Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-tfzs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-27T17:56:04.472Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social function in adolescent eating disorders: lived experience framework for clinical practice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2025

Dasha Nicholls*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
Daniella Boules
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
Nikita Julius
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
Emerie Sheridan
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
Victoria Burmester
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK
*
Correspondence: Dasha Nicholls. Email: d.nicholls@imperial.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Social function is increasingly demonstrated as a factor in risk, maintenance and outcome of eating disorders, but not emphasised in theoretical models of, and treatment approaches to, adolescent eating disorders.

Aims

To adapt Schmidt and Treasure’s cognitive interpersonal model of anorexia nervosa to incorporate developmental and transdiagnostic components.

Method

Qualitative interviews with young people aged 12–16 years (inclusive), who are in contact with child and adolescent community eating disorders services, and their parents, subjected to thematic analysis.

Results

Five key themes emerged that were mutually dependent on a sixth theme of emotion regulation and coping. These themes were: peer relationships, change and uncertainty, thinking styles, appearance and achievement-based values, and family relationships.

Conclusions

Peer relationships emerged as distinct from family relationships in this population, and a unifying theme was emotion regulation and coping. The framework could guide clinical assessment and the development or adaptation of interventions to address the themes identified. Research is needed to understand the role of the themes in treatment response and outcomes.

Information

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Social brain networks undergo dramatic changes during adolescence, Reference Blakemore1 resulting in heightened social awareness and self-consciousness, peer-directed social interactions and increased complexity of relationships. Reference Steinberg and Morris2 Early adolescence is associated with greater emotionality, and fosters abstract reasoning and social stress susceptibility. Reference Bingham, McFadden, Zhang, Bhatnagar, Beck and Valentino3 Peers surpass parents as the primary source of social support, Reference Masten, Juvonen and Spatzier4 and motivation for intimate interpersonal relationships increases. Reference Furman and Buhrmester5 Peer relations have a protective effect against mental ill health, Reference La Greca and Harrison6 but social anxiety can disrupt their formation and stability. Abstract reasoning and emotional processing skills emerge, but inhibitory control over risky behaviours does not develop until later adolescence. Reference Goddings, Burnett Heyes, Bird, Viner and Blakemore7 Intact hormonal systems are necessary for these processes to develop, systems that are affected by illness. Adolescence is therefore a period of learning and brain growth, during which mental health interventions may have particular success. Reference Fuhrmann, Knoll and Blakemore8

This sensitive period coincides with the onset of eating disorders. Eating disorders affect over 13% of young people and adult patients, Reference Allen, Byrne, Oddy and Crosby9,Reference Stice, Marti and Rohde10 with a cost to the UK economy of £15 billion/year. 11 According to Beat, the UK eating disorder charity, eating disorder prevalence has increased 7% year on year since 2009, 11 and hospital admissions for eating disorders have risen, although this trend may be changing. 12 Despite their prevalence, our understanding and treatment repertoire is limited, and eating disorder research funding is inadequate. Social cognition and the quality of interpersonal relationships predict the development of eating disorder Reference Stice13 course and outcome. Reference Stice13,Reference Wentz, Gillberg, Anckarsäter, Gillberg and Råstam14 Up to 37% of adults with anorexia nervosa – the most common diagnosis in eating disorder services – have an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or ASD features, Reference Westwood and Tchanturia15 with slightly lower estimates in adolescent eating disorder populations. Prolonged illness accentuates ASD traits, but ASD is sexually dimorphic, so, in the absence of a strongly suggestive developmental history, it is often difficult to diagnose in females who may have higher cognitive empathy. Since eating disorders disproportionately affect females, this may result in underdiagnosis.

Causal or maintenance models of eating disorders have largely been developed and empirically tested in adult populations. The cognitive interpersonal model of anorexia nervosa (Fig. 1), outlined initially in 2006, has underpinned studies examining cognitive and emotional traits in anorexia nervosa, and to develop and test interventions. Most recently, the model has been applied to the neuroprogression that results in chronic illness, Reference Treasure and Schmidt16 and suggests that autism spectrum conditions (ASC) and traits have no direct impact on physical outcomes or eating disorder symptoms, but could be associated with higher rates of comorbidities and greater use of intensive eating disorder treatment. Patients with ASC characteristics may benefit more from individual rather than group sessions. Reference Tchanturia, Smith, Glennon and Burhouse17 Treatment adaptations such as the ‘PEACE’ pathway, a co-produced approach to anorexia nervosa comorbid with ASC, Reference Goddard, Hibbs, Raenker, Salerno, Arcelus and Boughton18 show early evidence of cost-savings and favourable treatment outcomes, suggesting that recognising and addressing aspects of social function within treatment approaches has the potential to influence course and outcome.

Application of the cognitive interpersonal model to young people has not been fully explored. One study that included young people found they had a better treatment response than adults to specialist in-patient or day patient treatment for anorexia nervosa (adult units n = 12; adolescent units n = 2; total patients n = 177), and that carer behaviour and interpersonal functioning may influence the response to in-patient care and recovery. Reference Goddard, Hibbs, Raenker, Salerno, Arcelus and Boughton18 Yet evidence that problems in social functioning are pertinent to the onset, course and outcome of eating disorders is mounting. Characterising social difficulties unique to young people with eating disorders, and understanding their bidirectional relationship to eating disorder symptoms and progression, is an important next step for potential identification of prodromal symptoms and the design of new interventions.

Fig. 1 The cognitive interpersonal model for anorexia nervosa. Reference Schmidt and Treasure19

The Medical Research Council framework for the development of complex interventions proposes theory development as a first step in the process of intervention development. Consulting young people and parents with lived experience of eating disorders when critiquing and adapting theoretical models ensures that science and research is addressing the needs and concerns of the group they intend to help. The aim of this study was to work with young people and their parents to integrate their views with contemporary scientific findings on social function in eating disorders into the cognitive interpersonal model, as a framework for psychological and pharmacological intervention in this population. The study was part of a programme of work examining the relationship between various aspects of social and interpersonal function in young people with eating disorders.

Method

Adaptation of the model was informed by recent literature and qualitative data from repeated sessions with a young person and parent focus group run at Imperial College London, for people with experience of eating disorders with or without ASC. Participants were recruited via poster advertisements in four community eating disorder clinics within NHS Trusts treating young people with anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa. Participants completed an online screening form to ensure adherence with inclusion and exclusion criteria, and to provide informed consent or parental consent and young person assent to participate.

Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) young people or parent of a young person aged 13–16 years, with anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa (bulimia nervosa), with or without comorbid ASC, treated at a community eating disorder service; (b) fluent in English and (c) have internet access.

Exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) presence of neurological pathology, (b) presence of other serious mental illnesses (e.g. schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), (c) significant life event in the past 30 days (such as a bereavement or financial windfall), (d) hearing or sight impaired without correction, (e) diagnosis of ASD (parent only) and (f) presence or history of eating disorder (parent only).

Objectives of the focus groups for this study (topic guide available from the authors) were to explore:

  1. (a) What is the understanding of parents and young people with lived experience of anorexia nervosa/bulimia nervosa of the role of social functioning in the onset and maintenance of eating disorders?

  2. (b) What model best captures the role of social and interpersonal functioning in the onset and maintenance of eating disorders in adolescents?

  3. (c) How acceptable and valuable to parents and young people with lived experience of anorexia nervosa/bulimia nervosa is the development of new psychological and psychopharmacological interventions that target the social domain?

Data analysis

Data from young people and parents generated were analysed using reflexive thematic analysis, by researchers experienced in social functioning and eating disorders (authors: V.B., N.J. and E.S.). After immersion in the data, each coder independently generated initial themes. We addressed reflexivity by critically examining our own influence on the research process, including our assumptions, positionality and interactions with participants. This involved group discussions to reflect on individual interpretations and potential biases, as well as documenting strong reactions and analytical decisions during manuscript review. To reduce bias, a fourth researcher (D.B.) drew the preliminary themes together from the coders for the parent group and the young person group, and consensus was achieved via meetings with the initial three authors who coded the scripts. A fifth author with expertise in young people with eating disorders (D.N.) synthesised the parent and young person themes and revised them, drawing on the literature. There was no difficulty in reaching consensus with initial codes and themes that were generated individually. No software was used to assist analysis.

Results

Participants were two parent/child dyads: a 13-year-old child with ongoing severe anorexia nervosa, and a 15-year-old child with anorexia nervosa who was recovering. The latter also had a 19-year-old sister in hospital with anorexia nervosa. A further three parents contributed without their children’s involvement. They were the parents of a 14-year-old daughter recently diagnosed with anorexia nervosa, and of a 16-year-old girl with anorexia nervosa and autism, who was diagnosed age 10 years.

Key themes

Five key themes emerged that were mutually dependent on a sixth theme of emotion regulation and coping (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2 Developmental and transdiagnostic adaptation of the cognitive interpersonal model for adolescent eating disorders.

Theme 1: peer relationships

Key elements: low empathy and low social connectedness.

Participants talked about having small friendship groups and the difficulty they had receiving and giving support to friends.

‘I certainly didn’t have a big group of friends and they’re not necessarily close.’ Young person

‘I have quite a small circle, probably about nearly four or five, and I wouldn’t say that I kind of talked to them in school, but not really out of school.’ Young person

‘I found it hard to trust them enough…’ Young person

‘I’m not very good when it comes to sort of people comforting each other.’ Young person

‘It just sort of made me come across as sort of not listening, thinking that they’re not important to me.’ Young person

‘I don’t I think that I’m generally there for my friends when they need me. But I don’t… that does not come from a place of dislike or annoyance or anything. It’s just from being busy.’ Young person

These challenges around friendships became more marked at the height of their illness, when they were focused on their own issues.

‘Maybe sometimes I’m a little bit distracted with myself and like school work. I know it sounds like really selfish.’ Young person

‘I think that because sometimes I can be so caught up in my own thoughts and like obviously cause… I’m just not sometimes not in the mood for putting on a brave face in front of them.’ Young person

‘I just feel so bad for my friends cause I was probably not a really nice person around that time. You know I wouldn’t have been able to support them if they wanted to talk to me about anything.’ Young person

These themes were echoed in parents’ accounts.

‘Now she has a very small group of friends… but I would say that she would find it very difficult to make new friends.’ Parent

‘And umm it was one of the really big things I think was ..sort of holding her back. I think she became – and she would agree that she became - not a very nice person to hang out with. Really. Withdrawn, moody.’ Parent

‘She really got used to having no friends and it didn’t seem to worry her. And we found that really sad as parents.’ Parent

‘It’s very difficult to separate the autism from the eating disorder for our daughter. Because that definitely affects her friendships in that she doesn’t see the need for people.’ Parent

Nonetheless, social support and connection was something both young people and parents viewed as important.

‘I basically missed all my friendships, broke down just because I did not have the energy to be speaking to anyone because I was, so I was struggling to keep myself alive.’ Young person

‘Now I try and make myself listen to music so I can talk to them about it and join in on their conversations.’ Young person

‘Umm, so now she’s got a she’s sort of found a really nice group of friends who are really supportive, and she feels really comfortable with.’ Parent

As well as not feeling they fitted in socially, being a victim of bullying featured strongly in young people’s narratives.

‘I know a lot of people just disliked me for some reason because I had many different interests and didn’t necessarily like what they did.’ Young person

‘I think when I was a lot heavier and I was not… I was bullied more… Even though I’m a pretty unpleasant person.’ Young person

‘She was targeted by one particular girl for the whole of year seven and the school were very much like, it’s not bullying. It’s just you, you know, parents dramatising kind of thing. Then she started self-harming, and they realised when we sent various screenshots and stuff….’ Parent

‘Another girl who was also unwell outed her private story, and that resulted in complete strangers calling her names, spitting at her, harassing her.’ Parent

Theme 2: change and uncertainty

Key elements: pubertal change, school transition, adverse childhood experiences.

Although struggling with change may reflect a degree of cognitive inflexibility, childhood and adolescence is associated with changes outside of young people’s control. For example, pubertal changes brought increasing self-awareness, whereas external changes, such as change of school at age 11 (or 13) years, or resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic, rendered the young person’s world unpredictable. Change has potential to be accompanied by preoccupation with predictability and control. This might apply to friendships (small manageable groups of friends), food (eating at specific times, following dietary rules) or appearance (pigtails exactly symmetrical).

‘(Friends) not being, like, clear or precise with what they want … I’m a bit like overly specific about things, but just saying like I’ll be there and I’ll be there or giving me a time frame because I like plan my entire day around that one thing.’ Young person

‘I quite like things being planned out, so if the plan doesn’t go the way that I want it to, it can cause me a lot of stress and feel overwhelmed.’ Young person

‘Things being orderly and in a specific way and pattern is very reassuring and comforting.’ Young person

‘“A” used to be very popular and have a very large group of friends. Then they all went to one [secondary] school and she went to another and since then she’s never really been able to find her feet with the right group of people. And this is when all the problems started ….’ Parent

‘We’ve gone through a spate of cancer and lots of people and, you know, even their karate teacher died recently.’ Parent

‘Moving schools, COVID, the way this girl treated her – none of that was within her control.’ Parent

Theme 3: thinking styles

Key elements: negative interpretation bias, rejection sensitivity, interpersonal threat perception, low self-worth, rigidity.

In addition to the cognitive inflexibility (difficulties with set-shifting) and perseveration (central coherence) that have been described in anorexia nervosa, a tendency to negative cognitive styles was noted across eating disorder diagnoses were evident in young people’s narratives, aligning with the literature. These include negative interpretation bias, sensitivity to social rejection and low self-worth, such that even pre-planned events perceived as exciting were disappointing when they happened.

‘I don’t want to look at myself and it causes me to feel very low and just like not in the mood to do anything or talk to anyone.’ Young person

‘Often because like I just overthink most of the things I say, I will be too embarrassed.’ Young person

‘if something embarrassing happened at school it would you put me off school for quite a while. I overthink it and too embarrassed for a while because I’m so like my ego is so bruised.’ Young person

‘I was so insecure, but I know that a lot of people treated me very differently from how they did when I lost weight and when I lost weight, people would often compliment me much more, even though I looked terrible, and I felt terrible and I was just a very unpleasant, unpleasant person to be around.’ Young person

Parents also identified sensitivity to social rejection as key triggers of their child’s distress, although interestingly, thought that having autism may provide some protection from this.

‘(Friends) would organise to meet up and then not tell her and then say, oh, you know, you can come at the last minute and then she’d go over and then they would not speak to her the whole evening, just completely blank her. I’d go and pick her up and she would be absolutely just in floods of tears.’ Parent

‘The more she sort of felt rejected, the more she felt she couldn’t speak to them… It’s sort of a kind of a cyclical thing.’ Parent

‘In a way, I’m glad that she’s autistic because I think she’s able to block that out more than maybe my other child who would find that a lot harder.’ Parent

They also mentioned the extent to which rigid thinking and need for control was a factor.

‘You know sort of timings and you know, only with that particular spoon or, you know, it has to be at 6:00 o’clock on the dot.’ Parent

‘My daughter will make an instant decision about a person – there is no changing her mind.’ Parent

‘I honestly don’t know anymore. I thought it was about being thin. Then I thought it was about control. Now I think it’s autism driven.’ Parent

Theme 4: appearance and achievement-based values

Key elements: overvaluation of the thin ideal and perfectionism.

A strong narrative about desire for approval from others, especially friends, was evident, with an emphasis on both appearance and achievement. Academic achievement often took precedence over friendship. In addition to beliefs that people are treated differently on the basis of their weight and shape, young people talked about the importance of makeup, skincare and appearance more broadly, for example, changing behaviours to fit in with or impress peers, such as picking up mannerisms. They did note that close friends tended not to judge on the basis of appearance, while recognising that they treated people differently themselves on this basis.

‘I would in my mind I’d like, ohh, they’d think I’m really overweight. They think I’m fat. They all hate me.’ Young person

‘If I had to choose. I know I would pick the nicer looking one like to be friends with.’ Young person

‘The fact that I’ve been successful, and I have that validation from complete strangers that don’t know me is very reassuring.’ Young person

‘I hold myself to a really high moral standard, and if that’s being questioned by anyone… I have like basic core values that I live by.’ Young person

‘I think she sort of wondered with, you know, would people still like her if she wasn’t the sort of super super skinny person.’ Parent

‘I’ve definitely seen more perfectionism coming in and stupid little things. Like if her plaits aren’t symmetrical, she’ll have a meltdown… or eyelashes, ones got more mascara on than the other and that’s the end of the world. All these things that we would never have had before the anorexia.’ Parent

School was perceived as having an important role in driving perfectionism and competition rather than facilitating social connection, with a lot of importance placed on success and a highly negative opinions of failure.

‘I strive for success and I like, I like success. But I like to know that I succeed at everything. I can’t just go out and know that I’ve failed, that’s quite a mood kill for me.’ Young person

‘I think if I don’t succeed, I don’t feel as important to anyone.’ Young person

‘The fear that I feel is a bit irrational in that it’s like I’m scared that I’m gonna do something wrong even if I haven’t done it wrong yet.’ Young person

’She’s luckily very academic but she obviously feels she’s not good enough and that that breaks my heart more than anything, I think because she is perfect just the way she is.’ Parent

‘She’s just very sort of self-motivated and her friends don’t seem super motivated in the same way. So, it actually defines her.’ Parent

Theme 5: family relationships

Key elements: suboptimal communication with parents, low parental self-efficacy, parent reflective function, familial affection and support.

The central role that parents/family play in young people’s eating disorder journey is reflected in the conflicted relationships with parents, particularly mothers, seen both as their biggest support/champion (major sacrifices made to be available to support, lots of gratitude) and as the enemy (making the young person eat, not trusted as motive is to get them to eat). Families were important for ensuring meals were eaten when possible, and despite being moody, rude and withdrawn during their illness, causing arguments at home, there were also opportunities for affection and confiding. A positive aspect was how their illness had enabled young people to strengthen their relationship with their parents and family.

‘It took me a while to open up to my mum, but after a while I did open up to my mum and dad about what was happening and then they went to the school because they thought that it obviously shouldn’t be happening.’ Young person

‘It took me a while to realise that they were there for me and it took me a while to start to open up to them, but I grew more safe as such around them. It became easier for me to open up and so now if, if I feel strong enough to share how I’m feeling with them, then I go straight to them straight away.’ Young person

‘I get from my parents is reassurance more than anything else. Just because I constantly worry that I’m not enough.’ Young person

‘I think I feel a little closer with my mum because I spent a lot of time with her while I was ill cause you know she has to be with me all the time, so I’d have to tell her lots of things.’ Young person

‘Her little sister was her rock… Yeah, she was literally at her side, you know.’ Parent

‘And my dad. That was a bit difficult because he didn’t really understand what was going on, so he’d just get angry at me all the time and I just felt like he was really disappointed in me for like not being able to do stuff.’ Young person

‘My older sister… she was also very ill at the same time as me. And we had an absolutely terrible relationship… we just argue and argue.’ Young person

‘My dad is not very emotionally available if that’s what you call it, but if he’s feeling upset, he’ll just kind of bust up and then it will come out in like a massive rage.’ Young person

‘And you know, it has brought us closer in a way.’ Parent

Theme 6: overarching theme of emotion regulation/coping

Linking all the main themes were the coping strategies around emotions that the young people emphasised as being crucial to their recovery. They saw these as interacting with each of the other elements rather than as a separate theme. They talked about the ‘real me’, the ‘sick me’, and the newly therapised self. Emotion regulation strategies included avoiding confrontation (such as the reality of their illness), punishing the self and of being too depleted of energy to experience fear or embarrassment. Conversely, they talked about learning to express emotions.

‘Managing my emotions can feel quite overwhelming because obviously there’s a lot of them, so managing them can sometimes feel quite overwhelming.’ Young person

‘I find it difficult to regulate them (emotions) on a daily basis. So like how I’m feeling and be able to express that without shame. Is quite difficult, so I often just tend to repress them.’ Young person

‘I tried to just avoid the mirror in general… in the past and it lets me to feel more negatively about myself.’ Young person

’Sometimes I didn’t know how to react to affection that was being shown to me.’ Young person

‘I just went quiet and stopped talking and would never go to school anymore.’ Young person

‘I think fear stops me.’ Young person

Parents experience of their child’s emotions reflected the inaccessibility that accompanied this avoidance, and the central role of control over self and others.

‘I would say the way that she shows her upset is by restricting her food or I mean she won’t even drink water now. So food and water intake and previous to that it was by the self-harm. I would know she would be having a really tough day by finding like a sharpener in their pocket. That would be a way of her saying to me. Mum, I need help. But now there is no emotional contact really.’ Parent

‘And basically, you know, all the emotion comes out when you try and make them eat. And then the distress around that….’ Parent

‘Maybe she is a perfectionist. Maybe she has control. And this was in the easy way to put herself into control of maybe thoughts that were uncomfortable for her.‘ Parent

‘It’s a mixture of not being able to read emotions and desperately needing that approval.’ Parent

Discussion

In this paper, we worked with young people and parents with lived experience of eating disorders to adapt Schmidt and Treasure’s cognitive interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa to young people and to a wider diagnostic profile, in line with emerging literature on social and emotional functioning in adolescent eating disorders. Where applicable, we retained language used in the Schmidt and Treasure model and, similarly, our adaptation aims to emphasise the interaction between vulnerability traits and eating disorder behaviours, and how they serve to maintain the disorder over time. Although some elements are similar, key differences related to biological and social transitions inherent in adolescent development emerged, and young people and parents suggested a role for some factors in onset as well as maintenance.

Peer relationships and the challenges of developing and maintaining friendships emerged as an area of importance for young people, and as quite distinct from family relationships. Social function is an important outcome predictor across mental disorders, and social cognition in childhood (as rated by parents when their child was age 7 years) strongly predicts disordered eating in later adolescence. This effect was significantly stronger for social cognition than for emotion recognition or bullying, and much stronger for disordered eating outcomes than for self-harm. Reference Warne, Heron, Mars, Solmi, Biddle and Gunnell20 Difficulties in social interaction may take the form of social anxiety, a risk factor for eating disorder; for others, social difficulties may be one element of broader deficits in social function, signalling possible ASC traits such as low empathy and poor social cognition. Research suggests a role for ASC and ASC traits as both a risk factor and prognostic indicator, although data are clearer for adults than for adolescents. Reliably diagnosing emerging neurodevelopmental disorders can be challenging when the emotional and cognitive deficits associated with eating disorders are dominant and autism assessments may not be readily available. Brief, objective and valid measures of neurocognitive processes that suggest ASC are needed to facilitate the diagnostic process, and clarify whether treatment adaptations, such as those outlined in the PEACE pathway, may be required.

Regardless of their nature, social difficulties and low levels of social connectedness rapidly become a perpetuating factor as the eating disorder isolates the individual from normal adolescent interactions, especially if prolonged hospital stay is required. Even when treated in the community, young people’s thinking styles, preoccupations and ‘reality testing’ can impair peer as well as family relationships. Steiger and colleagues demonstrated the prognostic significance of pretreatment social adaptation, defined as social and vocational adjustment, DSM-III-R Axis-V ratings (Highest Level of Adaptive Functioning Past Year on a scale of 0-100), and ‘object-relations’ capacities, to multimodal treatment for adults with bulimia nervosa (n = 44). Reference Steiger, Leung and Thibaudeau21 Pretreatment social adjustment explained substantial and significant proportions of variance in posttreatment binge/purge symptoms, after accounting for eating disorder severity and concurrent psychiatric symptoms. More research is needed to explore the role of social function, including social interactions on social networks, in adolescent eating disorders, and whether targeted interventions are of benefit to those for whom it is a significant contributor to their presentation.

Closely related to social function is the theme of thinking styles and cognitive biases that influence interpersonal functioning. Young people with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa experience heightened sensitivity to social rejection and a negative bias toward their social environment, yet research in young people remains scarce. Rowlands et al Reference Rowlands, Beaty, Simic, Grafton, Hirsch and Treasure22 found that interpersonal sensitivity predicted eating disorder symptoms, which was partially mediated by negative interpretation bias towards social rejection, and poorer social relationships were associated with more severe eating disorder symptoms. It is not clear whether or to what extent the results could be explained by ASC symptoms, which were not assessed. The study also found no attentional social biases when using a dot-probe test experimental paradigm with negative and positive valenced faces. Similarly, a recent study found that adolescents with eating disorders have more negative self-attribution bias than controls, even when controlling for depression, and experimental studies suggest that young adults with both anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa are hypervigilant to social rejection and avoidant of social reward. Reference Burmester, Sheridan, Julius, Elliott, Thackeray and Nicholls23Reference Harper, Palka and McAdams25 More studies are needed to determine how negative verbal appraisal relates to clinical course.

Studies addressing cognitive biases associated with eating disorders are ongoing. Rowlands et al found that remotely delivered computerised cognitive bias modification (CBM) training reduced expectations of social rejection in 67 (all but one) female adolescents with eating disorders randomised to receive CBM in addition to treatment as usual (TAU). Reference Rowlands, Grafton, Cerea, Simic, Hirsch and Cruwys26 Young people who completed the intervention (22/37 randomised) displayed a significantly greater reduction in negative interpretations of ambiguous social scenarios, and eating disorder psychopathology compared with TAU, suggesting that CBM might be a useful treatment adjunct for some patients. There were no significant between-group differences on emotional responses to criticism, or on anxiety and depression symptoms.

Cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) is an adjunct treatment targeting set-shifting and weak central coherence, thought to play a maintaining role in anorexia nervosa, whereas cognitive remediation and emotional skills training aims to address cognitive and emotional factors. Reference Meneguzzo, Bonello, Tenconi and Todisco27 Nine studies included in a systematic review and meta-analysis of CRT in young people suggest improvements in cognitive performance with small effect sizes, positive patient feedback and low drop-out. Reference Tchanturia, Giombini, Leppanen and Kinnaird28,Reference Timko, Bhattacharya, Fitzpatrick, Howe, Rodriguez and Mears29 CRT for anorexia nervosa can be delivered remotely, although the risk of disengagement is slightly higher. Reference Orloff, McGinley, Lenz, Mack and Timko30 However, assessment of set-shifting and central coherence in routine clinical practice may present practical challenges. Furthermore, a recent study of adolescent in-patients with anorexia nervosa found set-shifting and central coherence improved over time irrespective of whether patients received CRT, Reference Herbrich-Bowe, Bentz, Correll, Kappel and van Noort31 underscoring the extent to which cognitive inefficiencies may reflect state rather than trait. Although most studies have been undertaken in anorexia nervosa, both set-shifting and central coherence are comparably impaired in people with bulimia nervosa, although not in binge eating disorder, Reference Keegan, Tchanturia and Wade32 justifying inclusion in our adapted model.

In our model, we included perfectionism as a value rather than as a thinking style, because of its close association with the extent to which striving for success, including around weight and shape, was framed by young people as core beliefs. Meta-cognitive training for eating disorders Reference Balzan, Gilder, Thompson and Wade33 is designed to address cognitive flexibility and perfectionism, with corresponding effects on eating disorder psychopathology. In a feasibility and preliminary efficacy trial, 35 adolescent females were randomised to TAU plus meta-cognitive training for eating disorders, or TAU alone. Reference Balzan, Gilder, Thompson and Wade33 The meta-cognitive training condition had low attrition (<15%) and was positively received. However, between-group differences favouring meta-cognitive training for eating disorders were not sustained at 3-month follow-up.

Although we recognise that problems coping with change is associated with some of the factors outlined above, we included change as a separate construct because change is faster in this developmental period than at any other stage, barring the neonatal period. These biological, psychological and social changes are, for the most part, completely outside of young people’s control. For example, ‘social emotions’ (guilt, shame, embarrassment, pride, contempt) are more sensitive to hormones than those processing basic emotions (fear, disgust, psychic pain), which are better correlated with age. The ability to ‘put yourself in another person’s shoes’ is therefore highly dependent on a functioning hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Furthermore, the brain undergoes up to 50% synaptic pruning during adolescence, Reference West, Sweeting and Young34 making it vulnerable to stress during this period. The centrality of ‘self-control’ has been debated across eating disorder literature for decades. Reference Fairburn, Shafran and Cooper35 Young people’s narratives suggest the need for control in the context of constant transitions and a specific developmental period is qualitatively different from the need for self-control in an otherwise ordered life. Reference Izquierdo, Plessow, Becker, Mancuso, Slattery and Murray36

The role of family relationships to the course, outcome and effectiveness of intervention in young people with eating disorders is well established in both anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Reference Le Grange, Lock, Loeb and Nicholls37,Reference Monteleone, Pellegrino, Croatto, Carfagno, Hilbert and Treasure38 Our adapted model highlights family communication and emotional support from family members. Parental expressed emotion, even marginally elevated, Reference Bohon, Flanagan, Welch, Rienecke, Le Grange and Lock39 is an established mediator of treatment response, Reference Eisler, Simic, Russell and Dare40 and targeting parent critical comments and parental warmth show preliminary efficacy. Reference Aarnio-Peterson, Le Grange, Mara, Modi, Offenbacker North and Zegarac41 Enhancing parental reflective function, through individual or group mentalisation-based therapy interventions, is another possible avenue for therapeutic innovation in adolescent eating disorders, Reference Byrne, Murphy and Connon42 given the impact on treatment response. Reference Jewell, Herle, Serpell, Eivors, Simic and Fonagy43

The final theme is around emotion regulation or coping strategies. Core elements of emotion regulation involve thresholds for emotional triggers, which are strongly related to cognitive processes; recognition of emotions in oneself and others (alexithymia and empathy); moderating emotional intensity, including acceptance of negative emotions; distress tolerance and the ability to use adaptive strategies to modulate emotions/influence behaviours. Reference La Greca and Harrison6,Reference Bergomi, Ströhle, Michalak, Funke and Berking44Reference Koole46 Emotional regulation is gaining traction as a key modifiable transdiagnostic factor in a range of mental disorders, from addiction to autism to self-harm. Reference Youngstrom47 In their systematic review, Jewell et al found that mentalising difficulties and eating disorder pathology were correlated in adolescent eating disorders, with particular emphasis on poor emotion recognition. Reference Jewell, Collyer, Gardner, Tchanturia, Simic and Fonagy48 Learning to recognise and manage emotions is a core task of adolescence, as well as of most forms of therapy. Females generally have superior cognitive theory of mind compared with males, although no gender difference in affective theory of mind has been established.

In conclusion, the cognitive interpersonal model of anorexia nervosa was adapted with young people and parents to incorporate developmental factors and to fit a broader diagnostic profile. It emphasises peer relationships as distinct from family relationships, and highlights the central and unifying theme of emotion regulation. It is intended as a framework to guide clinical assessment and inform new or adaptations of existing interventions to address the themes identified. Further research is needed to understand the role of each of the themes in determining onset, treatment response and outcome, and explore treatment approaches targeting social functioning, e.g. directed psychological interventions and pharmacological agents such as psychedelics. Whether the model is applicable to young people with avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder if overvalued ideas about weight/shape is excluded or modified needs further exploration.

Data availability

Data are available from the corresponding author on request.

Author contributions

D.N. and V.B. designed the study. V.B. conducted the interviews. D.B., N.J., E.S. and V.B. generated the initial themes. D.N. refined the model and wrote the first draft. All authors approved the final version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Rosetrees Foundation (grant number PGS21/10227). D.N. is also supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Applied Research Collaboration Northwest London and NIHR Imperial Biomedical Research Collaboration. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR, the Department of Health and Social Care, NHS England or NHS Improvement.

Declaration of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The study received ethical approval from the Health Research Authority and Health and Care Research Wales (Integrated Research Application System reference: 294230; Research Ethics Committee reference: 22/LO/0495).

References

Blakemore, S. The social brain in adolescence. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9: 267–77.10.1038/nrn2353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L, Morris, AS. Adolescent development. Annu Rev Psychol 2001; 52: 83110.10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bingham, B, McFadden, K, Zhang, X, Bhatnagar, S, Beck, S, Valentino, R. Early adolescence as a critical window during which social stress distinctly alters behavior and brain norepinephrine activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011; 36: 896909.10.1038/npp.2010.229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, CL, Juvonen, J, Spatzier, A. Relative importance of parents and peers: differences in academic and social behaviors at three grade levels spanning late childhood and early adolescence. J Early Adolesc 2009; 29: 773–99.10.1177/0272431608325504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furman, W, Buhrmester, D. Childrens perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Dev Psychol 1985; 21: 1016.10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Greca, AM, Harrison, HM. Adolescent peer relations, friendships, and romantic relationships: Do they predict social anxiety and depression? J Clin Child Adolesc 2005; 34: 49–61.10.1207/s15374424jccp3401_5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddings, A, Burnett Heyes, S, Bird, G, Viner, RM, Blakemore, S. The relationship between puberty and social emotion processing. Dev Sci 2012; 15: 801–11.10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01174.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuhrmann, D, Knoll, LJ, Blakemore, S. Adolescence as a sensitive period of brain development. Trends Cogn Sci 2015; 19: 558–66.10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, KL, Byrne, SM, Oddy, WH, Crosby, RD. DSM–IV–TR and DSM-5 eating disorders in adolescents: prevalence, stability, and psychosocial correlates in a population-based sample of male and female adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol 2013; 122: 720.10.1037/a0034004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stice, E, Marti, CN, Rohde, P. Prevalence, incidence, impairment, and course of the proposed DSM-5 eating disorder diagnoses in an 8-year prospective community study of young women. J Abnorm Psychol 2013; 122: 445.10.1037/a0030679CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BEAT. The Costs of Eating Disorders: Social, Health and Economic Impacts. BEAT, 2015 (https://beat.contentfiles.net/media/documents/The_costs_of_eating_disorders_2015.pdf).Google Scholar
Stice, E. Modeling of eating pathology and social reinforcement of the thin-ideal predict onset of bulimic symptoms. Behav Res Ther 1998; 36: 931–44.10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00074-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wentz, E, Gillberg, IC, Anckarsäter, H, Gillberg, C, Råstam, M. Adolescent-onset anorexia nervosa: 18-year outcome. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 194: 168–74.10.1192/bjp.bp.107.048686CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westwood, H, Tchanturia, K. Autism spectrum disorder in anorexia nervosa: an updated literature review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2017; 19: 41.10.1007/s11920-017-0791-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treasure, J, Schmidt, U. The cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model of anorexia nervosa revisited: a summary of the evidence for cognitive, socio-emotional and interpersonal predisposing and perpetuating factors. J Eating Disorders 2013; 1: 13.10.1186/2050-2974-1-13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tchanturia, K, Smith, K, Glennon, D, Burhouse, A. Towards an improved understanding of the anorexia nervosa and autism spectrum comorbidity: PEACE pathway implementation. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11: 640.10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00640CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddard, E, Hibbs, R, Raenker, S, Salerno, L, Arcelus, J, Boughton, N, et al. A multi-centre cohort study of short term outcomes of hospital treatment for anorexia nervosa in the UK. BMC Psychiatry 2013; 13: 287.10.1186/1471-244X-13-287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, U, Treasure, J. Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive-interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol 2006; 45: 343–66.10.1348/014466505X53902CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warne, N, Heron, J, Mars, B, Solmi, F, Biddle, L, Gunnell, D, et al. Emotional dysregulation in childhood and disordered eating and self-harm in adolescence: prospective associations and mediating pathways. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2023; 64: 797–806.10.1111/jcpp.13738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steiger, H, Leung, F, Thibaudeau, J. Prognostic value of pretreatment social adaptation in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disorder 1993; 14: 269–76.10.1002/1098-108X(199311)14:3<269::AID-EAT2260140305>3.0.CO;2-C3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowlands, K, Beaty, T, Simic, M, Grafton, B, Hirsch, C, Treasure, J, et al. Cognitive bias modification training of attention and interpretation to reduce expectations of social rejection in adolescents with eating disorders: a small efficacy randomized controlled trial. Int J Eat Disorder 2022; 55: 1506–20.10.1002/eat.23809CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burmester, V, Sheridan, E, Julius, NC, Elliott, J, Thackeray, O, Nicholls, D. Oxytocin amplifies negative response to ambiguity in adolescent females with and without eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2025; 33: 691–9.10.1002/erv.3167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardi, V, Turton, R, Brazil, C, Harrison, A, Rowlands, K, Treasure, J, et al. Training Rejection Interpretation in Eating disordeRs (TRIER): preliminary findings of a feasibility study in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Cogn Ther Res 2019; 43: 1109–17.10.1007/s10608-019-10034-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harper, JA, Palka, JM, McAdams, CJ. Interpersonal attribution bias and social evaluation in adolescent eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2023; 31: 258–70.10.1002/erv.2954CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowlands, K, Grafton, B, Cerea, S, Simic, M, Hirsch, C, Cruwys, T, et al. A multifaceted study of interpersonal functioning and cognitive biases towards social stimuli in adolescents with eating disorders and healthy controls. J Affect Disorders 2021; 295: 397404.10.1016/j.jad.2021.07.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meneguzzo, P, Bonello, E, Tenconi, E, Todisco, P. Enhancing emotional abilities in anorexia nervosa treatment: a rolling-group cognitive remediation and emotional skills training protocol. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2024; 32: 1026–37.10.1002/erv.3113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tchanturia, K, Giombini, L, Leppanen, J, Kinnaird, E. Evidence for cognitive remediation therapy in young people with anorexia nervosa: systematic review and meta-analysis of the literature. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2017; 25: 227–36.10.1002/erv.2522CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timko, CA, Bhattacharya, A, Fitzpatrick, KK, Howe, H, Rodriguez, D, Mears, C, et al. The shifting perspectives study protocol: cognitive remediation therapy as an adjunctive treatment to family based treatment for adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Contemp Clin Trials 2021; 103: 106313.10.1016/j.cct.2021.106313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orloff, NC, McGinley, K, Lenz, K, Mack, AS, Timko, CA. Adaptations of cognitive remediation therapy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa for delivery via telehealth. Int J Eat Disorder 2023; 56: 72–9.10.1002/eat.23850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbrich-Bowe, L, Bentz, LK, Correll, CU, Kappel, V, van Noort, BM. Randomized controlled trial of cognitive remediation therapy in adolescent inpatients with anorexia nervosa: neuropsychological outcomes. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2022; 30: 772–86.10.1002/erv.2921CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keegan, E, Tchanturia, K, Wade, TD. Central coherence and set-shifting between nonunderweight eating disorders and anorexia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disorder 2020; 54: 229–43.10.1002/eat.23430CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balzan, RP, Gilder, M, Thompson, M, Wade, TD. A randomized controlled feasibility trial of metacognitive training with adolescents receiving treatment for anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disorder 2023; 56: 1820–5.10.1002/eat.24009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, P, Sweeting, H, Young, R. Transition matters: pupils’ experiences of the primary–secondary school transition in the west of Scotland and consequences for well-being and attainment. Res Pap Educ 2010; 25: 2150.10.1080/02671520802308677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairburn, CG, Shafran, R, Cooper, Z. A cognitive behavioural theory of anorexia nervosa. Behav Res Ther 1999; 37: 113.10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00102-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izquierdo, A, Plessow, F, Becker, KR, Mancuso, CJ, Slattery, M, Murray, HB, et al. Implicit attitudes toward dieting and thinness distinguish fat-phobic and non-fat-phobic anorexia nervosa from avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder in adolescents. Int J Eat Disorder 2019; 52: 419–27.10.1002/eat.22981CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Grange, D, Lock, J, Loeb, K, Nicholls, D. Academy for eating disorders position paper: the role of the family in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disorder 2010; 43: 1.10.1002/eat.20751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monteleone, AM, Pellegrino, F, Croatto, G, Carfagno, M, Hilbert, A, Treasure, J, et al. Treatment of eating disorders: a systematic meta-review of meta-analyses and network meta-analyses. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2022; 142: 104857.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104857CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohon, C, Flanagan, K, Welch, H, Rienecke, RD, Le Grange, D, Lock, J. Expressed emotion and early treatment response in family-based treatment for adolescent anorexia nervosa. Eat Disord 2024; 32: 153–68.10.1080/10640266.2023.2277054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisler, I, Simic, M, Russell, GFM, Dare, C. A randomised controlled treatment trial of two forms of family therapy in adolescent anorexia nervosa: a five-year follow-up. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007; 48: 552–60.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01726.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aarnio-Peterson, CM, Le Grange, D, Mara, CA, Modi, AC, Offenbacker North, E, Zegarac, M, et al. Emotion coaching skills as an augmentation to family-based therapy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa: a pilot effectiveness study with families with high expressed emotion. Int J Eat Disorder 2024; 57: 682–94.10.1002/eat.24149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, G, Murphy, S, Connon, G. Mentalization-based treatments with children and families: a systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry 2020; 25: 1022–48.Google ScholarPubMed
Jewell, T, Herle, M, Serpell, L, Eivors, A, Simic, M, Fonagy, P, et al. Attachment and mentalization as predictors of outcome in family therapy for adolescent anorexia nervosa. Eur Child Adoles Psychiatry 2023; 32: 1241–51.10.1007/s00787-021-01930-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergomi, C, Ströhle, G, Michalak, J, Funke, F, Berking, M. Facing the dreaded: does mindfulness facilitate coping with distressing experiences? A moderator analysis. Cogn Behav Therapy 2013; 42: 2130.10.1080/16506073.2012.713391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratz, KL, Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav 2004; 26: 4154.10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koole, SL. Does emotion regulation help or hurt self-regulation? In Psychology of Self-Regulation: Cognitive, Affective, and Motivational Processes (eds JP Forgas, RF Baumeister, DM Tice): 217–31. Psychology Press, 2009.Google Scholar
Youngstrom, EA. Adding measures of emotion dysregulation to our toolkits. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 2023; 62: 716–7.10.1016/j.jaac.2023.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jewell, T, Collyer, H, Gardner, T, Tchanturia, K, Simic, M, Fonagy, P, et al. Attachment and mentalization and their association with child and adolescent eating pathology: a systematic review. Int J Eat Disorder 2016; 49: 354–73.10.1002/eat.22473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 The cognitive interpersonal model for anorexia nervosa.19

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Developmental and transdiagnostic adaptation of the cognitive interpersonal model for adolescent eating disorders.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.