Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-11T13:07:13.778Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lexical influences on predictive mouse cursor movements

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2025

Anuenue Kukona*
Affiliation:
School of Human Sciences, University of Greenwich, London, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Two mouse cursor tracking experiments investigated lexical prediction. Participants heard predictive sentences (e.g., “What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the…”) and viewed visual arrays with predictable targets (e.g., book) and phonological competitors (e.g., bull) or unrelated distractors (e.g., goat). Participants tasked with clicking on the (i.e., target) object referred to in sentences (Experiment 1), or with doing so interleaved with a cloze procedure (e.g., completing “What the librarian will read is this.”; Experiment 2), made predictive mouse cursor movements to targets. However, predictive attraction to phonological competitors was not observed. Implications for theories of predictive sentence processing are discussed.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Lexical prediction refers to the pre-activation of word forms (e.g., reflecting their phonology and/or orthography) before these words are heard or read during sentence processing (e.g., Luke & Christianson, Reference Luke and Christianson2016). Lexical prediction has attracted considerable attention (e.g., De Long et al., Reference DeLong, Urbach and Kutas2005), but is not without controversy (e.g., Nieuwland et al., Reference Nieuwland, Politzer-Ahles, Heyselaar, Segaert, Darley, Kazanina, Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, Bartolozzi, Kogan, Ito, Mézière, Barr, Rousselet, Ferguson, Busch-Moreno, Fu, Tuomainen, Kulakova, Husband, Donaldson, Kohút, Rueschemeyer and Huettig2018). Crucially, how pervasive lexical prediction is and how pervasive its effects are across methodologies remain unresolved. The aim of this study was to assess the pervasiveness of lexical prediction by using mouse cursor tracking to capture comprehenders’ predictive behaviours.

Predictive sentence processing is supported by evidence from the visual world paradigm (e.g., Tanenhaus et al., Reference Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard and Sedivy1995). For example, participants in Altmann and Kamide (Reference Altmann and Kamide1999) heard predictive sentences like ‘The boy will eat the…’ and viewed visual scenes with predictable targets like a cake and other unrelated distractors. Before hearing the predictable target word (e.g., “cake”), participants predictively fixated the target, suggesting that it was pre-activated. However, these findings do not resolve whether comprehenders pre-activate the forms of words (i.e., throughout, form is used to refer to phonological and/or orthographic representations) or merely their semantics (e.g., edible). Building on these findings, participants in Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018) heard predictive sentences like ‘The tourists expected rain when the sun went behind the…’ and viewed visual arrays with predictable targets like a cloud, phonological competitors like a clown or unrelated distractors like a globe. Before hearing the predictable target word (e.g., “cloud”), participants predictively fixated the phonological competitor, suggesting that the form (e.g., “kla”) of the target word was pre-activated, which co-activated the phonological competitor. Kukona (Reference Kukona2020) replicated this finding, and studies in Mandarin (e.g., Li et al., Reference Li, Li and Qu2022; Li & Qu, Reference Li and Qu2024; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Ji, Li and Pickering2024; Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Ding, Wang, Chen and Li2023) also provide converging evidence. Taken together, these findings suggest that comprehenders generate lexical predictions during sentence processing.

The study of lexical prediction is closely connected to the cloze procedure (e.g., Taylor, Reference Taylor1953). Participants in the cloze procedure are typically tasked with completing (i.e., incomplete) sentence fragments. For example, (i.e., norming) participants in Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018) were tasked with completing sentence fragments like ‘In order to have a closer look, the dentist asked the man to open his _’, which 100% of participants completed with the word ‘mouth’. The cloze probability of ‘mouth’ was thus 100%, which is calculated as the percentage of participants who provided this completion. Cloze probabilities provide a measure of lexical predictability that is widely used in other tasks (e.g., to confirm the predictability of stimuli). The cloze procedure also provides evidence that comprehenders can activate (e.g., produce) word forms that are predictable given a sentence fragment, which is not unlike lexical prediction, at least when they are explicitly tasked with doing so. Moreover, Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018) hypothesise that comprehenders can generate lexical predictions during sentence processing generally by engaging cognitive processes like they do in the cloze procedure, including production processes (e.g., which are essential for producing word completions in the cloze procedure), which they term prediction-by-production.

Lexical prediction is not without controversy. For example, participants in De Long et al. (Reference DeLong, Urbach and Kutas2005) read sentences like ‘The day was breezy so the boy went outside to fly…’, which had a high cloze probability ending like ‘a kite’ or a low cloze probability ending like ‘an airplane’ with differing articles. During the article, high versus low cloze probability endings yielded differing ERP responses, suggesting that the form (e.g., consonant versus vowel onset) of the noun was pre-activated. These findings complement Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018) and support theoretical approaches like prediction-by-production (e.g., Pickering & Gambi, Reference Pickering and Gambi2018), suggesting that comprehenders generate lexical predictions during sentence processing. In contrast, Nieuwland et al. (Reference Nieuwland, Politzer-Ahles, Heyselaar, Segaert, Darley, Kazanina, Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, Bartolozzi, Kogan, Ito, Mézière, Barr, Rousselet, Ferguson, Busch-Moreno, Fu, Tuomainen, Kulakova, Husband, Donaldson, Kohút, Rueschemeyer and Huettig2018) recruited hundreds of participants across multiple labs and did not replicate this finding from De Long et al. (Reference DeLong, Urbach and Kutas2005), suggesting that comprehenders may not generate lexical predictions. Clark (Reference Clark2013) hypothesises that prediction is fundamental to cognition, yet these findings suggest that lexical prediction may be surprisingly elusive.

The aim of this study was to capture comprehenders’ ability to pre-activate word forms using mouse cursor tracking. To summarise, while lexical prediction is supported by findings from the visual world paradigm (e.g., Ito et al., Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018), evidence from the wider literature is mixed (e.g., Nieuwland et al., Reference Nieuwland, Politzer-Ahles, Heyselaar, Segaert, Darley, Kazanina, Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, Bartolozzi, Kogan, Ito, Mézière, Barr, Rousselet, Ferguson, Busch-Moreno, Fu, Tuomainen, Kulakova, Husband, Donaldson, Kohút, Rueschemeyer and Huettig2018). Thus, fundamental questions remain about the pervasiveness and importance of lexical prediction, and further study, applying a diversity of methodologies, is essential for advancing understanding. Across a range of psycholinguistic studies, participants’ mouse cursor movements have been found to closely complement their eye movements. For example, participants in Allopenna et al. (Reference Allopenna, Magnuson and Tanenhaus1998) heard words like ‘beaker’ and viewed visual arrays with targets like a beaker, phonological competitors like a beetle and unrelated distractors like a carriage. Shortly after hearing the onset of the word, participants fixated the competitor more than distractor. Complementing this finding, participants in Spivey et al. (Reference Spivey, Grosjean and Knoblich2005) heard words like ‘candle’ and viewed visual arrays with targets like a candle and phonological competitors like a candy or unrelated distractors like a jacket. Participants’ mouse cursor movements to targets (i.e., which they were tasked with clicking on) were more attracted to competitors than distractors, which Kukona and Jordan (Reference Kukona and Jordan2023) also replicated using internet-mediated mouse cursor tracking. In addition, predictive sentence processing is supported by evidence from mouse cursor tracking. For example, participants in Kukona (Reference Kukona2023; see also Kukona & Hasshim, Reference Kukona and Hasshim2024) heard predictive sentences like ‘What the man will ride, which is shown on this page, is the…’ and viewed visual arrays with predictable targets like a bike and unrelated distractors like a kite. Before hearing the predictable target word (e.g., “bike”), participants made predictive mouse cursor movements to the target, suggesting that it was pre-activated (e.g., including when hearing rapid speech). These findings complement Altmann and Kamide (Reference Altmann and Kamide1999), but likewise, do not resolve whether comprehenders pre-activate the forms of words or merely their semantics (e.g., rideable).

Building on both Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018) and Kukona (Reference Kukona2023), this internet-mediated study assessed (i.e., predictive) phonological competition using mouse cursor tracking. In Experiment 1, participants heard predictive sentences like ‘What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the…’ and viewed visual arrays (e.g., see Figure 1) with predictable targets like a book and phonological competitors like a bull or unrelated distractors like a goat. In Experiment 2, to assess task effects, these trials were interleaved with a cloze procedure. If comprehenders generate lexical predictions during sentence processing, participants were expected to make (i.e., predictive) mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors before hearing predictable target words (e.g., “book”).

Figure 1. Object visual array depicting the predictable target book and phonological competitor bull for the example sentence, “What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the book.”.

2. Experiment 1

Experiment 1 assessed lexical prediction by measuring mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors (e.g., bull) of predictable targets (e.g., “What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the…”). Crucially, participants’ (i.e., predictive) mouse cursor movements were assessed before they heard predictable target words (e.g., “book”). This experiment was pre-registered (https://osf.io/93mja) and the data and analysis code are available at OSF (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G48CQ).

Method

Participants

Fifty-two monolingual English speakers from the UK with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing (age M = 37.77, SD = 12.18; 37 female, 14 male, 1 other) were recruited from prolific.com. The sample size enabled detection of an average two-level within-participants psychological effect size (d z = 0.40, power = 0.80, alpha = 0.05; Brysbaert, Reference Brysbaert2019).

Design and materials

Object type (phonological competitor and unrelated distractor) was manipulated within participants. Twenty-eight object sets were created, which each included a target (e.g., book), phonological competitor (e.g., bull), and unrelated distractor (e.g., goat) from the MultiPic databank of visual stimuli (Duñabeitia et al., Reference Duñabeitia, Crepaldi, Meyer, New, Pliatsikas, Smolka and Brysbaert2018). Targets and phonological competitors shared an onset (e.g., bʊ…), which was not shared with unrelated distractors. A predictive sentence (e.g., “What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the book.”), in which both a noun/NP (e.g., “librarian”) and verb/VP (e.g., “read”) were associated with the target, was recorded for each object set using a synthesised text-to-speech Neural2 voice. The mean sentence duration was 4.51 seconds, and the mean onsets of ‘which’ and the target (e.g., “book”) were 2.21 and 3.97 seconds, respectively. Based on latent semantic analysis (e.g., Landauer & Dumais, Reference Landauer and Dumais1997), phonological competitors and unrelated distractors were closely matched in their semantic relatedness to targets (competitors: M = 0.11, SD = 0.08; distractors: M = 0.08, SD = 0.07), target-associated nouns (competitors: M = 0.07, SD = 0.07; distractors: M = 0.07, SD = 0.09), and target-associated verbs (competitors: M = 0.09, SD = 0.07; distractors: M = 0.10, SD = 0.09) (cosines were unavailable for barista or shapeshift). Objects were used as both phonological competitors and unrelated distractors across targets, controlling non-target properties. Two counterbalanced lists (e.g., see Table 1 of the Appendix for the full list of stimuli) were created, which each included all 28 target and 28 non-target objects and paired one half of targets with phonological competitors and the other half with unrelated distractors. Each target was paired with both its phonological competitor and unrelated distractor across lists.

Procedure

The internet-mediated experiment was created in PsychoPy (Peirce et al., Reference Peirce, Gray, Simpson, MacAskill, Höchenberger, Sogo, Kastman and Lindeløv2019) and run on pavlovia.org. Participants were presented with one practice trial followed by 28 experimental trials. On each trial, participants were presented a blank visual array followed by an object visual array like Figure 1. Visual arrays used normalised coordinates ranging from −1 to 1. In object visual arrays, a target and either phonological competitor or unrelated distractor were presented, sized 0.30 × 0.60 (i.e., in normalised units, reflecting 0.30/2 = 15% of the width of the visual display and 0.60/2 = 30% of the height of the visual display) and centred at (±0.85, 0.70). In blank visual arrays, each object was replaced with a shaded box. Participants clicked on an icon at the bottom of the visual display (0, −0.85) to begin each trial. Each trial began with the blank visual array. After 0.50 seconds, participants heard the (i.e., predictive) sentence. At the onset of ‘which’ in the sentence, participants were presented the object visual array. Thus, following Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018), the visual array did not provide information relevant to the target until after the target-associated noun and verb (e.g., “librarian will read…”) were heard. Participants were instructed to click on the (i.e., target) object referred to in each sentence. Trial order and object location were randomised.

Results

One participant who did not complete the experiment and three participants whose data was sampled at less than 30 Hz were removed from the analysis. Mean accuracy was 99.92% (SD = 0.52), which is comparable to Kukona (Reference Kukona2020, Reference Kukona2023; e.g., >99%). Inaccurate trials and trials with log RTs more than 2.5 standard deviations above the global mean were also removed from the analysis (0.82%). Figure 2A depicts mean trajectories across the visual array by object type. Trajectories were aggregated by dividing each trial into 101 normalised time slices and inverting the horizontal axis for targets on the left (e.g., see Spivey et al., Reference Spivey, Grosjean and Knoblich2005). Normalised coordinates were only used to depict participants’ mean trajectories across the visual array and were not analysed statistically. Figure 2B depicts mean horizontal × coordinates by object type in 0.10 time slices from 2 seconds before target onset to 1 second afterward. Positive horizontal × coordinates reflect attraction to targets, while negative horizontal × coordinates reflect attraction to non-targets.

Figure 2. Time-normalised mean mouse cursor trajectories across the visual array (A) and mean (shaded bands show SEs) horizontal mouse cursor movements (i.e., x coordinates) from 2 seconds before the onset of the predictable target word (e.g., "book") to 1 second afterward (B) with phonological competitors (e.g., bull) versus unrelated distractors (e.g., goat) in Experiment 1.

The analysis focused on (i.e., raw) horizontal × coordinates during the two second time window preceding the onset of the target (e.g., depicted in Figure 2B), which reflected (i.e., predictive) behaviours typically just after hearing the target-associated noun and verb (e.g., “librarian will read…”) but always before hearing the target (e.g., “book”). A (i.e., mean) predictive horizontal × coordinate was calculated for each trial by averaging across the within-trial horizontal × coordinates during the (i.e., predictive) time window. Log RTs were computed from the onset of ‘which’. No responses were made before the onset of ‘which’, but 14.45% of responses were made before the onset of the target; on these trials, predictive horizontal × coordinates were averaged up to a response was made. In addition, 38.23% of responses were made before the offset of the target/sentence.

Following the preregistration plan for this experiment, trial-level predictive horizontal × coordinates and RTs were submitted to linear mixed effects models with deviation-coded fixed effects of object type (phonological competitor = −0.5, unrelated distractor = 0.5) and random intercepts and slopes by participants and items. Models were run in R using lme4 (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017), and their random effects were simplified when there were issues with fit. The analysis of predictive horizontal × coordinates revealed a significant intercept, Est. = 19.09 × 10−2, SE = 1.49 × 10−2, t(57.84) = 12.78, p < 0.001, such that trajectories were attracted to targets over non-targets, and a non-significant effect of object type, Est. = −0.89 × 10−2, SE = 1.20 × 10−2, t(25.04) = −0.74, p = 0.46, such that trajectories did not differ between phonological competitors (M = 0.20, SD = 0.11) and unrelated distractors (M = 0.19, SD = 0.10). Likewise, the analysis of RTs (which excluded random slopes by participants due to issues with fit) revealed a non-significant effect of object type, Est. = −0.44 × 10−2, SE = 1.41 × 10−2, t(25.87) = −0.31, p = 0.76, such that RTs did not differ between phonological competitors (M = 2.42, SD = 0.44) and unrelated distractors (M = 2.39, SD = 0.43).

As an exploratory analysis, Bayes factors were also computed by aggregating predictive horizontal × coordinates and RTs by participants and submitting these to participant t-tests comparing phonological competitors and unrelated distractors. The analysis of both predictive horizontal × coordinates, t(47) = 0.82, p = 0.42, d x = 0.12, BF01 = 4.65, and RTs, t(47) = 0.90, p = 0.37, d x = 0.13, BF01 = 4.36, provided substantial support (BF01 > 3) for a null effect. Finally, maximum signed deviations were also analysed, which did not yield significant experimental effects; this is included in the analysis code.

Discussion

Participants hearing predictive sentences like ‘What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the…’ made mouse cursor movements to predictable objects like a book rather than a bull or goat. Consistent with Kukona (Reference Kukona2023), these results reveal that comprehenders generate predictions during sentence processing and that comprehenders’ mouse cursor movements are sensitive to these predictions. However, lexical prediction, as reflected in mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors like a bull, was not observed; rather, exploratory Bayes factors provide evidence against predictive phonological competition. These results suggest that participants generated conceptual but not lexical predictions, pre-activating the semantics but not forms of words.

Experiment 2 aimed to build on Experiment 1 by assessing task effects. In Experiment 2, mouse cursor tracking trials (i.e., from Experiment 1) were interleaved with a cloze procedure, which explicitly tasked participants with activating word forms that were predictable given a sentence fragment. Inspired by Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018; also see Hintz et al., Reference Hintz, Meyer and Huettig2016), if explicit engagement in a cloze procedure boosts lexical prediction (e.g., across trials), participants were expected to make predictive mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors on interleaved mouse cursor tracking trials.

3. Experiment 2

Experiment 2 again assessed lexical prediction by measuring mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors of predictable targets. As in Experiment 1, participants’ (i.e., predictive) mouse cursor movements were assessed before they heard predictable target words. To assess task effects, these trials were interleaved with a cloze procedure that tasked participants with completing similar sentences (e.g., “What the librarian will read is this.”). This experiment was pre-registered (https://osf.io/e96j5) and the data and analysis code are available at OSF (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G48CQ).

Method

Participants

Fifty-two participants (age M = 36.24, SD = 10.23, 1 unreported; 28 female, 23 male, 1 other) were recruited following the same criteria as Experiment 1.

Design and materials

In contrast to Experiment 1, the task was manipulated within participants. On one half of trials, participants performed the mouse cursor tracking task from Experiment 1. On the other half of trials, they performed a cloze procedure. For the mouse cursor tracking task, the design and materials were the same as Experiment 1. For the cloze procedure, each predictive sentence was modified to include the target-associated noun/NP and verb/VP but not the target (e.g., “What the librarian will read is this.”).

Four counterbalanced lists were created with all 28 targets, which presented one half of targets in the mouse cursor tracking task and the other half in the cloze procedure and paired one half of targets in the former with its phonological competitor and the other half with its unrelated distractor. Each target was presented in both tasks and paired with both non-targets across lists.

Procedure

The internet-mediated experiment was again created in PsychoPy (Peirce et al., Reference Peirce, Gray, Simpson, MacAskill, Höchenberger, Sogo, Kastman and Lindeløv2019) and run on pavlovia.org. Participants were presented with one practice trial from each task (i.e., two practice trials total) followed by 14 experimental trials from each task (i.e., 28 experimental trials total). The tasks were randomly interleaved. Participants clicked on an icon at the bottom of the visual display (0, −0.85) to begin each trial. For the mouse cursor tracking task, the procedure was the same as Experiment 1. For the cloze procedure, each trial began with the blank visual array. After 0.50 seconds, participants heard the (i.e., cloze) sentence. At the onset of ‘is’ in the sentence, participants were presented a text entry box. Participants were not presented with any objects. Thus, information about the task was not provided until after the target-associated noun and verb (e.g., “librarian will read…”) were heard. Participants were instructed to type the first word(s) to come to mind.

Results

In the cloze procedure, the target reflected the modal response for 85.71% of sentences. The mean percentage of responses reflecting the target (i.e., the cloze probability) was 77.30% (SD = 23.08, Min = 32.26%, Max = 100.00%). Probabilities are reported by sentence in the appendix.

In the mouse cursor tracking task, two participants whose data was sampled at less than 30 Hz were removed from the analysis. Mean accuracy was 100% (SD = 0.00). Trials with log RTs more than 2.5 standard deviations above the global mean were also removed from the analysis (1.57%). Figure 3A depicts mean trajectories across the visual array by object type, and Figure 3B depicts mean horizontal × coordinates by object type. No responses were made before the onset of ‘which’, but 38.97% of responses were made before the onset of the target, and 56.74% of responses were made before the offset of the target/sentence.

Figure 3. Mouse cursor trajectories across the visual array (A) and horizontal mouse cursor movements from 2 seconds before the onset of the predictable target word (B) with phonological competitors versus unrelated distractors in Experiment 2.

Following the preregistration plan for this experiment, trial-level predictive horizontal × coordinates and RTs were submitted to similar analyses to Experiment 1. The analysis of predictive horizontal × coordinates revealed a significant intercept, Est. = 23.01 × 10−2, SE = 1.35 × 10−2, t(51.77) = 17.08, p < 0.001, such that trajectories were attracted to targets over non-targets, and a non-significant effect of object type, Est. = −0.90 × 10−2, SE = 1.39 × 10−2, t(20.74) = −0.65, p = 0.52, such that trajectories did not differ between phonological competitors (M = 0.24, SD = 0.09) and unrelated distractors (M = 0.22, SD = 0.10). Likewise, the analysis of RTs (which excluded random slopes by participants due to issues with fit) revealed a non-significant effect of object type, Est. = 0.89 × 10−2, SE = 2.76 × 10−2, t(24.27) = 0.32, p = 0.75, such that RTs did not differ between phonological competitors (M = 2.13, SD = 0.66) and unrelated distractors (M = 2.13, SD = 0.64).

As an exploratory analysis, Bayes factors were also computed by aggregating predictive horizontal × coordinates and RTs by participants and submitting these to participant t-tests comparing phonological competitors and unrelated distractors. The analysis of both predictive horizontal × coordinates, t(49) = 0.88, p = 0.38, d x = 0.13, BF01 = 4.50, and RTs, t(49) = −0.08, p = 0.93, d x = −0.01, BF01 = 6.48, provided substantial support (BF01 > 3) for a null effect.

In addition, predictive horizontal × coordinates and RTs were combined across experiments and submitted to linear mixed effects models with deviation-coded fixed effects of object type and experiment (E1 = −0.5, E2 = 0.5), as well as their interaction and random intercepts and slopes by participants and items. The analysis of predictive horizontal × coordinates (which was simplified to included random intercepts by participants and items and random slopes by items for object type due to issues with fit) revealed a significant intercept, Est. = 21.02 × 10−2, SE = 1.11 × 10−2, t(84.24) = 18.98, p < 0.001, and a significant effect of experiment, Est. = −3.85 × 10−2, SE = 1.80 × 10−2, t(99.20) = −2.14, p < 0.05, such that trajectories were attracted to targets over non-targets in both experiments, and this attraction was also greater in Experiment 2 than 1. In contrast, the analysis revealed a non-significant effect of object type, Est. = −0.89 × 10−2, SE = 0.96 × 10−2, t(32.89) = −0.93, p = 0.36, and interaction, Est. = 0.01 × 10−2, SE = 1.73 × 10−2, t(121.82) = 0.00, p = 1.00. Likewise, the analysis of RTs (which was simplified to included random intercepts by participants and items due to issues with fit) revealed a significant effect of experiment, Est. = 17.10 × 10−2, SE = 5.38 × 10−2, t(95.03) = 3.18, p < 0.01, such that RTs were faster in Experiment 2 than 1, and a non-significant effect of object type, Est. = 0.07 × 10−2, SE = 1.58 × 10−2, t(28.93) = 0.04, p = 0.97, and interaction, Est. = −1.02 × 10−2, SE = 2.23 × 10−2, t(1895.33) = −0.46, p = 0.65. Finally, maximum signed deviations were also analysed, which did not yield significant experimental effects; this is included in the analysis code.

Discussion

Consistent with Experiment 1, participants made predictive mouse cursor movements to targets but not phonological competitors. These results again suggest that participants pre-activated the semantics but not forms of words. In addition, while the interleaved cloze procedure did not affect mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors, it did facilitate both mouse cursor movements to targets and RTs, suggesting that predictive sentence processing is sensitive to task. Finally, the cloze procedure confirmed that targets were predictable on average (e.g., in comparison, a criterion of 67% is reported by Luke & Christianson, Reference Luke and Christianson2016).

4. General discussion

Two mouse cursor tracking experiments investigated lexical prediction. Participants hearing predictive sentences like ‘What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the…’ made predictive mouse cursor movements to predictable targets like a book but not phonological competitors like a bull (e.g., versus unrelated distractors like a goat). These results provide novel insight into predictive sentence processing, suggesting that varieties of prediction are distinguished by motor movements of the hand but also raising questions about the pervasiveness and importance of lexical prediction.

These results complement a range of findings from the psycholinguistic literature. Participants generated predictions (e.g., reflecting verb selectional restrictions), complementing Altmann and Kamide (Reference Altmann and Kamide1999), and their mouse cursor movements were sensitive to these predictions, complementing Kukona (Reference Kukona2023). Moreover, participants in Experiment 1 activated phonological competitors after target word onset (e.g., reflective of spoken word recognition), complementing Spivey et al. (Reference Spivey, Grosjean and Knoblich2005); as depicted in Figure 2B, mouse cursor trajectories were attracted to phonological competitors (M = 0.70, SD = 0.16) over unrelated distractors (M = 0.75, SD = 0.10) from 0.50 to 1 second after target word onset, t(45) = −2.26, p < 0.05. Finally, mouse cursor movements to phonological competitors before target word onset, reflecting lexical predictions, were not observed, complementing Nieuwland et al. (Reference Nieuwland, Politzer-Ahles, Heyselaar, Segaert, Darley, Kazanina, Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, Bartolozzi, Kogan, Ito, Mézière, Barr, Rousselet, Ferguson, Busch-Moreno, Fu, Tuomainen, Kulakova, Husband, Donaldson, Kohút, Rueschemeyer and Huettig2018).

These results also contrast with a growing body of evidence for lexical prediction from the visual world paradigm (e.g., Ito et al., Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018; Kukona, Reference Kukona2020; Li et al., Reference Li, Li and Qu2022; Li & Qu, Reference Li and Qu2024; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Ji, Li and Pickering2024; Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Ding, Wang, Chen and Li2023). There is perhaps no doubt that comprehenders can activate word forms (e.g., reflecting their phonology and/or orthography) that are predictable given a sentence fragment, which they robustly do in the cloze procedure. Moreover, Ito (Reference Ito2024) conducted a meta-analysis on studies addressing predictive phonological competition in the visual world paradigm (e.g., including Ito et al., Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018) and found a small but significant (e.g., lexical prediction) effect. Thus, even outside a cloze procedure, comprehenders can pre-activate word forms that are predictable. However, the results of Experiments 1 and 2 suggest that the pre-activation of word forms may be optional rather than necessary, complementing Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018). This optionality may depend on a range of factors (e.g., timing, cognitive resources, etc.), but a factor highlighted by these results is lexical predictability. Relatedly, cloze probability was a significant mediator of the predictive phonological competition effect in Ito’s (Reference Ito2024) meta-analysis, which was based on studies with mean cloze probabilities ranging from 85% to 98%. The mean cloze probability in Experiments 1 and 2 was just outside this range (i.e., 78%), which may explain these (e.g., null) results. However, a limitation of this study is that lexical predictability was not manipulated systematically (e.g., experimentally). Thus, an important direction for future research will be to carefully address the (e.g., quantitative) relationship between lexical predictability and lexical prediction.

The literature distinguishes varieties of prediction. For example, Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018) distinguish prediction-by-production from prediction-by-association, and Luke and Christianson (Reference Luke and Christianson2016) distinguish lexical prediction from graded prediction (e.g., also see Huettig, Reference Huettig2015). Relatedly, an important difference between Nieuwland et al. (Reference Nieuwland, Politzer-Ahles, Heyselaar, Segaert, Darley, Kazanina, Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, Bartolozzi, Kogan, Ito, Mézière, Barr, Rousselet, Ferguson, Busch-Moreno, Fu, Tuomainen, Kulakova, Husband, Donaldson, Kohút, Rueschemeyer and Huettig2018) and these experiments is that while neither supported lexical prediction, these results robustly support (e.g., non-lexical) pre-activation. Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018) argue that prediction-by-production, which relies on production processes, ‘constitutes the most effective mechanism for prediction during language comprehension’ (p. 1014). However, these results highlight a potential important caveat: lexical prediction may be an extreme rarity that is only effective when cloze probabilities approach ceiling. Based on a more generous criterion (i.e., a cloze probability above 67%), Luke and Christianson (Reference Luke and Christianson2016) estimated that predictable words comprised only 5% of content words, suggesting that lexical prediction may be extremely rare.

We conjecture that these results are underpinned by conceptual predictions, such that participants pre-activated the semantics but not forms of words. Relatedly, Luke and Christianson (Reference Luke and Christianson2016) describe graded prediction as the prediction of linguistic features, and Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018) hypothesise that comprehenders can generate predictions during sentence processing based on spreading activation among associated concepts in memory, which they term prediction-by-association. Accordingly, participants in Experiments 1 and 2 may have pre-activated the concept of a book when hearing ‘the librarian will read…’ based on spreading activation among the concepts (e.g., or features; Luke & Christianson, Reference Luke and Christianson2016) corresponding to librarian, read and book without necessarily pre-activating the word ‘book’. Supporting prediction-by-association, Kukona et al. (Reference Kukona, Cho, Magnuson and Tabor2014) found that participants pre-activated associated but unpredictable concepts. Their participants heard predictive sentences like ‘The boy will eat the white…’ and viewed visual arrays with predictable targets like a white cake, adjective-associated competitors like a white car and other unrelated distractors. Before hearing the predictable target word (e.g., “cake”), participants predictively fixated the competitor, suggesting that associated but unpredictable concepts were pre-activated (e.g., also see Kukona et al., Reference Kukona, Braze, Johns, Mencl, Van Dyke, Magnuson, Pugh, Shankweiler and Tabor2016; Langlois et al., Reference Langlois, Ness, Kim and Novick2024; Nozari et al., Reference Nozari, Trueswell and Thompson-Schill2016; Prystauka et al., Reference Prystauka, Altmann and Rothman2024; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Veríssimo, Schad, Oltrogge, Vasishth and Lago2021). Moreover, this variety of prediction may constitute an especially effective mechanism for prediction in these experiments and the visual world paradigm generally, because associations between the unfolding language and visual stimuli can guide prediction. However, semantically associated competitors were not included in this study, so it provides only indirect evidence of graded prediction and/or prediction-by-association.

These results also suggest that predictive behaviours are task-sensitive. In Experiment 2, mouse cursor tracking trials were interleaved with a cloze procedure, and both mouse cursor movements to targets and RTs were facilitated. In addition, Figure 3B suggests that mouse cursor movements were not attracted to phonological competitors after target word onset, which may be because over 50% of responses were made before target word offset (e.g., versus under 40% in Experiment 1). These results complement Hintz et al. (Reference Hintz, Meyer and Huettig2016), whose participants read predictable (e.g., “the man breaks … a glass”) or non-predictable (e.g., “the man borrows … a glass”) sentences. When participants performed a self-paced reading task interleaved with a naming task, reading times were facilitated on predictable sentences in the former, but when participants only performed a self-paced reading task, this facilitation was not observed. Taken together, Hintz et al. (Reference Hintz, Meyer and Huettig2016) and Experiment 2 support a link between prediction and production, suggesting that engaging in the latter may facilitate the former across trials, although neither provides specific support for lexical prediction.

This study also highlights important methodological considerations. An important difference between these experiments and Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018) is that the latter did not depict targets and competitors together in the same visual array. One potential limitation of mouse cursor tracking is that targets are typically depicted with competitors to provide a destination for participants’ mouse cursor movements, which may swamp potential competitor effects (e.g., see Ito, Reference Ito2024). Thus, how lexical prediction is affected by the visual array is a potential direction for future research. Relatedly, factors such as participants’ screen sizes, whether participants used a mouse or trackpad and whether participants used speakers or headphones were uncontrolled in this internet-mediated study. Thus, how lexical prediction is affected by these uncontrolled factors remains unresolved. Nevertheless, complementing Kukona (Reference Kukona2023), these results again suggest that internet-mediated mouse cursor tracking is sensitive to at least some aspects of predictive sentence processing. In addition, while lexical prediction was not observed in this study, Experiment 1 afforded more power than Ito et al. (Reference Ito, Pickering and Corley2018), with more participants (e.g., 52 versus 24) and items (e.g., 28 versus 16; although the latter was halved in Experiment 2). Relatedly, Ito’s (Reference Ito2024) meta-analysis also emphasises that more careful attention to power is necessary in future research.

In conclusion, these results suggest that varieties of prediction are distinguished by motor movements of the hand, such that participants generated conceptual but not lexical predictions. Complementing Pickering and Gambi (Reference Pickering and Gambi2018), these results suggest that lexical prediction may be optional rather than necessary. Finally, complementing Kukona (Reference Kukona2023), these results also suggest that mouse cursor tracking is a powerful tool for investigating predictive sentence processing.

Data availability statement

The data and analysis code are available at OSF (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/G48CQ).

Competing interest

The author declares none.

Appendix

Table 1 reports predictive sentences, predictable targets, phonological competitors and unrelated distractors from Experiments 1 and 2, as well as target cloze probabilities from Experiment 2.

Table 1. Stimuli from Experiments 1 and 2

References

Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38(4), 419439. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247264. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00059-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 11171121. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1504CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, J. A., Crepaldi, D., Meyer, A. S., New, B., Pliatsikas, C., Smolka, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2018). MultiPic: A standardized set of 750 drawings with norms for six European languages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(4), 808816. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1310261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2016). Encouraging prediction during production facilitates subsequent comprehension: Evidence from interleaved object naming in sentence context and sentence reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ), 69(6), 10561063. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1131309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huettig, F. (2015). Four central questions about prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1626, 118135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ito, A. (2024). Phonological prediction during comprehension: A review and meta-analysis of visual-world eye-tracking studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 139, 104553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2024.104553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Pickering, M. J., & Corley, M. (2018). Investigating the time-course of phonological prediction in native and non-native speakers of English: A visual world eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 98, 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukona, A. (2020). Lexical constraints on the prediction of form: Insights from the visual world paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(11), 21532162. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000935Google ScholarPubMed
Kukona, A. (2023). Predictive sentence processing at speed: evidence from online mouse cursor tracking. Cognitive Science, 47(4), e13285. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13285CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kukona, A., Braze, D., Johns, C. L., Mencl, W. E., Van Dyke, J. A., Magnuson, J. S., Pugh, K. E., Shankweiler, D. P., & Tabor, W. (2016). The real-time prediction and inhibition of linguistic outcomes: Effects of language and literacy skill. Acta Psychologica, 171, 7284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.09.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kukona, A., Cho, P. W., Magnuson, J. S., & Tabor, W. (2014). Lexical interference effects in sentence processing: Evidence from the visual world paradigm and self-organizing models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 326347. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034903Google ScholarPubMed
Kukona, A., & Hasshim, N. (2024). Mouse cursor trajectories capture the flexible adaptivity of predictive sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 50(10), 16501661. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001397Google ScholarPubMed
Kukona, A., & Jordan, A. (2023). Online mouse cursor trajectories distinguish phonological activation by linguistic and nonlinguistic sounds. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 30(1), 362372. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02153-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 126. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211240. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langlois, V. J., Ness, T., Kim, A. E., & Novick, J. M. (2024). Does cognitive control modulate referential ambiguity resolution? A remote visual world study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 39, 924938. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2024.2369182CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, X., Li, X., & Qu, Q. (2022). Predicting phonology in language comprehension: Evidence from the visual world eye-tracking task in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 48(5), 531547. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000999Google ScholarPubMed
Li, X., & Qu, Q. (2024). Verbal working memory capacity modulates semantic and phonological prediction in spoken comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 31(1), 249258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-023-02348-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luke, S. G., & Christianson, K. (2016). Limits on lexical prediction during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 88, 2260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwland, M. S., Politzer-Ahles, S., Heyselaar, E., Segaert, K., Darley, E., Kazanina, N., Von Grebmer Zu Wolfsthurn, S., Bartolozzi, F., Kogan, V., Ito, A., Mézière, D., Barr, D. J., Rousselet, G. A., Ferguson, H. J., Busch-Moreno, S., Fu, X., Tuomainen, J., Kulakova, E., Husband, E. M., Donaldson, D. I., Kohút, Z., Rueschemeyer, S.-A., & Huettig, F. (2018). Large-scale replication study reveals a limit on probabilistic prediction in language comprehension. ELife, 7, e33468. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33468CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nozari, N., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2016). The interplay of local attraction, context and domain-general cognitive control in activation and suppression of semantic distractors during sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 19421953. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1068-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, J., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman, E., & Lindeløv, J. K. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 195203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C. (2018). Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 10021044. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prystauka, Y., Altmann, G. T., & Rothman, J. (2024). Online eye tracking and real-time sentence processing: On opportunities and efficacy for capturing psycholinguistic effects of different magnitudes and diversity. Behavior Research Methods, 56(4), 35043522. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-023-02176-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spivey, M. J., Grosjean, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(29), 1039310398. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0503903102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, K., Veríssimo, J., Schad, D. J., Oltrogge, E., Vasishth, S., & Lago, S. (2021). The interaction of grammatically distinct agreement dependencies in predictive processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 36(9), 11591179. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2021.1921816CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 16321634. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7777863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, W. L. (1953). “Cloze procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Quarterly, 30, 415433. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769905303000401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, X., Ji, C., Li, T., & Pickering, M. J. (2024). The prediction of segmental and tonal information in Mandarin Chinese: an eye-tracking investigation. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2024.2395549Google Scholar
Zhao, Z., Ding, J., Wang, J., Chen, Y., & Li, X. (2023). The flexibility and representational nature of phonological prediction in listening comprehension: evidence from the visual world paradigm. Language and Cognition, 16(2), 481504. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2023.38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Object visual array depicting the predictable target book and phonological competitor bull for the example sentence, “What the librarian will read, which is shown here, is the book.”.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Time-normalised mean mouse cursor trajectories across the visual array (A) and mean (shaded bands show SEs) horizontal mouse cursor movements (i.e., x coordinates) from 2 seconds before the onset of the predictable target word (e.g., "book") to 1 second afterward (B) with phonological competitors (e.g., bull) versus unrelated distractors (e.g., goat) in Experiment 1.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Mouse cursor trajectories across the visual array (A) and horizontal mouse cursor movements from 2 seconds before the onset of the predictable target word (B) with phonological competitors versus unrelated distractors in Experiment 2.

Figure 3

Table 1. Stimuli from Experiments 1 and 2