, more than sentimentality surrounding animal cruelty and the aim was related to improving the morals of the lower social orders rather than simply protecting animals (Radford Reference Radford2001). Towards the middle of the 19th Century, legislation was extended to protecting other species from suffering, with the rationale moving more towards protection from the point of view of the animals (Radford Reference Radford2001). With the intensification of farming practices in the 1960s, public concern for the quality of life of animals increased and the concept of animal welfare as the Five Freedoms (FF) for animals was developed (Farm Animal Welfare Council 1993). The FF have since become the most widely referenced framework for animal welfare, conceiving of animal welfare as ‘Freedom from hunger, malnutrition and thirst’, ‘Freedom from fear and distress’, ‘Freedom from discomfort’, ‘Freedom from pain, injury and disease’ and ‘Freedom to express normal patterns of behaviour’. The FF and the Brambell Report (Reference Report1965) which led to their development, specifically consider animal welfare to be related to animal feelings (hunger, thirst, pain, fear etc) and constituted a shift away from focusing on preventing animal cruelty to reflect the public desire for animals to be provided with good welfare (Broom Reference Broom2011). The Animal Welfare Act (2006) expanded the species covered to include all vertebrates under human care and required owners to provide for an animal’s welfare needs and relevant Codes and Regulations offer practical standards for each species of animal. Subsequently, the Animal Welfare (Sentience) Act (2022) focused on recognising the capacity of animals to have positive and negative experiences and recognised that these capacities may also be present in certain invertebrates.

Japan is an island country in East Asia with a similarly developed economy to the UK. The first animal protection law (Act on Protection and Management of Animals [Doubutsu No Hogo Oyobi Kanri Ni Kansuru Houritsu]) was established in 1973 in response to external pressure from Western countries (Nakanishi Reference Nakanishi and Nakanishi2016). This law has been amended every few years and was renamed the Act on Aigo and Management of animals (Doubutsu No Aigo Oyobi Kanri Ni Kansuru Houritsu; Aigo law) in 1999. The term ‘Aigo’ is composed of the characters for ‘love (愛)’ and ‘protection (護)’and its development is considered to originate from a combination of the ethics of Shinto religion, Buddhism, and Confucianism (Sato Reference Sato, Brown, Seddon and Appleby2016). Although the definition has not reached a consensus in Japan, Aigo is generally perceived to be a subjective and emotional view held by humans wanting to protect animals in a paternalistic way (Uchikoshi Reference Uchikoshi2016). One of the largest surveys concerning pet management was carried out in 2020 (Japan Pet Food Association 2020) and showed around 80% of pet owners are aware of the Aigo law, while a previous survey (taken in 2016) examining public awareness of animal welfare revealed that nearly 90% of the public had never heard of the term ‘animal welfare’ (Tokyo City University 2017). This suggests that Aigo is a predominant ethical view among Japanese when managing animals (Honjo Reference Honjo, Peters, Stilt and Stucki2022). The Aigo Law is the only legislation responsible for welfare of animals in Japan, and it applies to cattle, horses, pigs, sheep, goats, dogs, cats, domestic rabbits, chickens, domestic pigeons, domestic ducks and all other animals that are either owned or managed by humans. Although the Aigo law does not provide species-specific, evidence-based standards with legal penalties, except for dogs and cats managed for businesses such as breeding, exhibition, and training, the essence of the FF, such as ensuring an environment provides water, food, and healthcare with a consideration made for the animals’ habitat, has been included via an update in 2012 (Article 2 Fundamental Principal, Article 7 Responsibilities, etc of Owners and Possessors of Animals). The Ministry of Agriculture, Forestry and Fisheries (MAFF) has a set of newly formulated guidelines for animal management that were produced in 2023 and are based on the WOAH Terrestrial Animal Health Code for farm animals. However, neither of these guidelines are legally enforceable (MAFF 2023a).

In both the UK and Japan, most animals are similarly managed for a variety of purposes: farm, companion, experimental, zoo and free-living wildlife. Veterinarians and animal behaviour/welfare scientists play a central role in upholding and improving the welfare of those managed animals in not only practical but legal ways (Doyle et al. Reference Doyle, Wieland, Saville, Grace and Campbell2021). Recently, a number of veterinary schools in Japan (as well as those in the UK) have become accredited for their education by the European Association of Establishments for Veterinary Education (EAEVE), suggesting similarities in veterinary standards and training between both countries (EAEVE 2024). However, clear cultural differences in veterinary practice exist between the UK and Japan – an example of which being the attitude to euthanasia in companion animals. While in the UK an average case number for euthanasia per clinic would be 5.80 per month (Dickinson et al. Reference Dickinson, Roof, Roof and Paul2014; Pegram et al. Reference Pegram, Gray, Packer, Richards, Church, Brodbelt and O’Neill2021), the average for Japan was 2.48 occasions per clinic per year (Sugita & Irimajiri Reference Sugita and Irimajiri2016). Taking a broader perspective, the actual definition of animals differs between both countries. In EU and UK legislation, animals are officially defined as ‘sentient beings’ based on scientific evidence indicating that some animals have a capacity to feel and engage with the environment (Birch et al. Reference Birch, Burn, Schnell, Browning and Crump2021). Therefore, the welfare frameworks developed in those countries have been formulated to cater for animals’ needs in light of this, with a focus on what the animal feels or experiences. On the other hand, in the Aigo law of Japan, animals are defined as ‘living beings’ and proper treatment to maintain an animal’s health and safety throughout their lives is emphasised by showing respect for an animal being alive.

In this study, we hypothesised that those cultural perceptions could influence the value veterinary professionals place on good lives for animals. A questionnaire in English and Japanese was developed to investigate attitudes towards the FF as a common, global framework when managing animals. We developed two companion animal vignettes, that presented different welfare scenarios, to understand their views on approaches to managing ‘real-world’ situations. Understanding the similarities and differences regarding veterinary professionals’ perceptions of animals in different countries is key in helping develop strategies to implement good animal welfare in these diverse cultures.

Materials and methods

Ethical approval

The study was approved by Human Ethical Review Committee (HERC) of The Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, The University of Edinburgh, UK, approval number HERC_776_21. The survey was entirely voluntary and anonymous, and the data collected did not contain any personal and identifiable information. At the beginning of the survey, the purpose of the study was explained and the uses to which the data would be put outlined. Respondents gave informed consent to such use of these data and were made aware they could withdraw consent at any time prior to submission.

Survey development

The survey was designed to explore participants’ understanding of the different components of animal welfare as exemplified in the FF, and to explore how important they thought these were for different groups of animals. Briefly, the initial draft of the questionnaire was created in English originally prior to being translated into Japanese by a native speaker who was also fluent in English and familiar with veterinary science and animal welfare. Questions were entirely novel and revised through several iterations among the researchers. The preliminary version of the questionnaire was piloted with a total of five volunteer veterinarians and researchers from the UK and Japan. No major changes were made from the piloted survey, however a number of minor changes were implemented in terms of numbers of a point scale (6 to 5), styles of answer (Yes/No or a point scale) and specific wordings to help ensure questions made sense in both English and Japanese.

In the first part, questions were asked relating to respondents’ demographic variables, focusing on variables known to influence animal welfare knowledge and attitudes in other studies (Serpell Reference Serpell2004; Hazel et al. Reference Hazel, Signal and Taylor2011; Carnovale et al. Reference Carnovale, Xiao, Shi, Arney and Phillips2024), including age, gender, profession, and previous education and training in animal welfare.

In the next part, to investigate understanding of the concept of animal welfare, respondents were asked how many of the FFs they would be able to explain to other people. Prior to proceeding to these questions, respondents were directed to look through the explanation of the FF as given by the webpage of Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (RSPCA) for the UK and the Ministry of Environment (MOE) for Japan to ensure participants had equal knowledge of what the FF constitute. To investigate their perception of animal welfare, respondents were asked to select which of the FF they considered most important for companion, farmed, experimental, zoo or wildlife animals, respectively. Respondents were allowed to select multiple responses if they believed that several or all freedoms were equally important.

In the final section, respondents were shown two original fictional scenarios as ‘vignettes’: one focusing on cat husbandry practices and the other on situations in which euthanasia of a dog with incurable disease might be advised. The vignettes were carefully developed to ensure that the situation was realistic in both the UK and Japan. Questions here focused on the animal’s needs developed from the FF and relevant potential issues which might influence the animal’s welfare, including the owner’s situation with reference to relevant legislation and information from governmental bodies, veterinary associations, and animal charities. Since no metrics for measuring Japanese Aigo were included in the survey, questions relating to the individual’s love for the animal (Ai) were incorporated into the vignettes. Respondents were asked how they thought a cat owned by a man who lives in an urban area should be managed (six options, ranging from completely indoors to completely outdoors) and what they recognise as the benefits and risks to the cat when it is provided or not provided access to the outdoors. The second vignette related to a dog with an incurable femoral tumour with respondents asked whether they thought the dog should be euthanased if the situation worsened in different ways. The vignettes and related questions are described in Table 1. In this part, all questions, apart from a question regarding management preferences for the cat, were answered using a five-point Likert scale, ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree.

Table 1. Detailed descriptions of two fictional scenarios (vignettes) which formed part of a survey to understand attitudes to animal welfare in Japanese (n = 321) and UK (n = 212) veterinary professionals. These vignettes formed the baseline scenario from which respondents were asked their views on how the animals should be managed

FPV: Feline Panleukopenia Virus, FHV: Feline Herpesvirus, FCV: Feline Calicivirus, FeLV: Feline Leukemia Virus.

Google Form was used as a platform for the survey to make it accessible for respondents in both countries and the survey can be seen in Supplementary material_1.

Respondents

The study target sample population consisted of veterinarians, and animal behaviour and welfare scientists who were working in either the UK or Japan. Participants were recruited via a variety of means, including emails sent to national and international associations related to veterinary medicine, farm animals, zoo, racehorses, animal behaviour and welfare. Further, recruitment was advertised on the webpage, and social media (Facebook and X [formerly Twitter]) of the Jeanne Marchig International Centre for Animal Welfare Education at the Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies in Edinburgh, UK, and we also called for participants at a Royal Canin Veterinary Symposium in Japan and in The Veterinary Record of the British Veterinary Association. The survey was open from 11th November 2021 to 31st January 2022 with respondents divided into either the UK or Japan depending upon where they were currently working. In total, 215 and 363 respondents from, respectively, the UK and Japan were recruited.

Statistical analysis

Responses from respondents who were not in the target population (including veterinary nurses and students), and those for whom eligibility was unclear, were excluded from the analysis. The final analysis, therefore, included data from 212 and 321 respondents from UK and Japan, respectively. To facilitate analysis, age categories were regrouped into four categories (< 35, 35–44, 45–54, > 54 years) from 6 (< 25, 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, 55–64, > 65 years old) to deal with low numbers in the extreme age classes. Similarly, categories of profession were regrouped to provide three classes for analysis (veterinarian in practice, veterinary researcher, animal behaviour/welfare researcher) from seven original options (veterinarian in practice, veterinary researcher, veterinarian in public health, veterinarian in another role, researcher of animal behaviour, researcher of animal welfare, other) in order to make them sufficiently balanced. The data were entered into Microsoft Excel® (Microsoft 365) and all analyses performed using the statistical software STATA 16.1 (StataCorp, TX, USA). The answers obtained via Likert scales were converted to a number (strongly disagree: 1 to strongly agree: 5) and then used for statistical analysis. In the section considering cat management, respondents were asked to evaluate if the cat in the vignette would have access to factors relating to the FF (food, water, proper environment, interaction, treatment etc) if management included access to the outdoors or not (on a five-point Likert scale). The view of the respondents regarding the relative benefits or risks of being outdoors were then calculated as the difference in responses given in relation to the cat being completely indoors. Then, the answers were converted into numbers, with the values of answers when completely indoors subtracted from values for when the cat has access to outdoors, in order to evaluate the relative benefit or risks recognised by respondents. Chi-squared or Mann-Whitney U tests were used to compare responses across countries. Knowledge and attitude towards the relative importance of the FF within each country were investigated using Binary Logistic regression fitting age, profession and education as fixed effects. Binary Logistic Regression for familiarity with the FF was conducted by dividing responses into two groups (those who felt they could explain 0–3 = 0, those who were more confident in being able to explain 4–5 = 1) to investigate predictors of this familiarity. Factor Analysis with promax rotation was performed on the data investigating respondents’ attitudes to euthanasia of the dog in response to different scenarios (Table 1) to investigate relationships of variables relevant to an animal and owner when discussing potential euthanasia of the dog.

Results

Respondent demographics

The distribution of respondents between different categories is shown in Table 2. In both countries, respondents were predominantly practising veterinarians (approximately two-thirds of respondents in both countries) and around 90% of all respondents were veterinarians including researchers or in another role. The gender distribution was, however, significantly different with more than 85% of respondents from the UK being women whereas 57% of respondents from Japan were men (χ2[2] = 110.1; P < 0.001). The UK participants were skewed towards the younger demographic with more than half of respondents younger than 35 years old and less than 20% of respondents older than 45 years old. For the Japanese respondents, ages were more evenly distributed across age categories (χ2[3] = 41.9; P < 0.001).

Table 2. Demographic information of 533 veterinary professional respondents who took part in a survey of attitudes to animal welfare from UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321). The number of respondents in each category are presented with the proportion of the total shown as a percentage in parenthesis

In the comparison of educational background, 94% of UK respondents answered they had received a formal education in animal welfare compared to 56% of Japan respondents (χ2[1] = 87.6; P < 0.001). The effects of age and profession were assessed using Binary Logistical Regression (Table S1; Supplementary material_2) to investigate predictors of education in animal welfare. Despite there being no significant effects of age or profession on the likelihood of receiving formal education in animal welfare in the UK, in Japan respondents under the age of 35 years were significantly more likely to have received formal education in animal welfare compared to older respondents (P < 0.001). For self-education in animal welfare, equivalent numbers of UK and Japanese respondents (67.9 and 66.7%, respectively) had sought to find animal welfare education themselves as well as or instead of receiving it as part of their formal education (χ2[1] = 0.0916; P = 0.762). In both countries this was more prevalent in animal welfare researchers than in veterinarians, whether they worked in practice or in research (Table S1; Supplementary material_2).

Attitude towards the Five Freedoms

To investigate how familiar respondents were with the FF, they were asked how many they could explain to others. Most (83.5%) respondents from the UK considered themselves able to explain all five (Figure 1). In Japan, however, 51.7% of respondents were of the opinion they could explain four or five but nearly 20% responded that they could explain none or one only, resulting in a significant difference between the two countries (χ2[5] = 108.8; P < 0.001). For both countries, seeking self-education in animal welfare was associated with a greater likelihood of the belief of being able to explain four or five freedoms (Table 3). Further, in Japan, younger respondents were more confident they could explain four or five freedoms than older respondents. Similarly, animal behaviour/welfare scientists considered themselves more able to explain four or five freedoms than did the veterinarians.

Figure 1. The percentage of UK (n = 212) and Japanese (n = 321) respondents in a survey of attitudes to animal welfare who considered themselves able to explain 0, 1, 2, 3, 4 or 5 of the Five Freedoms to others.

Table 3. Result of binary logistic regression using demographic data to investigate predictors of the number of the Five Freedoms respondents can explain to others in the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321) (Original responses are scaled as 0-3=0, 4-5=1). In both countries, respondents who undertook self-education were able to explain more Freedoms whereas age and profession were also associated for Japanese respondents

OR: Odds Ratio; CI: Confidence Interval.

We next investigated which freedoms respondents considered the most important for animals in each category (Figure 2). In the UK, respondents selected ‘Freedom from hunger and thirst’ and ‘Freedom from injury, pain and disease’ most frequently for all categories of animals, although at a lower rate for wildlife (companion 89.6, 87.7%; farm 89.2, 90.1%; experimental 83.4, 86.3%; zoo 84.4, 89.5%; wild 73.1, 72.6%, respectively). ‘Freedom from fear and distress’ was selected at a lower rate but still considered important by most respondents in the UK (companion 80.2%; farm 76.9%; experimental 80.2%; zoo 76.0%; wild 66.0%). UK respondents consistently rated ‘Freedom from discomfort’ as the least important freedom, in all categories (companion 58.5%; farm 59.4%; experimental 62.7%; zoo 59.4%; wildlife 48.1%). ‘Freedom to express normal behaviours’ was also considered to be less important than other freedoms for companion, farm and experimental animals, but to be comparable with ‘Freedom from hunger and thirst’, and ‘Freedom from injury, pain and disease’ for zoo and wildlife welfare (companion 68.9%; farm 68.4%; experimental 64.2%; zoo 81.1%; wildlife 75.0%).

Figure 2. Histogram showing (a) UK (n = 212) and (b) Japanese (n = 321) respondents’ responses when asked to choose which of the Five Freedoms was most important for companion, farm, experimental, zoo or wildlife animals. Each bar shows the percentage of respondents who selected the freedom in question as most important. Respondents were able to select 1 to 5 freedoms for each category of animals. Key: Hunger/thirst: Freedom from hunger, malnutrition and thirst; Fear/distress: Freedom from fear and distress; Discomfort: Freedom from discomfort; Injury/disease: Freedom from pain, injury and disease; Behaviour: Freedom to express normal patterns of behaviour.

For Japanese respondents, ‘Freedom from hunger and thirst’ was also consistently selected as the most important except for wildlife (companion 84.1%; farm 79.8%; experimental 65.1%; zoo 75.1%; wildlife 33.0%). The next most commonly prioritised freedoms were ‘Freedom from fear and distress’ and ‘Freedom from injury, pain and disease’ except for wildlife (companion 74.5, 70.1%; farm 66.0, 67.3%, experimental 54.2, 66.4%; zoo 65.1, 64.5%, wildlife 28.7, 24.0%, respectively). Similarly to the UK, ‘Freedom from discomfort’ was selected less frequently in Japan (companion 61.1%; farm 59.5%; experimental 54.2%; zoo 62.0%; wildlife 21.2%). However, ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ was less likely to be selected as important by Japanese respondents for companion, farm and experimental animals, although it was ranked equivalent to other freedoms for zoo animals and was the highest ranked freedom for wildlife (companion 42.4%; farm 35.2%; experimental 28.7%; zoo 66.7%; wildlife 81.6%). These attributes resulted in a variety of the numbers of freedoms that respondents selected among animal category within country and also between countries (Figure S1; Supplementary material_2; UK vs Japan: companion 3.85 vs 3.32; farm 3.84 vs 3.08; experimental 3.77 vs 2.90; zoo 3.84 vs 3.33; wildlife 3.35 vs 1.88; P < 0.001, Mann-Whitney U test, n = 212 [UK]; 321 [Japan]). In particular, Chi-squared tests confirmed that ‘Freedom from injury and disease’ and ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ were less frequently selected in Japan compared to the UK, regardless of categories apart from wildlife (Table S2; Supplementary material_2).

To further investigate factors influencing differences between countries, we focused on the data for companion animals and Binary Logistic Analysis was performed on the results of ‘Freedom from injury and disease’ and ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ using demographic data (Table 4). There was no effect of age, profession or education in animal welfare on the probability of selecting ‘Freedom from injury and disease’ as important in either the UK or Japan. This was also true for ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ for Japanese respondents. However, in the UK, those respondents who had invested in self-education in animal welfare were more likely to consider ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ as important (Odds Ratio: 3.00; [95% Confidence Interval: 1.55, 5.82]; P = 0.001).

Table 4. Result of binary logistic regression using demographic data to investigate predictors for selecting ‘Freedom from injury and disease’ and ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ as the most important (or equally most important to others) of the Five Freedoms from respondents in the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321)

OR: Odds Ratio; CI: Confidence Interval

Vignette 1: Opinions on cat keeping

When asked how the cat should be managed, although both groups of respondents considered that cats should spend most of their time indoors (more than 95% of responses; Figure 3), respondents from the UK were much less likely to consider the cat should be kept completely indoors compared to those from Japan (UK 33.0%; Japan 77.9%; χ2[5] = 116.12; P < 0.001; Figure 4). UK respondents were most likely to choose: ‘mostly indoors, but the cat can go outdoors when she wants’ (42.5%) and ‘mostly indoors, but the cat can go outdoors when the owner wants’ (22.2%) rather than keeping the cat only indoors.

Figure 3. Bar chart showing the percentage of UK (n = 212) and Japanese (n = 321) respondents’ answers when presented with a series of management options relating to the scenario of a city-dwelling owner with a single cat. Respondents were allowed to select 1 of 6 management categories that differed in terms of whether the cat was indoor, outdoor, or both; and whether the decision was made by the cat or the owner.

Figure 4. Box and whisker plot of the relative value of a cat going outdoors in relation to relevant factors as selected by respondents as the benefit or risk factors for cat management, in comparison to staying completely indoors. The relative value was calculated by subtracting the answers when the cat was managed completely indoors from the answers when access to outdoors was provided. The box and whisker plot represents the minimum, first quartile, median, third quartile, and maximum. Cross marks represent an average, and asterisks show significant differences between the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321). *P < 0.05, Mann-Whitney U test.

We next investigate perceptions of respondents toward going outdoors and staying indoors in order to understand what were deemed to be the benefits and risks for the cat. Figure 4 shows the relative value of the cat being allowed access to outdoors compared to being kept entirely indoors for different attributes based on the FF and Aigo concepts. Overall, respondents from both countries were in broad agreement regarding their assessment of the risks and benefits of allowing the cat access to outdoors access, i.e. that such access could provide positive experiences to help express normal behaviour while risks relating to injury, becoming lost, and having conflicts with humans became elevated (Figure 4).

However, Japanese respondents were more likely than their UK counterparts to view those risks more negatively for the cat (P < 0.01, Mann-Whitney U test). In addition, Japanese respondents considered outdoor access to negatively impact access to food, water and a comfortable environment, whereas UK respondents perceived it to be neutral for food and water access and relatively positive for the environment. Further, Japanese respondents also considered that allowing a cat access to outdoors reduced the love or affection the owner had for the cat while UK respondents considered this to be neutral.

Vignette 2: Opinions on dog euthanasia

When considering the second vignette of a dog euthanasia case, 5.0% of respondents from Japan supported euthanasia for the dog as presented in the base scenario, whereas 40.6% of UK respondents agreed that euthanasia at that point should be performed (Figure 5). Furthermore, 79.4% of Japanese respondents either disagreed or strongly disagreed with euthanasia for the dog at this point while only 18.4% of UK respondents were opposed (χ2[2] = 104.5; P < 0.001). These opinions were further investigated in light of alterations to the status of the dog and the owner. For both the UK and Japan, all changes (with the exception of the owner’s love) increased the percentages of agreement (strongly agree and agree) and UK respondents were consistently more likely than the Japanese to support euthanasia in all the conditions (P < 0.05, Mann-Whitney U test). All the respondents from UK (100%) and virtually all from Japan (92.5%) expressed agreement for euthanasia when severe pain was no longer able be managed by any medication. Both countries also showed another large increase in the scenario whereby the dog’s mental stress became severe, although for Japanese respondents the increase was not as large as seen for the dog’s uncontrollable pain (UK 99.5%; Japan 85.1%). For UK respondents, the proportion agreeing euthanasia was required was also comparable for the scenario whereby the dog was no longer physically able to take in food or water of its own accord (97.2%), or able to control defaecation (90.6%) or able to walk unaided (96.2%). On the other hand, in Japan, 54.8, 32.7, and 24.9% of respondents, respectively, expressed a positive attitude towards the euthanasia under those respective situations and approximately 40% expressed opposition to euthanasia in situations of uncontrolled defaecation or inability to walk. When conditions were suggested relating to the owners’ requests or abilities to take care of the dog, similar responses between both Japanese and UK respondents were observed.

Figure 5. Percentage of UK (n = 212) and Japanese (n = 321) respondents who selected Strongly disagree, Disagree, Neutral, Agree or Strongly agree to euthanase a dog when presented with the scenario of a nine year old dog with an incurable femoral tumour. In addition to the baseline scenario, altered dog and owner scenarios were also presented (as shown on the left of the graph).

In the Factor Analysis (Table 5), for both the UK and Japan, ten variables associated with the dog or owner’s condition were grouped similarly into three dimensions. The first factor identified was characterised by five variables relating to the views of, or interaction with, the owner (factor variance/proportion: UK 2.825/0.717; Japan 2.944/0.636). The second factor was characterised by variables related to the dog’s inabilities to express self-directive behaviours, including eating, drinking, defaecating, and walking (factor variance/proportion: UK 1.742/0.442; Japan 2.459/0.532). The third factor characterised the dog’s mental stress and pain (factor variance/proportion: UK 1.052/0.267; Japan 2.120/0.458). For Japanese respondents, all variables clearly showed the highest factor loadings (more than 0.6 except for two variables) in one of three factors. On the other hand, in the UK, factor loadings of variables in Factors 2 and 3 were lower than those of the Japanese respondents, and dog’s mental health contributed to both Factor 2 (0.34) and 3 (0.39), while in Japan it contributed only to Factor 3 with a higher loading (0.73).

Table 5. Result of factor analysis of respondents’ responses to situations asking when they would or would not advocate euthanasia of a dog with an incurable tumour in the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321). Variables were grouped similarly into three dimensions in both countries. actors with the owner as Factor 1; the dog’s inabilities to express self-directive behaviours as Factor 2; the dog’s mental stress and pain as Factor 3

Discussion

In this study, the attitude of veterinarians and animal behaviour/welfare scientists from the UK and Japan towards animal welfare and animal management were investigated. Most of the participants from both countries were veterinarians however there was a significant difference in the gender ratio. Since most of the UK respondents were under 45, this ratio approximately reflects the current population demographics for veterinarians in each country (MAFF 2023b; IES 2024). We found that respondents from the UK were more familiar with the concept of the FF than those from Japan and considered themselves more confident in explaining these but younger respondents in Japan were more likely to have received some formal education and knowledge in animal welfare. Almost two-thirds of respondents in both countries had sought out education in animal welfare themselves and this contributed to improving their knowledge on the FF compared to undertaking passive education. When asked to nominate the most important freedom(s) for each category of animal, respondents from Japan selected a significantly smaller number of freedoms than those from the UK. Taking the two vignettes, Japanese respondents were more likely than those from the UK to consider that cats should be managed indoors for the entirety of their lives, although the benefits and risks of the cat being outdoors were similarly recognised by both. While 40% of UK respondents considered euthanasia to be the best option for the dog in this vignette, approximately 80% of respondents in Japan expressed their clear opposition to such an outcome. Severe pain and mental stress for the dog were most likely to motivate respondents from both countries to consider euthanasia. However, the association between mental stress and the dog’s inabilities to drink water, eat food, defaecate and walk was recognised in respondents from the UK, not Japan.

Despite those differences both countries prioritise ‘survival-related’ attributes for welfare (health and nutrition) over ‘situation-related’ attributes (behavioural interactions; Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Beausoleil, Littlewood, McLean, McGreevy, Jones and Wilkins2020), although the environmental impacts (discomfort) were also not considered the most important. This may have been a direct result of the majority of participants being veterinarians. For both countries, the respective veterinary curricula focus upon animals’ physical health, with less attention paid to behavioural responses, leading veterinarians to sometimes present the view that health is the core part of welfare (Broom Reference Broom2011). On the other hand, respondents from both countries considered behaviour to be more important for the welfare of wild animals, whether captive or free-ranging. Neither the UK nor Japan extends animal welfare legislation to include free-living wildlife, since it focuses on animals under human control for which a perceived duty of care exists to consider an animal’s physical and mental needs. Wildlife may be considered well adapted to their environments and thus able to access food for as long as they are able to roam and forage. Further, fear and injury are perhaps considered to be reasonably inevitable in their wild habitat. Alternatively, or additionally, the emphasis on behaviour only for wildlife may be associated with an assumption that domesticated animals have less need to show behavioural adaptations and may be less motivated to show specific behaviours. This is not supported by research demonstrating domesticated animals to still continue to show highly motivated behaviours in captivity (e.g. pigs [Sus scrofa]: Stolba & Wood-Gush Reference Stolba and Wood-Gush1984), and prevention of these can cause behavioural frustration and physiological indicators of stress (for a review, see Mason & Burn Reference Mason, Burn, Appleby, Olsson and Galindo2018). Many of the welfare issues facing farm, experimental and companion animals, such as stereotypy, tail-biting, and destructive behaviours, stem from preventing the expression of highly motivated components of the animal’s behavioural repertoire (e.g. Bradshaw et al. Reference Bradshaw, McPherson, Casey and Larter2002; D’Eath et al. Reference D’Eath, Arnott, Turner, Jensen, Lahrmann, Busch, Niemi, Lawrence and Sandøe2014; Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, Hemmings, McBride and Parker2017). These data suggest that respondents from both countries potentially undervalue the impact of behaviour-related welfare issues in favour of more immediate or possibly short-term physical issues. Poor health or a lack of suitable nutrition are clearly very important for the immediate survival of an animal, and it may be that participants considered these ‘survival-critical’ factors in their consideration of the importance of the different factors, rather than taking into account the chronic or ongoing welfare impacts that behavioural restriction can cause. As UK respondents who had sought to find animal welfare education themselves improved their knowledge, education should help people become aware of the importance of behavioural interactions.

There was also a country-specific impact whereby less than 40% of respondents from Japan classified ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ as being important for companion, farm and experimental animals compared to around 70% for UK respondents. This was not explained by any demographic background variables suggesting that some contextual characteristics in Japan might contribute to the attitude. It is possible that the Japanese interpretation of how humans should treat animals (based on Aigo), which focuses more on ‘protection’ of animals, has led to a greater focus on survival-related attributes, while in the UK, a history of considering animal experience and how behaviour can be both a tool in this assessment (Dawkins Reference Dawkins2003, Reference Dawkins2004), and an important component (Fraser Reference Fraser2008; Hemsworth et al. Reference Hemsworth, Mellor, Cronin and Tilbrook2015) predominates. A marked increase of the importance of the ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ for wildlife could be affected by religious and cultural norms in each country (Yeshey et al. Reference Yeshey, Keenan and Nitschke2024). Practically, for instance, the Japanese government officially discourages citizens from feeding wildlife from the viewpoint of respecting the interdependence of wildlife as part of ecology, the potential risk of conflict between animals and humans, and infectious disease (MOE 2021), all of which might mirror Japanese traditional attitudes toward wildlife. On the other hand, in the UK, a survey by the RSPCA (2022) reported that people considered feeding wild birds to be ethically good, suggesting human intervention, at least for wild birds, may be more accepted as an action to improve their welfare.

Our data suggest that both countries’ respondents altered their attitude depending on the category of animal, with the tendency more pronounced in the Japanese. Uchikoshi (Reference Uchikoshi2016) described this disparity in Japan as a “compartmentalised view on animals” and hypothesised that it may be partially explained as people’s concern/interest and academic discipline being compartmentalised depending on the categories into which animals are divided. This unconscious/conscious compartmentalisation might be applied in other countries, including the UK, to a different extent or to different species. In the UK, however, sentient animals, that is those capable of experiencing positive and negative emotions, have been identified as all vertebrates, decapod crustaceans and cephalopods in the Animal Welfare (Sentience) Act 2022. This legal definition of animal sentience might encourage respondents to be aware that these animals should be afforded an equally good quality of life as sentient beings, regardless of their purpose or use by humans and thereby minimise the compartmentalisation.

The use of vignettes describing animal management helped us further investigate how attitudes towards different aspects of welfare translated into preferred actions. In responses to both vignettes, the rather subtle differences in attitude towards the FF in our UK and Japanese respondents resulted in quite marked differences in practices towards animal management. Questions regarding the cat highlighted a clear difference in terms of whether cats should be allowed access to the outdoors, with only 20% of respondents from Japan suggesting the cat should be allowed to go outside compared to over 60% of UK respondents. Interestingly, although all respondents broadly identified the same risks and benefits to welfare in terms of indoor and outdoor management, Japanese respondents tended to estimate the disadvantages more negatively than those from the UK, particularly the risk of injury, conflict with humans and becoming lost. Hofstede (Reference Hofstede2011) proposed six dimensions of national cultures and one of the most remarkable gaps between the UK and Japan was ‘Uncertain avoidance’ (UK score 35; Japan score 92) (Hofstede Reference Hofstede2011; Taniguchi et al. Reference Taniguchi, Enoch, Theofilatos and Ieromonachou2022). This might explain that Japanese respondents viewed the possible lack of control and potential risks the cat may face outdoors more negatively and would thereby seek to avoid these situations more keenly than those from the UK.

These preferences may also be influenced by each country’s social norms, and the relevant legislation and professional training they will have received. In Japan, Standards relating to the Care and Keeping Animals at Home, which is based on the Aigo law, describes that owners of cats should make efforts to keep cats completely indoors (MOE 2013). Government advertisements stress the potential risks of infection, road traffic accidents, being lost, unpredictable pregnancy, and aggressive encounters with other cats (MOE 2014a). Consequently, numbers of completely indoor cats are on the increase and 80% of owned cats in Japan were reportedly kept completely indoors in 2022 (Japan Pet Food Association 2022). In the UK, on the other hand, there is no veterinary consensus regarding indoor or outdoor keeping of cats (Yeates & Yates Reference Yeates and Yates2017), but the People’s Dispensary for Sick Animals (PDSA) PAW report (2024a) suggests that 68% of UK cats have a degree of outdoor access. The Department for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs (DEFRA) makes no recommendation regarding whether cats should live indoors or outdoors in their Code of Practice for the welfare of cats (DEFRA 2017) but they do emphasise the need to provide opportunities for play and exercise indoors if the cat does not go outside. Furthermore, some UK charities, such as Cats Protection and the PDSA describe providing a cat outdoor access as being crucial for their mental health, to provide a natural life and to avoid obesity and stress (Cats Protection 2015; PDSA 2024b). However, these charities, amongst others, also provide information for promotion of natural behaviour in indoor-only cats, giving due consideration to the risks posed by disease, traffic, and potentially becoming lost.

Many countries actively debate the issue of whether or not cats should be allowed to free-roam outside, with the USA, Canada and Australia advocating for increased indoor management of cats, while Europe and New Zealand more commonly sees a mixed indoor/outdoor lifestyle (Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019; Tan et al. Reference Tan, Stellato and Niel2020). However, not all these decisions are predicated on cat welfare, and may be impacted more by risks of predation of native wildlife by outdoor cats, an issue which extends beyond the scope of our study. It is striking, however, that in the UK the benefits of natural behaviour and reduced stress associated with outdoor access are typically emphasised, whereas in Japan it is the negative consequences of injury, disease and conflict that are most likely to be cited. As Aigo represents a subjective and emotional perception there may be no subjective scale to evaluate it. However, the amount of time and effort expended by the owner would be relevant to Aigo ethics, since Japanese respondents consideration of indoor keeping involved complete control over food, water, and the cat’s environment by the owner. Moreover, this closer and more intimate physical relationship encapsulates the love the owner has for their pet. These views mirror the concerns and responses of our participants both in the vignette and in the wider survey.

The second vignette explored views on euthanasia of a dog suffering from an incurable disease. Delayed euthanasia is deemed one of the most prevalent welfare problems for companion animals in the UK (Rioja-Lang et al. Reference Rioja-Lang, Bacon, Connor and Dwyer2020), and timely euthanasia is considered an important aspect of a veterinarian’s responsibilities (British Veterinary Association 2016). In our vignette, around 40% of UK respondents considered euthanasia of the dog in the baseline scenario to be appropriate and less than 20% disagreed, whereas more than 80% of respondents from Japan showed clear disagreement with this action. Nearly 90% of dogs die through euthanasia in the UK and an average case number for euthanasia per clinic is 5.80 per month (Dickinson et al. Reference Dickinson, Roof, Roof and Paul2014; Pegram et al. Reference Pegram, Gray, Packer, Richards, Church, Brodbelt and O’Neill2021), suggesting it to be a common experience for small animal veterinarians. However, in a separate survey, in response to questions regarding whether or not they had conducted euthanasia, over 20% of veterinarians in Japan had not in the previous year, and the average number was 2.48 occasions per clinic per year (Sugita & Irimajiri Reference Sugita and Irimajiri2016), suggesting that UK veterinary respondents were much more likely than their Japanese counterparts, to have experience of euthanasia of dogs. A previous interview study reported that veterinarians practising in the UK did not typically regard euthanasia as stressful unless they felt an attachment to the animal, they identified with the owner’s experience or had to deal with convenience euthanasia (O’Connor Reference O’Connor2019). A survey conducted in 2019 for practising veterinarians in Japan reported that respondents having not experienced euthanasia found the procedure more stressful than respondents who had experience (Sugita Reference Sugita2022). This suggests that differences in experience might influence how positively euthanasing an animal is viewed.

It has been suggested, under the Aigo concept or with the Buddhism and Shinto cultural background in Japan, that animals should undergo a natural death and not be killed, which could lead to hesitation to euthanase their pets among owners and veterinarians alike (Tsuruta Reference Tsuruta2012; Sato Reference Sato, Brown, Seddon and Appleby2016). In our study, however, a relatively equivalent percentage of Japanese to UK respondents found euthanasia should be performed when the dog’s severe mental stress and pain were apparent. The survey by Sugita and Irimajiri (Reference Sugita and Irimajiri2016) also reported that 84.9% of veterinarians in Japan agreed to euthanase a companion animal with unrelievable pain or suffering as long as euthanasia is requested by the owner. These results might suggest that Japanese veterinary professionals construe euthanasia as an essential or inevitable procedure for animals that are suffering severely, irrespective of their religious or cultural principles. On the other hand, in the study of Sugita and Irimajiri (Reference Sugita and Irimajiri2016), only 2.7% of veterinarians agreed to euthanase the animal with incurable suffering when the owner requested further treatment, although an aggressive treatment for terminally ill animals is regarded as a stronger stressor than performing euthanasia for veterinarians in Japan as well as the UK (Batchelor & McKeegan Reference Batchelor and McKeegan2012; Sugita Reference Sugita2022). For both countries’ respondents, a request for euthanasia by the owner increased their rate of agreement to euthanase the dog but the change was far more pronounced in the Japanese respondents. These results indicate the possibility that Japan’s much-valued harmonious interdependence between humans (Markus & Kitayama Reference Markus and Kitayama1991), could manifest as veterinary professionals’ decision-making being substantially influenced by an owner’s opinion regarding the best treatment for their pet. Be it to respect their wishes or even to simply avoid conflict.

Our results using factor analysis suggest that for UK respondents an inability to express normal behaviours, such as eating and walking, was associated with mental suffering as well as severe pain while for Japanese respondents those were independent from mental stress. This alternate perception of the dog’s ability to behave could at least partly explain the lack of accord between the respondents from countries as regards opting for euthanasia when the dog could no longer eat, drink, defaecate, or walk unaided. We suggest that the differing definition of animals – ‘sentient beings’ in UK and ‘living beings’ in Japan – could mirror the cultural attitude toward an animal’s life. In the context of euthanasia of companion animals, the ‘quality of life’ and ‘life worth living’ frameworks are discussed in Western culture (Yeates Reference Yeates2011). With the regard to animals’ sentience, the dog’s life would no longer be worth living once there was an inability to engage in maintenance behaviours as the majority of the UK participants agreed with euthanasia in this case. On the other hand, the ‘living beings’ definition could enable the value or length of an animal’s life to be respected as opposed to their experience and feelings since the Japanese respondents considered it ethically justifiable to keep the dog alive. There have been no studies investigating how the ‘living being’ definition affects attitudes towards understanding for animal welfare, but a Japanese report suggested that a pet’s life being ended by a human decision could be deemed a poorer outcome ethically than waiting for the animal to die naturally with suffering (Sugita Reference Sugita2019). As Yeates (Reference Yeates2011) argued, the complexities of quantifying mental experiences of animals and various ethical views regarding the value of an animal’s life have limitations for the ‘life worth living’ concept. The responses to our vignette questions suggest that the thresholds for ‘quality of life’ and ‘a life worth living’ vary between the UK and Japan.

Making the decision to euthanase an animal is highly complex and both owner factors (such as ability to pay, the owners’ perceived benefit of treatment options, owner ability to manage the animal, etc) and animal factors (such as age, individual responses to treatment, relationship with an owner) are relevant. This study used the FF framework to illustrate that contradictory stances on the notion of dog euthanasia held by the UK and Japan might be partially explained by differing perceptions regarding the dog’s ability to express normal behaviours. Asian countries, such as South Korea and Taiwan, are likely to possess a similarly reluctant attitude towards animal euthanasia (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Liao, Chu, Zhai, Yen and Liu2017; UC Davis 2024) while countries with a Western culture, including the US and France, share the same broad attitude that euthanasia for animals is ethically justifiable (Loscos & Marignac Reference Loscos and Marignac2023). However, each country and culture may show small variations. For instance, only 40% of companion animals had their lives ended by euthanasia in Italy (Pugliese et al. Reference Pugliese, Falcone, Alibrandi, Zirilli and Passantino2022), and while Taiwanese law generally prohibits the euthanasia of animals in public shelters, there are no such laws on this in Japan (Yan & Teng Reference Yan and KT-yun2023). Broadening out this study to incorporate a larger number of countries would provide a deeper insight into cultural variation and how it relates to our understanding of animal welfare.

The limitations of this study include the limited representativeness of the population. Since the survey was distributed via email and social media circulation within several academic associations, those candidates may adhere to stricter professional and ethical values as well as possessing a more profound baseline knowledge of animal welfare compared to non-members. Additionally, the questionnaire only being open online and having participants recruited via social media could have caused systematic bias in the study. For instance, respondents possessing greater familiarity with digital searching may have greater access to information advocating for animal welfare. Also, this online approach may have caused an imbalance in age distribution, especially in the UK. Japan, on the other hand, saw a relatively even balance of respondents. Further, original development of questionnaires did not take content validity into account. This study, however, was the first to develop questions based on the FF and vignettes were carefully created to ensure scenarios were realistic for both countries. Further investigation may be needed to confirm validities, but we believe the findings of our study were valid. It would also be important to note the possible biases of a cross-cultural survey. For instance, it is reported that people in collectivism culture (Japanese respondents in this study) will tend to select an answer toward the centre of a scale or a modest answer whereas people characterised by individualism (as in the UK) are more likely to choose a more extreme answer (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Lee and Stevenson1995; Heine et al. Reference Heine, Lehman, Peng and Greenholtz2002). However, during questionnaire completion both countries’ respondents were clear in their expressions of agreement/disagreement and the individualism-collectivism theory to select an extreme/centre answer was not observed. Concrete scenarios can be a useful measure of values when comparing the culture (Heine et al. Reference Heine, Lehman, Peng and Greenholtz2002). Thus, we conclude our vignettes enabled respondents to think about animal welfare situations more realistically. It is possible that the study participants were influenced by ‘socially desirable’ action for animals in each culture as shown in sections of cat-keeping and dog euthanasia, and people in collectivism are reported to be influenced more than those in individualism (Bernardi Reference Bernardi2006). Even if this were the case there was still merit in knowing what factors influenced decision-making on animal management in each country.

Animal welfare implications and Conclusion

There is no doubt that implementing animal welfare standards is important for all animals in the world. However, as the WOAH describes, animal welfare is a multi-dimensional concept, and encouraging diverse societies and people to make changes to improve animal welfare is challenging. Although the FF describe fundamental principles of animal welfare, a variety of factors, including geography, environment, economy, politics and the cultural perception of animals, sometimes do not allow us to provide all freedoms equally. Recently, a cross-cultural study investigating human perceptions of animals found it important to develop a strategy for providing quality of life to an animal with a respect for diverse cultures (Sinclair et al. Reference Sinclair, Lee, Hötzel, de Luna, Sharma, Idris, Derkley, Li, Islam, Iyasere, Navarro, Ahmed, Khruapradab, Curry, Burns and Marchant2022; Yamanashi et al. Reference Yamanashi, Ikkatai, Honjo, Tokuyama, Akami, Wilson and Buchanan-Smith2025). This study is the first to compare understanding of FF in different countries and presents novel evidence that veterinarians and animal welfare scientists in the UK and Japan have distinctive attitudes, particularly towards the ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour.’ Further, the results highlighted this difference can lead to alternative interpretations of what constitutes ‘good’ management of animals. This is very relevant when we consider how welfare improvements may be brought about, in both countries, and may suggest conflicts can occur when the ideals of one country are imposed upon another or presented as the only approach to acceptable animal management. Of particular interest in this study is the Japanese concept of Aigo and our data provide preliminary evidence that this might lead to different views from animal welfare regarding what is ethically good in terms of animal management that should be further investigated. So far, as Aigo is officially translated as ‘welfare’ in the English title of the Aigo law (MOE 2014b), there is the possibility that the idea of animal welfare could be interpreted with a different cultural understanding from the UK. Here, our results reconfirmed that there is no one-size-fits-all strategy to implement animal welfare standards and understanding how people perceive animals in different contexts and cultures is extremely important.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2025.10026.

Acknowledgements

We would like to express deep gratitude to all of our participants for sharing their views with us. This research was supported by JSPS Overseas Challenge Programme for Young Researchers, JSPS Overseas Research Fellowships programme (YO), JSPS post-doctoral fellowship (MK) and JSPS KAKENHI (HK, grant number 23K05515) to support this study. We would thank Dr Rebecca Doyle, Dr Miguel Somarriba Soley, Dr Louise Connelly, Ms Hayley Walters, and Ms Emily Gorman for supporting the survey and for valuable discussions regarding the content of this paper.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: YO, CMD; Data curation: YO, MK, HK, CMD; Formal analysis: YO, MK, CMD; Resources: YO, CMD; Funding acquisition: YO, HK; Investigation: YO, CMD; Methodology: YO, MK, HK, CMD; Project administration: YO, CMD; Software: YO; Validation: YO, MK, HK; Visualisation: YO; Supervision: YO, CMD; Writing (original draft): YO, CMD; Writing (review & editing): YO, MK, HK, CMD

References

Batchelor, CEM and McKeegan, DEF 2012 Survey of the frequency and perceived stressfulness of ethical dilemmas encountered in UK veterinary practice. Veterinary Record 170. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.100262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernardi, RA 2006 Associations between Hofstede’s cultural constructs and social desirability response bias. Journal of Business Ethics 65. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-005-5353-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birch, J, Burn, C, Schnell, A, Browning, H and Crump, A 2021 Review of the evidence of sentience in cephalopod molluscs and decapod crustaceans. LSE Consulting: LSE Enterprise Ltd, The London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK.Google Scholar
Bradshaw, JWS, McPherson, JA, Casey, RA and Larter, IS 2002 Aetiology of separation-related behaviour in domestic dogs. Veterinary Record 151. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.151.2.43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Report, Brambell 1965 Report of the Technical Committee to Enquire into the Welfare of Animals kept under Intensive Livestock Husbandry Systems. HMSO: London, UK.Google Scholar
British Veterinary Association 2016 Guide to euthanasia. https://www.bva.co.uk/media/2981/bva_guide_to_euthanasia_2016.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Broom, DM 2011 A history of animal welfare science. Acta Biotheoretica 59: 121137. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10441-011-9123-3/METRICSCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carnovale, F, Xiao, J, Shi, B, Arney, DR and Phillips, CJC 2024 The effects of occupation, education and dwelling place on attitudes towards animal welfare in China. Animals 14. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14050713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cats Protection 2015 Indoor and outdoor cats essential guide 12. https://www.cats.org.uk/media/1023/eg12_indoor_and_outdoor_cats.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024.Google Scholar
Chen, C, Lee, SY and Stevenson, HW 1995 Response style and cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales among east Asian and North American students. Psychological Science 6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00327.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, MS 2003 Behaviour as a tool in the assessment of animal welfare. Zoology 106: 383387.10.1078/0944-2006-00122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, MS 2004 Using behaviour to assess animal welfare. Animal Welfare 13: S3S7. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0962728600014317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Eath, RB, Arnott, G, Turner, SP, Jensen, T, Lahrmann, HP, Busch, ME, Niemi, JK, Lawrence, AB and Sandøe, P 2014 Injurious tail biting in pigs: How can it be controlled in existing systems without tail docking? Animals 8. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1751731114001359Google ScholarPubMed
DEFRA 2017 Code of practice for the welfare of cats. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5ac77fa4ed915d76a313cd4e/pb13332-cop-cats-091204.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Dickinson, GE, Roof, KW, Roof, PD and Paul, ES 2014 UK veterinarians’ experiences with euthanasia. Veterinary Record 175. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.102636CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyle, R, Wieland, B, Saville, K, Grace, D and Campbell, A 2021 The importance of animal welfare and Veterinary Services in a changing world. OIE Revue Scientifique et Technique 40: 469481. https://doi.org/10.20506/rst.40.2.3238Google Scholar
Farm Animal Welfare Council 1993 Second report on priorities for research and development in farm animal welfare. DEFRA: London, UK.Google Scholar
Foreman-Worsley, R and Farnworth, MJ 2019 A systematic review of social and environmental factors and their implications for indoor cat welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104841CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, D 2008 Understanding animal welfare. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica 50. https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0147-50-S1-S1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, D, Weary, DM, Pajor, EA and Milligan, BN 1997 A scientific conception of animal welfare that reflects ethical concerns. Animal Welfare 6: 187205. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600019795CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazel, SJ, Signal, TD and Taylor, N 2011 Can teaching veterinary and animal-science students about animal welfare affect their attitude toward animals and human-related empathy? Journal of Veterinary Medical Education 38: 7483. https://doi.org/10.3138/JVME.38.1.74/ASSET/IMAGES/SMALL/12_38.1HAZEL_F02.GIFCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, SJ, Lehman, DR, Peng, K and Greenholtz, J 2002 What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.903CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemsworth, PH, Mellor, DJ, Cronin, GM and Tilbrook, AJ 2015 Scientific assessment of animal welfare 2015 New Zealand Veterinary Journal 63: 2430. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.966167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G 2011 Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede Model in context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture 2. https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honjo, M 2022Japan: The case of Aigo.’ In: Peters, A, Stilt, K and Stucki, S (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Global Animal Law. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Huang, WH, Liao, AT, Chu, PY, Zhai, SH, Yen, IF and Liu, CH 2017 A 3-year surveillance on causes of death or reasons for euthanasia of domesticated dogs in Taiwan. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2017.08.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Japan Pet Food Association 2020 https://petfood.or.jp/data/chart2020/10.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Japan Pet Food Association 2022 https://petfood.or.jp/data/chart2022/3.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Loscos, A and Marignac, G 2023 Palliative care for dogs and cats in France: A qualitative and descriptive study based on professionals of the field. Ethics, Medicine and Public Health 31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jemep.2023.100953CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, HR and Kitayama, S 1991 Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivationPsychological Review 98: 224253.10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, GJ and Burn, CC 2018 Frustration and boredom in impoverished environments. In: Appleby, MC, Olsson, IA and Galindo, F (eds) Animal Welfare, 3rd Edition. CABI: Wallingford, UK.Google Scholar
Mellor, DJ, Beausoleil, NJ, Littlewood, KE, McLean, AN, McGreevy, PD, Jones, B and Wilkins, C 2020 The 2020 Five Domains Model: Including human-animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals 10: 124. https://doi.org/10.3390/ANI10101870CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MOE 2013 Standards Relating to the Care and Keeping Animals at Home. https://www.env.go.jp/nature/dobutsu/aigo/2_data/laws/nt_h25_82.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
MOE 2021 A Guideline in Protection and Control of Wild Birds and Mammals and Hunting Management Law. https://www.env.go.jp/nature/choju/plan/pdf/plan1-1b-R03.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Nakanishi, Y 2016 The principle of animal welfare in the EU and its influence in Japan and the world. In: Nakanishi, Y (eds) Contemporary Issues in Environmental Law. Environmental Protection in the European Union pp 87113. Springer: Tokyo, Japan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55435-6_6Google Scholar
O’Connor, E 2019 Sources of work stress in veterinary practice in the UK. Veterinary Record 184. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.104662Google ScholarPubMed
Pegram, C, Gray, C, Packer, RMA, Richards, Y, Church, DB, Brodbelt, DC and O’Neill, DG 2021 Proportion and risk factors for death by euthanasia in dogs in the UK. Scientific Reports 11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-88342-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugliese, M, Falcone, A, Alibrandi, A, Zirilli, A and Passantino, A 2022 Risk factors regarding dog euthanasia and causes of death at a veterinary teaching hospital in Italy: Preliminary results. Veterinary Sciences 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/vetsci9100554CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radford, M 2001 Animal Welfare Law in Britain: Regulation and Responsibility. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.10.1093/oso/9780198262510.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RSPCA 2022 The Animal Kindness Index. https://www.rspca.org.uk/whatwedo/latest/kindnessindex2022/report (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Rioja-Lang, F, Bacon, H, Connor, M and Dwyer, CM 2020 Prioritisation of animal welfare issues in the UK using expert consensus. Veterinary Record 187. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.105964CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, K, Hemmings, AJ, McBride, SD and Parker, MO 2017 Causal factors of oral versus locomotor stereotypy in the horse. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 20: 3743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sato, S 2016 Applied animal behaviour science in Japan and the culture of ‘aigo. In: Brown, JA, Seddon, YM and Appleby, MC (eds) Animals and Us pp 227240. Wageningen Academic Publishers:Wageningen, The Netherlands.10.3920/978-90-8686-828-5_11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, JA 2004 Factors influencing human attitudes to animals and their welfare. Animal Welfare 13: S145151. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600014500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, M, Lee, NYP, Hötzel, MJ, de Luna, MCT, Sharma, A, Idris, M, Derkley, T, Li, C, Islam, MA, Iyasere, OS, Navarro, G, Ahmed, AA, Khruapradab, C, Curry, M, Burns, GL and Marchant, JN 2022 International perceptions of animals and the importance of their welfare. Frontiers in Animal Science 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fanim.2022.960379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolba, A and Wood-Gush, DG 1984 The identification of behavioural key features and their incorporation into a housing design for pigs. Annales de Recherches Veterinaires 15: 287299.Google ScholarPubMed
Sugita, H 2019 Should pets suffering from fatal diseases be euthanized? Japanese attitudes toward euthanasia of pets as seen in data of JGSS-2006. JGSS Research Series 6: 5372.Google Scholar
Sugita, H 2022 A research report on veterinarians’ stress levels in the proccess of euthanasia. Osaka Shougyou Daigaku Ronshu 17: 4561.Google Scholar
Sugita, H and Irimajiri, M 2016 A survey of veterinarians’ attitudes toward euthanasia of companion animals in Japan. Anthrozoos 29: 297310. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2016.1152722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, SML, Stellato, AC and Niel, L 2020 Uncontrolled outdoor access for cats: An assessment of risks and benefits. Animals 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taniguchi, A, Enoch, M, Theofilatos, A and Ieromonachou, P 2022 Understanding acceptance of autonomous vehicles in Japan, UK, and Germany. Urban, Planning and Transport Research 10: 514535. https://doi.org/10.1080/21650020.2022.2135590CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tokyo City University 2017 https://www.ishes.org/docs/20170202_Press%20Release.pdf (accessed 29 August 2024).Google Scholar
Tsuruta, N 2012 The ethical issue of euthanasia on pets. Shakai to Rinri 27: 153165.Google Scholar
UC Davis 2024 https://www.vetmed.ucdavis.edu/node/23556 (accessed 17 April 2025).Google Scholar
Uchikoshi, A 2016 Public Policies about the Domestic Animals and Wild Lives in Japan. Nihon no doubutsu seisaku. Nakanishiya Shuppan: Kyoto, Japan.Google Scholar
Yamanashi, Y, Ikkatai, Y, Honjo, M, Tokuyama, N, Akami, R, Wilson, DA and Buchanan-Smith, HM 2025 A comparison of attitudes towards animal welfare between British and Japanese zoo visitors: Where and when do cultural differences diverge? PLoS ONE 20: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0320241CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, TY and KT-yun, Teng 2023 Trends in animal shelter management, adoption, and animal death in Taiwan from 2012 to 2020. Animals 13. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13091451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, J and Yates, D 2017 Staying in or going out? the dilemma for cat welfare. Veterinary Record 8: 193194. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.j938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, JW 2011 Is ‘a life worth living’ a concept worth having? Animal Welfare 20: 397406. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600002955CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeshey, Ford RM, Keenan, RJ and Nitschke, CR 2024 Religious beliefs and wildlife value orientations influence tolerance of wildlife impacts in Bhutan. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 29: 300318. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209.2023.2234389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Detailed descriptions of two fictional scenarios (vignettes) which formed part of a survey to understand attitudes to animal welfare in Japanese (n = 321) and UK (n = 212) veterinary professionals. These vignettes formed the baseline scenario from which respondents were asked their views on how the animals should be managed

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographic information of 533 veterinary professional respondents who took part in a survey of attitudes to animal welfare from UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321). The number of respondents in each category are presented with the proportion of the total shown as a percentage in parenthesis

Figure 2

Figure 1. The percentage of UK (n = 212) and Japanese (n = 321) respondents in a survey of attitudes to animal welfare who considered themselves able to explain 0, 1, 2, 3, 4 or 5 of the Five Freedoms to others.

Figure 3

Table 3. Result of binary logistic regression using demographic data to investigate predictors of the number of the Five Freedoms respondents can explain to others in the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321) (Original responses are scaled as 0-3=0, 4-5=1). In both countries, respondents who undertook self-education were able to explain more Freedoms whereas age and profession were also associated for Japanese respondents

Figure 4

Figure 2. Histogram showing (a) UK (n = 212) and (b) Japanese (n = 321) respondents’ responses when asked to choose which of the Five Freedoms was most important for companion, farm, experimental, zoo or wildlife animals. Each bar shows the percentage of respondents who selected the freedom in question as most important. Respondents were able to select 1 to 5 freedoms for each category of animals. Key: Hunger/thirst: Freedom from hunger, malnutrition and thirst; Fear/distress: Freedom from fear and distress; Discomfort: Freedom from discomfort; Injury/disease: Freedom from pain, injury and disease; Behaviour: Freedom to express normal patterns of behaviour.

Figure 5

Table 4. Result of binary logistic regression using demographic data to investigate predictors for selecting ‘Freedom from injury and disease’ and ‘Freedom to express normal behaviour’ as the most important (or equally most important to others) of the Five Freedoms from respondents in the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321)

Figure 6

Figure 3. Bar chart showing the percentage of UK (n = 212) and Japanese (n = 321) respondents’ answers when presented with a series of management options relating to the scenario of a city-dwelling owner with a single cat. Respondents were allowed to select 1 of 6 management categories that differed in terms of whether the cat was indoor, outdoor, or both; and whether the decision was made by the cat or the owner.

Figure 7

Figure 4. Box and whisker plot of the relative value of a cat going outdoors in relation to relevant factors as selected by respondents as the benefit or risk factors for cat management, in comparison to staying completely indoors. The relative value was calculated by subtracting the answers when the cat was managed completely indoors from the answers when access to outdoors was provided. The box and whisker plot represents the minimum, first quartile, median, third quartile, and maximum. Cross marks represent an average, and asterisks show significant differences between the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321). *P < 0.05, Mann-Whitney U test.

Figure 8

Figure 5. Percentage of UK (n = 212) and Japanese (n = 321) respondents who selected Strongly disagree, Disagree, Neutral, Agree or Strongly agree to euthanase a dog when presented with the scenario of a nine year old dog with an incurable femoral tumour. In addition to the baseline scenario, altered dog and owner scenarios were also presented (as shown on the left of the graph).

Figure 9

Table 5. Result of factor analysis of respondents’ responses to situations asking when they would or would not advocate euthanasia of a dog with an incurable tumour in the UK (n = 212) and Japan (n = 321). Variables were grouped similarly into three dimensions in both countries. actors with the owner as Factor 1; the dog’s inabilities to express self-directive behaviours as Factor 2; the dog’s mental stress and pain as Factor 3

Supplementary material: File

Otani et al. supplementary material 1

Otani et al. supplementary material
Download Otani et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 212.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Otani et al. supplementary material 2

Otani et al. supplementary material
Download Otani et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 311.1 KB