Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-65tv2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-24T16:58:22.083Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

United in Diversity: Building Bridges in the Study of Political Participation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2025

Marco Giugni*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science and International Relations, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Maria Grasso
Affiliation:
School of Political Science and International Relations, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Marco Giugni; Email: marco.giugni@unige.ch
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This review article points to a number of ways in which the study of political participation has followed separate tracks, hence preventing it from deploying its full potential. We argue that the field stands to benefit much from ‘bridging’ different approaches and insights, combining those from different disciplines or subdisciplines. We review works that try to build such bridges, with the aim of encouraging dialogue across the disciplinary boundaries between political science and political sociology, on the one hand, and sociology and social movement studies, on the other. We advocate that students of political participation take such ‘bridges’ seriously in their work and employ them as the basis for new dynamic theorization. We suggest five possible ways in which the study of political participation may combine different perspectives and research traditions. Three of them are theoretical: bridging rationalist, structuralist and culturalist theoretical approaches; bringing together cognitive (attitudes) and affective (emotions) explanations; and combining macro- and micro-level accounts. The fourth is methodological: bridging qualitative and quantitative methodological approaches. The fifth is substantive: bridging a focus on electoral (institutional) participation with one on non-electoral (non-institutional) participation.

Information

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Government and Opposition Ltd.

The study of political participation has become a major field of investigation in social sciences. Although it is generally considered as part of a specific branch or field of study in political science – namely, political behaviour, which also includes vote choice – it cuts across disciplines. This means it relates not only to political science, but also to sociology, psychology, economics, anthropology, geography and still others (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni, Grasso, Giugni and Grasso2022). As a result, this field of studies, like others, features a variety of theoretical perspectives, methodological approaches and substantive foci. In this review article we discuss ways in which the study of political participation has followed separate tracks, hence preventing it from deploying its full potential. We argue that the field stands to benefit from ‘bridging’ different approaches and insights, combining those from different disciplines or subdisciplines. We review works that try to build such bridges, with the aim of encouraging dialogue across the disciplinary boundaries between political science and political sociology, on the one hand, and sociology and social movement studies, on the other. We advocate that students of political participation take such ‘bridges’ seriously in their work and employ them as the basis for new dynamic theorization.

Following Henry Brady (Reference Brady, Robinson, Shaver and Lawrence1999: 737), we may define political participation as ‘action by ordinary citizens directed towards influencing some political outcomes’. However, while authoritative and highlighting the three key aspects of political participation – it is an action and not simply a belief or attitude, it is done by ordinary citizens and aims at influencing political outcomes – this definition could be understood as somewhat too broad. More recently, Yannis Theocharis and Jan van Deth (Reference Theocharis and van Deth2017) made a laudable effort to develop a more nuanced approach to define political participation. They suggested that there is more than a single, ‘one-size-fits-all’ definition, depending on a number of decision rules. At the most basic level (minimal definition), political participation is an action or activity, it is voluntary or optional, non-professional, and its locus is politics/government/state. If this last feature is absent, however, one can still speak of political participation insofar as it is targeted at the sphere of politics/government/state or aimed at solving collective or community problems (targeted definitions), or if this is done in a political context or is used to express the political aims and intentions of participants – that is, it is politically motivated (circumstantial definitions). This approach suggests that the definition of political participation depends very much on what we consider important as well as the focus we wish to give to some aspects as opposed to others.

Just as there might be different views about what political participation is – or should be – there exist a variety of ways in which it can be studied. The study of political participation has traditionally followed two distinct paths, each related to a specific research field: one in the political science and political sociology traditions and the other in sociology and social movement studies. Although links between the two traditions have recently begun to emerge (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni, Grasso, Giugni and Grasso2022), the two have rarely spoken to each other, let alone having been combined and, even less so, integrated. Furthermore, the field is characterized by a variety of disciplinary viewpoints, epistemological premises, theoretical perspectives and methodological approaches which, also for their part, are rarely considered together.

We believe that there are at least five possible ways in which the study of political participation may combine different perspectives and research traditions. Three of them are theoretical: bridging rationalist, structuralist and culturalist theoretical approaches; bringing together cognitive (attitudes) and affective (emotions) explanations; and combining macro- and micro-level accounts. A fourth is methodological: bridging qualitative and quantitative methodological approaches; a fifth way is substantive: bridging a focus on electoral (institutional) participation with one on non-electoral (non-institutional) participation. The remainder of the article discusses these five potential ‘bridges’ and makes a number of suggestions for building stronger connections between different approaches in scholarship on political participation. While these suggestions are not new as such, we trust they are presented in such a formal and explicit way to offer researchers new perspectives for the study of political participation.

Reason, structure or culture?

Since the emergence of the discipline in the 19th century, sociologists have debated the ontological underpinnings of human behaviour. One of the leitmotifs has been the debate over the primacy of structure over culture and vice versa. This debate is well represented through the opposition of a Marxist ontology with its focus on structural (economic) determinants of behaviour over cultural (ideological) epiphenomena, and a Weberian ontology stressing the fundamental role played by cultural aspects, including religion.

Another long-standing debate in the social sciences, which is partly linked to the previous one, is between structure and agency. While there have been laudable attempts to discuss and possibly overcome this (apparent) dichotomy (Archer Reference Archer1995; Bourdieu Reference Bourdieu1977; Giddens Reference Giddens1984), social theory has often stressed either the primacy of structure, leaving little margin for manoeuvre for human action or agency, while frequently neglecting the role of structural factors. Structural-functionalist thinking typically gives priority to structure, while rational choice theory – but also other approaches – tends to prioritize agency. This second debate is partly related to the first one as often culturalists have leaned towards agency, albeit with a different focus relative to rational choice theorists, while structuralists have obviously put structure first. Sometimes the same tension is present within the same author in different moments: for example, the early work of Talcott Parsons of the Structure of Social Action (Reference Parsons1937) is very different from the later one of the Social System (Reference Parsons1951).

If we combine these two fundamental debates in some way, we can outline three distinct ways of approaching the study of political participation, which has rarely engaged rationalist, structuralist and culturalist accounts in a constructive dialogue. To be sure, these three kinds of perspectives not only characterize theories of political participation. They have, for example, been discussed more broadly in relation to the underpinnings of comparative politics (Lichbach and Zuckerman Reference Lichbach, Zuckerman, Lichbach and Zuckerman1997). However, our point is that research on political participation has often followed one or the other of these perspectives, largely neglecting their possible combination.

Rationalist accounts emphasize the fact that actors’ decision to participate is self-interested and based on a cost/benefit calculus. In the most extreme version of the theory, individuals are understood as completely free from external constraints in taking decisions. In its more developed, moderate and plausible version, decision-making is influenced by external factors, and we can only speak of a ‘bounded rationality’ (Simon Reference Simon1957). This perspective is most common in studies of voting turnout and behaviour (Franklin Reference Franklin2004, Reference Franklin, Giugni and Grasso2022), although such approaches are also applied to other types of political participation (Whiteley Reference Whiteley, Giugni and Grasso2022) and inform work on social movements and protests (Oberschall Reference Oberschall1993; Oliver Reference Oliver1984; Opp Reference Opp1989). Rationalist accounts may also include work that is outside the rational choice tradition. Indeed, few today would maintain that actors are irrational. The difference is on the centrality given to this kind of motivation and the role played by self-interest and the maximization of individual utility in explanations of political participation.

Structuralist accounts are popular especially among students of social movements, less so in research on voting. In these accounts, political participation is strongly influenced, if not determined, by certain structural aspects of the broader environment, usually of the political-institutional context. When it comes to explaining participation in movements and protest activities, they most often refer to the constraining or channelling role played by political opportunity structures (Giugni Reference Giugni2009; Kriesi Reference Kriesi, now, Soule and Kriesi2004; Meyer and Minkoff Reference Meyer and Minkoff2004) and more generally by the political context of social movements (McAdam and Tarrow Reference McAdam, Tarrow, Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019). Explanations stressing the role of the economic context might also be included among this kind of account (Della Porta Reference Della Porta2015; Grasso and Giugni Reference Grasso and Giugni2016; Kurer et al. Reference Kurer, Häusermann, Wüest and Enggist2019), with economic constraints sometimes conceptualized as a kind of opportunity structure (Kousis and Tilly Reference Kousis and Tilly2005).

If structuralist accounts stress structure, culturalist accounts emphasize the role of culture. Here we may include works in the political science tradition that look at the impact of values (Hitlin and Allyn Piliavin Reference Hitlin and Allyn Piliavin2004; van Deth et al. Reference van Deth, Jan and Scarbrough1998), but especially research in the field of social movement studies. Explanations of participation in social movements that stress cultural factors abound. This includes work by sociologists (Berstein Reference Berstein2005; Flesher Fominaya Reference Flesher Fominaya, Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Jasper and Polletta2001; Johnston and Noakes Reference Johnston and Noakes2005; Jasper and Polletta Reference Jasper, Polletta, Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019; Polletta and Jasper Reference Polletta and Jasper2001) and social psychologists (Klandermans Reference Klandermans1997; van Zomeren and Iyer Reference van Zomeren and Iyer2009) that places the role of collective identity at centre stage. Others adopt framing theory with its emphasis on discourses and collective action frames (Benford and Snow Reference Benford and Snow2000; Snow Reference Snow, In Snow, Soule and Kriesi2004; Snow et al. Reference Snow, Vliegenthart, Ketelaars, In Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019). New social movements theory may also be included in this group of works, for its emphasis on identity and insofar as it looks at how new movements – such as, for example, environmental, peace, squatters’ and women’s movements – are culturally distinct from older ones in many respects (Buechler Reference Buechler1995; Pichardo Reference Pichardo1997).

These three types of explanation have largely followed separate tracks, leading students of political participation to prioritize certain explanatory factors at the expense of others. However, there are some attempts to combine them, such as in the social movement and protest politics literature. For example, Bert Klandermans (Reference Klandermans1997) starts from a social-psychological perspective that places different kinds of grievances, collective identities and emotions – such as group anger – centre stage. However, he also takes into account the social-structural embeddedness of movement participants and the broader structural context of participation in social movements, while acknowledging that participation may be based on instrumental motivations, in addition to identity-based and ideological ones. Karl-Dieter Opp (Reference Opp2009) has developed a structural-cognitive model from a rational choice perspective that emphasizes the role of cognitive processes and selective incentives for participation, but which also considers structural factors such as political opportunity structures and cultural ones such as identity (conceptualized as a selective incentive) and framing processes.

More recently, Marc Hooghe (Reference Hooghe, Giugni and Grasso2022) has suggested two combinations of theoretical perspectives, which he sees as most promising for the study of political participation. The first combines rationalist and structuralist perspectives by acknowledging that the individual actors’ self-interest is largely shaped by their structural position within society. More generally, structural conditions – economic, social, political – set the boundaries for the actors’ assessment of self-interest and therefore help explain why they take the (rational) decision to participate. The second combination of theoretical perspectives suggested by Hooghe (Reference Hooghe, Giugni and Grasso2022) relates to structural and cultural accounts. He states that structural conditions are not sufficient to explain participation, but it is equally important to take into account the potentially liberating norms that are present in the political system itself. Although he warns us against the temptation to combine all kinds of theoretical approaches into a general framework, Hooghe (Reference Hooghe, Giugni and Grasso2022) has sketched an integrated framework that brings together various perspectives of political participation, starting from the assumption that the different dimensions of political approaches to participation are inherently linked to one another. This framework integrates the motivation to participate at the individual level, ideology and the alignment with social cultural frames, and political opportunity structures.

Based on the above, to bridge the rationalist, structuralist and culturalist accounts, we make the following propositions, echoing those of Hooghe (Reference Hooghe, Giugni and Grasso2022):

  • Proposition 1: Structural conditions set the boundaries for the actors’ assessment of self-interest and therefore for their decision to participate.

  • Proposition 2: The presence of structural conditions conducive to political participation must be accompanied by cultural norms that are present in the political system itself.

Cognition and affect

Common wisdom holds that heart and mind are two distinct and separate spheres of life. The first is the realm of reason and cognition, the latter that of emotions and affect. This oversimplistic but widespread view has informed much work on political participation and social science more generally. The result is a division of labour between those examining the cognitive side of individual predispositions – that is, political attitudes such as political interest, trust and efficacy – and those stressing accounts emphasizing their affective side – most notably, identity and emotions, but to some extent also personality traits and motivations. This division, however, is largely artificial, if not misleading. It can be seen, for example, in the concept of values, which has cognitive and affective dimensions (Marini Reference Marini, Borgatta and Montgomery2000), or in that of collective identity, which can be seen as resulting from people’s cognitive, moral and emotional connections with a broader community (Polletta and Jasper Reference Polletta and Jasper2001).

The distinction between cognition and affect partly overlaps that between rationality, structure and culture, or perhaps better, it is informed by it. Indeed, rationalist accounts stress the cognitive side of human behaviour, while culturalist accounts often – but not always – emphasize the role of emotions and affect. Nonetheless, the study of political participation – and perhaps even more so that of protest participation – has tended to follow two distinct and separate tracks: one stressing ‘rational’ factors such as resources and attitudinal predispositions as a basis for the decision to participate and another focusing on such aspects as identity and emotions, but also framings, memory and narratives.

We believe that this division of labour provides invaluable insights on the reasons why people participate or do not participate in politics, but in the long term might be harmful and prevent us from reaching a more comprehensive understanding of the wide range of factors leading to political participation. In particular, we suggest that cognitive and affective elements combine in various ways to lead people to become politically active. Once again, the literature already offers several insights as to how these two types of factors might combine.

While research on political participation has traditionally stressed (cognitive) political attitudes such as political interest, knowledge, sophistication, efficacy and the like, scholars have paid increasing attention to the role of emotions in politics. Emotions, affects, feelings, sentiments, moods, passions and so forth have made their breakthrough into the study of voting behaviour as well as social movements and protest politics. Political psychologists have investigated the role of emotions to explain political choice and how they may influence political attitudes and behaviours (Brader Reference Brader2005, Reference Brader2006; Marcus Reference Marcus2000; Marcus and MacKuen Reference Marcus and MacKuen1993; Marcus et al. Reference Marcus, Russell Neuman and MacKuen2000; Neuman et al. Reference Neuman, Marcus, Crigler and MacKuen2007; Valentino et al. Reference Valentino, Brader, Groenendyk, Gregorowicz and Hutchings2011, Reference Valentino, Hutchings, Banks and Davis2008; Weber Reference Weber2013). They have thus long acknowledged that the connections between cognition and affect have important implications for political evaluation, decision and action (Redlawsk Reference Redlawsk2006; Way and Masters Reference Way and Masters1996). For example, scholars have examined the relationship between emotions and political efficacy (Rudolph et al. Reference Rudolph, Gangl and Stevens2000; Valentino et al. Reference Valentino, Gregorowicz and Groenendyk2009), the role of anxiety as well as its distinct effect from anger (Best and Krueger Reference Best and Krueger2011; Huddy et al. Reference Huddy, Feldman, Casses, Neuman, Marcus, Crigler and MacKuen2007) or the impact of affect on political sophistication (Marcus and MacKuen Reference Marcus and MacKuen1993; Marcus et al. Reference Marcus, Russell Neuman and MacKuen2000; Miller Reference Miller2011).

But emotions have also entered the study of social movements and protest politics, after years of domination by (rationalist and structuralist) resource mobilization and political opportunity theories. Social psychologists were at the forefront in this regard, examining different kinds of emotions. A much-studied emotion in this respect, is anger, which is seen as particularly conducive to protest participation (Klandermans et al. Reference Klandermans, van der Toorn and van Stekelenburg2008; Leach et al. Reference Leach, Iyer and Pedersen2006, Reference Leach, Iyer and Pedersen2007; Mummendey et al. Reference Mummendey, Kessler, Klink and Mielke1999; van Stekelenburg and Klandermans Reference van Stekelenburg, Klandermans, Klandermans and Roggeband2007). Importantly, social psychologists have looked at how emotions combine with attitudes to explain protest participation or its withdrawal (van Stekelenburg and Klandermans Reference van Stekelenburg and Klandermans2013). In this vein, Martijn van Zomeren et al. (Reference van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer and Leach2004, Reference van Zomeren, Leach and Spears2012) have proposed a ‘dynamic dual pathway model’. This model understands collective action as the outcome of two distinct processes: an emotion-focused approach revolving around the experience of group-based anger, on the one hand, and a problem-focused approach revolving around beliefs in the group’s efficacy, on the other.

In addition to social-psychologists, sociologists and political scientists are also paying increasing attention to the role of emotions in social movements and protest politics (Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Jasper and Polletta2000, Reference Goodwin, Jasper and Polletta2001; Goodwin and Jasper Reference Goodwin and Jasper2004; Jasper Reference Jasper2011; Van Ness and Summers‐Effler Reference van Ness, Summers‐Effler, In Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019; see Jasper and Zhelnina Reference Jasper, Zhelnina, Giugni and Grasso2022 for a discussion in relation to political participation). Such attention is part of a ‘cultural turn’ in the study of social movements. Along with new social movements theory and framing theory, where the cognitive dimension of culture has largely overshadowed its affective dimension, these efforts have developed in directions different from the ‘structuralist bias’ of political opportunity theory (Goodwin and Jasper Reference Goodwin and Jasper2004). Early accounts of crowd behaviour in the late 19th and early 20th centuries stressed the role of panic. Later on, breakdown theories of collective behaviour of the 1950s and 1960s viewed frustration as central to explaining collective behaviour. Collective behaviour was seen as deviant if not irrational. On the other hand, this new work on emotions in social movements – emerging in part as a reaction to what was considered at the time as an excessive focus on resources and political opportunity structures – shared, among other aspects, the view that emotions are not to be understood as separate from rationality. This understanding has meant steps can be taken towards bridging cognition and affect. In other words, people can act rationally while showing strong emotions and, conversely, they can act emotionally while being fully rational. Moreover, while emotions were understood primarily in their negative declination in the past, they were now seen as both negative (e.g. anger, fear) and positive (e.g. enthusiasm, joy) emotions. A restrictive view of emotions as short-term outbursts of frustration and other negative feelings was replaced by a broader perspective comprising both short-term, reflexive emotions (e.g. disgust, surprise) and long-term moods and affective predispositions (e.g. frustration, love).

As we can see from this brief review of works by social-psychologists, sociologists and political scientists, while often following two separate tracks, they have also combined cognitive and affective explanations of political participation. In our own work on participation in demonstrations, we also found that both political attitudes and emotions concur to explain protest participation. This occurs through the link between political interest and anger, hence suggesting that cognition and affect both contribute to commitment to engage in protest activities (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni and Grasso2019). To take two examples, participants in protests staged by movements such as Black Lives Matter in the US or various anti-austerity movements across Europe were arguably driven by a desire for policy change, but also by feelings of righteous anger as well as indignation. These two aspects interact in complex ways. In a recent effort at reviewing works that bridge cognitive-based and affective-based explanations of political participation, Alessandro Nai (Reference Nai, Giugni and Grasso2022), echoing a broader model for the drivers of mobilization based on both psychological and communicational mechanisms, concludes that emotions and cognition interact in two fundamental ways. On the one hand, this occurs through a direct and joint effect, in particular the fact that emotions have a stronger role in participation at higher levels of cognitive attitudes. On the other hand, they further play a mediating role by fuelling the effects of political information on participation.

Based on the above, we make the following propositions with regard to bridging cognitive-based and affective-based explanations of political participation, echoing those made by Nai (Reference Nai, Giugni and Grasso2022):

  • Proposition 3: Beyond a simple additive effect, cognitive drivers (attitudes) and affective drivers (emotions) of political participation interact, so that cognitive drivers moderate the effect of affective drivers and vice versa.

  • Proposition 4: Beyond a simple additive effect, cognitive drivers (attitudes) mediate the effect of affective drivers (emotions) on political participation and vice versa.

The macro/micro gap

Moving from ontological underpinnings and theoretical perspectives to methodological approaches, explanations of political participation have traditionally followed two distinct paths when it comes to the level of observation or analysis. Analyses of political participation tend to focus on the individual (micro) level: voting, protesting or some other form of participation such as consumerism or digital forms (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni, Grasso, Giugni and Grasso2022). Scholars have also examined participation at the aggregate (macro) level, such as in studies of electoral volatility and electoral geography (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni, Grasso, Giugni and Grasso2022). This does not take into account the literature on social movements and collective action, which by definition are phenomena located at the meso or macro level and therefore require explanations at that level of analysis.

Especially in recent years, scholars have paid increasing attention to bridging the macro/micro link (Anduiza Reference Anduiza2002; Dalton et al. Reference Dalton, Van Sickle and Weldon2010; Grasso and Giugni Reference Grasso and Giugni2016; Kern et al. Reference Kern, Marien and Hooghe2015; Vráblíková Reference Vráblíková2013) in the study of political participation, including in protest activities, also helped by the development of (quantitative) multilevel analysis techniques. Multilevel models share two important methodological features which, in turn, offer remarkable advantages for theorizing about political participation. First, they combine a micro model with a macro model, hence allowing for modelling contextual heterogeneity – for example, variation across countries – in a more powerful and accurate way. Second, they allow for the inclusion of contextual characteristics in the models.

But multilevel models have another feature that is perhaps even more important for pushing the study of political participation further, namely the potential to model cross-level interactions. While research on political participation has mainly focused on the individual level, perhaps the most interesting and relevant hypotheses that we can think of often involve an interaction between individual and contextual factors. For example, we show (Grasso and Giugni Reference Grasso and Giugni2016) that the impact of relative deprivation on protest activism is conditional on context – specifically, unemployment – arguing this means individuals are more likely to understand their individualized experience in politicized terms.

The macro-level, contextual factors that may influence the relationship between individual characteristics and political participation are of different sorts. Students of social movements have long acknowledged the role of political opportunity structures for the emergence, mobilization and outcomes of social movements (Kriesi Reference Kriesi, now, Soule and Kriesi2004). They have stressed a variety of dimensions of opportunity, such as the multiplicity of independent centres of power within the regime, the regime’s openness to new actors, the instability of current political alignments, the availability of influential allies or supporters for challengers, the extent to which the regime represses or facilitates collective claim-making, as well as decisive changes in these aspects (McAdam Reference McAdam, In McAdam, McCarthy and Zald1996; Tilly and Tarrow [Reference Tilly and Tarrow2015 [2006]]). More recently, however, scholars have expanded the concept to include discursive or cultural opportunity structures (Koopmans and Olzak Reference Koopmans and Olzak2004) or economic opportunity structures (Kousis and Tilly Reference Kousis and Tilly2005). These different sorts of opportunity structures – or just opportunities – refer to the impact of the political-institutional, cultural-discursive and economic context on the options people have to mount social movements and collective action.

Although these contextual factors were developed by social movement scholars to account for collective action, they have been shown also to have a direct effect on individual-level political participation, especially participation in protest activities. A variety of contextual factors have been examined in the literature, from unemployment to social spending, GDP, Gini index, certain features of the political system, and so forth (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni and Grasso2019; Vráblíková Reference Vráblíková2013).

Based on the above, we make the following propositions with regard to bridging the macro/micro gap:

  • Proposition 5: The effect of micro-level, individual predispositions and attitudes on political participation is moderated by macro-level, contextual factors such as macroeconomic conditions, institutional arrangements and prevailing public discourses.

  • Proposition 6: Micro-level, individual predispositions and attitudes mediate the effect of macro-level, contextual factors on political participation.

The qualitative/quantitative divide

Among the most persistent debates in the social sciences, from a methodological point of view, is the one between advocates of qualitative and defenders of quantitative methods. Sometimes this has led to a clash between qualitative researchers and their quantitative counterparts, not always relying on concrete arguments in support of one approach over the other.

The adoption of one or the other of these methodological approaches has its roots in the different epistemological and ontological premises on which the methodologies rely (Howe Reference Howe1992). Quantitative methods are usually associated with a Durkheimian positivist ontology, while qualitative methods are seen as reflecting a Weberian interpretivist ontology. This has led students of political participation – but also scholars in other fields – to favour one or the other of these two methodological approaches.

In spite of attempts at reconciliation (see, for example, King et al. Reference KIng, Keohane and Verba1994; Mann Reference Mann1981) as well as the development of mixed methods approaches, the qualitative/quantitative divide sometimes continues to hamper research on political participation, or at least to erect barriers to a deeper and more comprehensive understanding of the reasons why people become politically active or refrain from doing so. To be sure, just as it is perfectly fine to study political participation only at the micro level, there is nothing wrong in adopting qualitative or quantitative methods. Our point is that by combining them the analysis would find itself enriched, able to answer different research questions, and reconcile internal and external validity.

As with the macro/micro link discussed earlier, here important steps have been made to bridge what Raymond Boudon (Reference Boudon1969) called a ‘false dispute’. While the different approaches are seen as belonging to different cultures with contrasting norms, practices and toolkits, there are calls to encourage exchange between these two cultures (Goertz and Mahoney Reference Goertz and Mahoney2013). In this vein, an increasing number of studies today adopt some kind of combination of qualitative and quantitative data and methods. Indeed, combining qualitative and quantitative methods today is usually referred to as the mixed methods approach and is becoming increasingly popular among social scientists.

Mixed methods research can serve different purposes (Nunes and Henn Reference Nunes, Henn, In Giugni and Grasso2022). Jennifer Greene et al. (Reference Greene, Caracelli and Graham1989) distinguish between five main purposes: triangulation, complementarity, development, initiation and expansion. John Creswell (Reference Creswell2013) suggests there are three main strategies of inquiry for mixed methods research, each implying a different way of bridging qualitative and quantitative methods of data collection and analysis: concurrent mixed methods, transformative mixed methods and sequential mixed methods. In the first, ‘the researcher converges or merges quantitative and qualitative data in order to provide comprehensive analysis of the research problem’ (Nunes and Henn Reference Nunes, Henn, In Giugni and Grasso2022: 353). The second ‘involve[s] the researcher using a theoretical lens as an overarching perspective within a design that contains both quantitative and qualitative data’ (Nunes and Henn Reference Nunes, Henn, In Giugni and Grasso2022: 354). In the third, ‘the researcher seeks to elaborate on, or expand beyond, the findings of one method with another’ (Nunes and Henn Reference Nunes, Henn, In Giugni and Grasso2022: 354). In this last approach, the sequence as well as the importance given to one or the other type of method can vary.

Based on the above, we make the following propositions with regard to bridging the qualitative/quantitative divide, which reflect the five purposes of mixed methods research outlined by Greene et al. (Reference Greene, Caracelli and Graham1989):

  • Proposition 7: Qualitative and quantitative methods can be used to address the same aspects or dimensions of political participation (in different sequences and with a different emphasis on one or other methodology) to corroborate with one method the findings obtained with another (triangulation), to increase validity of measurements and research findings (development) or to reach a more complete understanding of that aspect or dimension (complementarity).

  • Proposition 8: Qualitative and quantitative methods can be used to address different aspects or dimensions of political participation (in different sequences and with a different emphasis on one or other methodology) to generate new understandings of it (initiation) or to extend the breadth and range of inquiry by using different methods for different inquiry components (expansion).

Ballots, barricades and beyond

As we mentioned in the introduction to this article, the literature on political participation has traditionally followed two distinct paths with little dialogue between them. This has led scholars to focus on either institutional modes of participation – mostly voting – or non-institutional ones, often protest behaviour. Underlying this division of labour is the assumption that voting and protest – or ballots and barricades, to use Ronald Aminzade’s (Reference Aminzade1993) as well as Doug McAdam and Sidney Tarrow’s (Reference McAdam and Tarrow2010) apt metaphor – are part of a zero-sum game: those who vote do not participate in protests and vice versa. However, empirical studies have shown that protesters often do go to the polls, and that institutional and non-institutional participation are not mutually exclusive (Heaney and Rojas Reference Heaney and Rojas2015; Hutter et al. Reference Hutter, Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019). This applies not only in democratic contexts but also in autocracies. Guillermo Trejo (Reference Trejo2014), for example, shows that the introduction of multiparty elections in a wide variety of autocracies around the world gave rise to major cycles of protest and that the relationship between the ballot and the street is a crucial factor for understanding the dynamics of stability and change in authoritarian regimes. More generally, it is important to acknowledge the iterative interactions between protest and policy (Meyer Reference Meyer, Meyer, Jenness and Ingram2005) and that activism and electoral politics are intertwined (Fisher Reference Fisher2012).

McAdam and Tarrow (Reference McAdam and Tarrow2010) have asked why two cognate literatures – social movements and electoral studies – have travelled along parallel paths with little conversation between them. They also propose several ways in which they can be linked, relying on the book Dynamics of Contention (McAdam et al. Reference McAdam, Tarrow and Tilly2001) and a focus on mechanisms and processes. More precisely, McAdam and Tarrow (Reference McAdam and Tarrow2010) specify six mechanisms and processes that link movement actors to routine political actors in electoral campaigns (although they only illustrate three of them): ‘[m]ovements introduce new forms of collective action that influence election campaigns; [m]ovements join electoral coalitions or, in extreme cases, turn into parties themselves; [m]ovements engage in proactive electoral mobilization; [m]ovements engage in reactive electoral mobilization; [m]ovements polarize political parties internally; [s]hifts in electoral regimes have a long-term impact on mobilization and demobilization’ (McAdam and Tarrow Reference McAdam and Tarrow2010: 533).

McAdam and Tarrow’s (Reference McAdam and Tarrow2010) call for a rapprochement of social movements and electoral studies has been answered. In fact, well before that call, proponents of the political process approach to the study of social movements had long acknowledged that electoral and non-electoral politics are intimately linked. The very concept of political opportunity structures and how they influence the rise and mobilization of social movements is all about the linkages between institutional and protest behaviour. Some of the most well-known and now ‘classic’ examples of this include Peter Eisinger’s (Reference Eisinger1973) study of the impact of local political opportunity structures on the incidence of protest activities in American cities in the late 1960s, Craig Jenkins and Charles Perrow’s (Reference Jenkins and Perrow1977) research on farmworkers’ movements in the United States, McAdam’s (1999 [Reference McAdam1999 [1982]]) work on the civil rights movements, Herbert Kitschelt’s (Reference Kitschelt1986) comparative analysis of the strategies and impacts of social movements, Tarrow’s (Reference Tarrow1989) study of the 1965–1975 protest cycle in Italy, and Hanspeter Kriesi et al.’s (Reference Kriesi, Koopmans, Duyvendak and Giugni1995) comparative study of new social movements in western Europe. The list is long and includes the vast body of literature looking at the institutional channelling of social movements or with one or other of the various components of political opportunities. In a similar fashion, historians and historical sociologists often point to the intimate relationship between elections and protest. The most obvious reference is the seminal work by Charles Tilly (Reference Tilly1986, Reference Tilly1995) on France and Britain, where the author shows how changes in the repertoires of contention relate to the emergence of new interests, opportunities and organizations due to the large-scale processes of the rise of capitalism and the formation of national states. While capitalism – and the related processes of urbanization and industrialization – refers above all to social and economic aspects, state formation clearly points to the fact that a transformation of political institutions may lead to new ways ordinary people have to make claims (Aminzade Reference Aminzade1993).

As hinted above with the work of Kitschelt (Reference Kitschelt1986), the literature on the political outcomes of social movements stresses the linkages between protest and institutions, albeit in the opposite direction (see Amenta et al. Reference Amenta, Andrews, Caren, Snow, Soule, Kriesi and McCammon2019 for a discussion). These works often stress the importance of certain features of the institutionalized political system and the role of political alliances as mediating the impact of protest activities on policy, as in the political mediation and joint-effect models of social movement outcomes (Amenta et al. Reference Amenta, Carruthers and Zylan1992; Giugni Reference Giugni2004).

Various recent works have examined the linkages between institutional and non-institutional politics, in particular between voting and protest. In this vein, research has, for example, shown the presence of a strong correlation between party membership and social movement activism (Giugni and Grasso Reference Giugni and Grasso2021). Scholars have made a strong case that electoral and non-electoral political behaviour need to be examined side by side (McAdam and Tarrow Reference McAdam and Tarrow2010). More recently, Endre Borbáth and Swen Hutter (Reference Borbáth, Hutter, Giugni and Grasso2022) have made an explicit attempt to assess the relationship between electoral and non-electoral political participation (more specifically, between voting and lawful public demonstrations) at the level of individual participants. They argue that individual-level predictors cannot sufficiently explain the variation in the extent to which citizens combine voting and demonstrating, and that to understand this, we need to look at structural conditions and opportunities for participation. Most importantly for our purpose, they illustrate the blurring boundaries between electoral and non-electoral mobilization and its repercussions on individual-level behaviour, showing the extent to which individuals combine voting and attending demonstrations in their action repertoire.

Based on the above, we make the following propositions with regard to bridging institutional and non-institutional forms of participation:

  • Proposition 9: Institutional participation is positively correlated with non-institutional participation.

  • Proposition 10: Partisanship is positively correlated with protest participation.

Conclusion

In this article we pointed to a number of ways in which the study of political participation – intended as individual-level participation – has followed separate tracks, hence preventing it from deploying its full potential, we believe. We have identified five such key examples: the separation of rationalist, structuralist and culturalist ontologies; the distinction between cognitive and affective explanations; the macro/micro theoretical link; the qualitative/quantitative methodological divide; and, more substantively, the opposition of institutional (or electoral) and non-institutional (or protest) forms of participation. The first aspect derives from two even more fundamental oppositions social scientists have traditionally been struggling with: those between structure and agency, and between structure and culture.

Our goal, however, was not simply to stress the existence of these different ways to address the subject matter, but rather to encourage scholars to move beyond them. For this purpose, we suggested several theoretical, methodological and substantial ‘bridges’ that can be further developed to enrich and push further the study of political participation. To be sure, we are not claiming that past or current research does not try to combine the various approaches we have underlined, as attested by the reference to works that do exactly that. However, we believe that more can be done, and therefore we made several, admittedly not exhaustive, suggestions or propositions in this regard. One may think of them as working hypotheses to be developed and addressed in further research.

Also, we are not claiming that the five antinomies and related ‘bridges’ are the only ones that exist. One can imagine other ones. For example, in recent years scholarship has paid increasing attention to digital forms of participation (see Earl and Kenski Reference Earl, Kenski, Giugni and Grasso2022 for a discussion). Whether online forms of participation are qualitatively distinct from more traditional offline forms is an open question. Yet, the risk exists that we start treating them as two fundamentally different and, worse, mutually exclusive ways to engage in politics, reproducing a dichotomic view which is not beneficial to the study of political participation. Another important kind of ‘bridging’ is the one provided by intersectional theories of political participation (see Slaughter and Brown Reference Slaughter, Brown, Giugni and Grasso2022 for a discussion), which explicitly connect different aspects relating to inequalities which are interrelated and mutually constitutive such as between gender, race and social class.

Let us be clear, in conclusion: conducting ‘unbridged’ research is perfectly fine and legitimate. Indeed, it has yielded a lot of helpful, sometimes fundamental, insights into the processes leading people to participate (or not) in politics. Yet, we are convinced that engaging, as many have started doing, in developing theory that relates to one or other of the ‘bridges’ we have pointed out here will enrich our understanding of political participation moving forward.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all those who provided feedback on our work and allowed us to refine the arguments presented in this article.

References

Amenta, E, Andrews, KT and Caren, N (2019) The Political Institutions, Processes, and Outcomes Movements Seek to Influence. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 449465.Google Scholar
Amenta, E, Carruthers, BG and Zylan, Y (1992) A Hero for the Aged? The Townsend Movement, the Political Mediation Model, and U.S. Old-Age Policy, 1934–1950. American Journal of Sociology 98, 308339. https://doi.org/10.1086/230010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aminzade, R (1993) Ballots and Barricades: Class Formation and Republican Politics in France, 1830–1871. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Anduiza, E (2002) Individual Characteristics, Institutional Incentives and Electoral Abstention in Western Europe. European Journal of Political Research 41, 643673. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.00025.Google Scholar
Archer, MS (1995) Realist Social Theory: The Morphogenetic Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511557675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benford, RD and Snow, DA (2000) Framing Processes and Social Movements: An Overview and Assessment. Annual Review of Sociology 26, 611639. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.26.1.611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berstein, M (2005) Identity Politics. Annual Review of Sociology 31, 4774. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.29.010202.100054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, SJ and Krueger, BS (2011) Government Monitoring and Political Participation in the United States: The Distinct Roles of Anger and Anxiety. American Politics Research 39, 85117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1532673X10380848.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borbáth, E and Hutter, S (2022) Bridging Electoral and Nonelectoral Political Participation In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 451–467.Google Scholar
Boudon, R (1969) Les méthodes en sociologie. Paris: PUF.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P (1977) Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511812507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brader, T (2005) Striking a Responsive Chord: How Political Ads Motivate and Persuade Voters by Appealing to Emo. American Journal of Political Science 49, 388405. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0092-5853.2005.00130.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brader, T (2006) Campaigning for Hearts and Minds: How Emotional Appeals in Political Ads Work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brady, HE (1999) Political Participation. In Robinson, JP, Shaver, PR and Lawrence, SW (eds), Measures of Political Attitudes. Waltham, MA: Academic Press, pp. 737801.Google Scholar
Buechler, SM (1995) New Social Movement Theories. Sociological Quarterly 36, 441464.10.1111/j.1533-8525.1995.tb00447.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creswell, JW (2013) Research Design: Qualitative, Quantitative, and Mixed Methods Approaches. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Dalton, R, Van Sickle, A and Weldon, S (2010) The Individual–Institutional Nexus of Protest Behaviour. British Journal of Political Science 40, 5173. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000712340999038X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Porta, D (2015) Social Movements in Times of Austerity: Bringing Capitalism Back into Protest Analysis. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Earl, J and Kenski, K (2022) Online Political Participation. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 435449.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisinger, PK (1973) The Conditions of Protest Behavior in American Cities. American Political Science Review 67, 1128. https://doi.org/10.2307/1958525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, DR (2012) Youth Political Participation: Bridging Activism and Electoral Politics. Annual Review of Sociology 38, 119137. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-071811-145439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flesher Fominaya, C (2019) Collective Identity in Social Movements: Assessing the Limits of a Theoretical Framework. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 429446.Google Scholar
Franklin, M (2004) Voter Turnout and the Dynamics of Electoral Competition since 1945. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Franklin, M (2022) Linking Electoral and Partisan Participation. Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 363380.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giddens, A (1984) The Constitution of Society: Outline of the Theory of Structuration. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Giugni, M (2004) Social Protest and Policy Change: Ecology, Antinuclear, and Peace Movements in Comparative Perspective Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Giugni, M (2009) Political Opportunities: From Tilly to Tilly. Swiss Political Science Review 15, 361368. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1662-6370.2009.tb00136.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giugni, M and Grasso, M (2021) Party Membership and Social Movement Activism: A Macro-Micro Analysis. Party Politics 27, 92102. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354068818823446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giugni, M and Grasso, M (2022) The Study of Political Participation across Research Traditions. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 122.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giugni, M and Grasso, MT (2019) Street Citizens: Protest Politics and Social Movement Activism in the Age of Globalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108693455CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goertz, G and Mahoney, J (2013) A Tale of Two Cultures: Qualitative and Quantitative Research in the Social Sciences. Princeton: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400845446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, J and Jasper, JM (eds) (2004) Rethinking Social Movements: Structure, Meaning, and Emotion. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Goodwin, J, Jasper, JM and Polletta, F (2000) The Return of the Repressed: The Fall and Rise of Emotions in Social Movement Theory. Mobilization 5, 6583. https://doi.org/10.17813/maiq.5.1.74u39102m107g748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, J, Jasper, JM and Polletta, F (2001) Passionate Politics: Emotions and Social Movements. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226304007.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, J, Jasper, JM and Polletta, F (2004) Emotional Dimensions of Social Movements. In Snow, DA, Soule, S and Kriesi, H (eds), The Blackwell Companion to Social Movements. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 413432.Google Scholar
Grasso, M and Giugni, M (2016) Protest Participation and Economic Crisis: The Conditioning Role of Political Opportunities. European Journal of Political Research 55, 663680. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.12153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, JC, Caracelli, VJ and Graham, WF (1989) Toward a Conceptual Framework for Mixed-Method Evaluation Designs. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis 11, 255274. https://doi.org/10.3102/01623737011003255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heaney, MT and Rojas, F (2015) Party in the Street: The Antiwar Movement and the Democratic Party after 9/11. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781316084007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hitlin, S and Allyn Piliavin, J (2004) Values: Reviving a Dormant Concept. Annual Review of Sociology 30, 359393. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.30.012703.110640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooghe, M (2022) Bridging Rationalist, Structuralist, and Culturalist Approaches to the Study of Political Participation. Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 216229.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, KR (1992) Getting over the Quantitative–Qualitative Debate. American Journal of Education 100, 236256. https://doi.org/10.1086/444015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, L, Feldman, S and Casses, E (2007) On the Distinct Political Effects of Anxiety and Anger. In Neuman, WR, Marcus, GE, Crigler, AN and MacKuen, M (eds), The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 202230.10.7208/chicago/9780226574431.003.0009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutter, S et al. (2019) Social Movements in Interaction with Political Parties. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 322337.Google Scholar
Jasper, JM (2011) Emotions and Social Movements: Twenty Years of Theory and Research. Annual Review of Sociology 37, 285303. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-081309-150015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jasper, JM and Polletta, F (2019) The Cultural Context of Social Movements. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 6378.Google Scholar
Jasper, JM and Zhelnina, A (2022) Identity, Emotions, and Political Participation. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford Unversity Press, pp. 652667.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, JC and Perrow, C (1977) Insurgency of the Powerless: Farm Worker Movements (1946–1972). American Sociological Review 42, 249268. https://doi.org/10.2307/2094604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, H and Noakes, JA (eds) (2005) Frames of Protest: Social Movements and the Framing Perspective. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Kern, A, Marien, S and Hooghe, M (2015) Economic Crisis and Levels of Political Participation in Europe (2002–2010): The Role of Resources and Grievances. West European Politics 38, 465490. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2014.993152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KIng, G, Keohane, RO and Verba, S (1994) Designing Social Inquiry: Scientific Inference in Qualitative Research. Princeton: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400821211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitschelt, H (1986) Political Opportunity Structures and Political Protest: Anti-Nuclear Movements in Four Democracies. British Journal of Political Science 16, 5785. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000712340000380X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klandermans, B (1997) The Social Psychology of Protest. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Klandermans, B, van der Toorn, J and van Stekelenburg, J (2008) Embeddedness and Identity: How Immigrants Turn Grievances into Action. American Sociological Review 73, 9921012. https://doi.org/10.1177/000312240807300606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koopmans, R and Olzak, S (2004) Discursive Opportunities and the Evolution of Right-Wing Violence in Germany. American Journal of Sociology 110, 198230. https://doi.org/10.1086/386271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kousis, M and Tilly, C (eds) (2005) Economic and Political Contention in Comparative Perspective. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publishers.Google Scholar
Kriesi, H (2004) Political Context and Opportunity. In Snow, DA, Soule, S and Kriesi, H (eds), The Blackwell Companion to Social Movements. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 6790.Google Scholar
Kriesi, H, Koopmans, R, Duyvendak, JW and Giugni, M (1995) New Social Movements in Western Europe. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Kurer, T, Häusermann, S, Wüest, B and Enggist, M (2019) Economic Grievances and Political Protest. European Journal of Political Research 58, 866892. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.12318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, CW, Iyer, A and Pedersen, A (2006) Anger and Guilt about Ingroup Advantage Explain the Willingness for Political Action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32, 12321245. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206289729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leach, CW, Iyer, A and Pedersen, A (2007) Angry Opposition to Government Redress: When the Structurally Advantaged Perceive Themselves as Relatively Deprived. British Journal of Social Psychology 46, 191204. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466606X99360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichbach, MI and Zuckerman, AS (1997) Research Traditions and Theory in Comparative Politics: An Introduction. In Lichbach, MI and Zuckerman, AS (eds), Comparative Politics: Rationality, Culture, and Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 316.Google Scholar
Mann, M (1981) Socio-Logic. Sociology 15, 544550. https://doi.org/10.1177/003803858101500410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, GE (2000) Emotions in Politics. Annual Review of Political Science 3, 221250. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.polisci.3.1.221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, GE and MacKuen, MB (1993) Anxiety, Enthusiasm, and the Vote: The Emotional Underpinnings of Learning and Involvement during Presidential Campaigns. American Political Science Review 87, 672685. https://doi.org/10.2307/2938743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, GE, Russell Neuman, W and MacKuen, M (2000) Affective Intelligence and Political Judgment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Marini, MM (2000) Social Values and Norms. In Borgatta, EF and Montgomery, RJV (eds), Encyclopedia of Sociology. New York: Macmillan, pp. .Google Scholar
McAdam, D (1996) Conceptual Origins, Current Problems, Future Directions. In McAdam, D, McCarthy, JD and Zald, MN (eds), Comparative Perspectives on Social Movements: Political Opportunities, Mobilizing Structures, and Cultural Framings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 2340.10.1017/CBO9780511803987.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdam, D (1999 [1982]) Political Process and the Development of Black Insurgency, 1930–1970, 2nd edn. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226555553.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdam, D and Tarrow, S (2010) Ballots and Barricades: On the Reciprocal Relationship between Elections and Social Movements. Perspectives on Politics 8, 529542. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592710001234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdam, D and Tarrow, S (2019) The Political Context of Social Movements. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 1942.Google Scholar
McAdam, D, Tarrow, S and Tilly, C (2001) Dynamics of Contention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511805431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, DS (2005) Social Movements and Public Policy: Eggs, Chicken, and Theory. In Meyer, DS, Jenness, V and Ingram, H (eds), Routing the Opposition: Social Movements, Public Policy, and Democracy. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 126.Google Scholar
Meyer, DS and Minkoff, DC (2004) Conceptualizing Political Opportunity. Social Forces 8, 14571492. https://doi.org/10.1353/sof.2004.0082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, PR (2011) The Emotional Citizen: Emotion as a Function of Political Sophistication. Political Psychology 32, 575600. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00824.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mummendey, A, Kessler, T, Klink, A and Mielke, R (1999) Strategies to Cope with Negative Social Identity: Predictions by Social Identity Theory and Relative Deprivation Theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76, 229245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nai, A (2022) Bridging Cognitive and Affective Explanations of Political Participation. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford Unversity Press, pp. 668683.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuman, WR, Marcus, GE, Crigler, AN and MacKuen, M (eds) (2007) The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nunes, AI and Henn, M (2022) Bridging Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches in the Study of Political Participation. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 344359.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberschall, A (1993) Social Movements: Ideologies, Interests, and Identities. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Oliver, P (1984) ‘If You Don’t Do It, Nobody Else Will’: Active and Token Contributors to Local Collective Action. American Sociological Review 49, 601610. https://doi.org/10.2307/2095418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Opp, K-D in collaboration with Hartmann P and Hartmann P (1989) The Rationality of Political Protest: A Comparative Analysis of Rational Choice Theory. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Opp, K-D (2009) Theories of Political Protest and Social Movements: A Multidisciplinary Introduction, Critique and Synthesis. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203883846CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, T (1937) The Structure of Social Action: A Study in Social Theory with Special Reference to a Group of Recent European Writers. New York and London: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Parsons, T (1951) The Social System. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.Google Scholar
Pichardo, NA (1997) New Social Movements: A Critical Review. Annual Review of Sociology 23, 411430. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.23.1.411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polletta, F and Jasper, JM (2001) Collective Identity and Social Movements. Annual Review of Sociology 27, 283305. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlawsk, D (ed.) (2006) Feeling Politics: Emotion in Political Information Processing. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1057/9781403983114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudolph, TJ, Gangl, A and Stevens, D (2000) The Effects of Efficacy and Emotions on Campaign Involvement. Journal of Politics 62, 11891197. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-3816.00053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, HA (1957) Models of Man: Social and Rational. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Slaughter, CM and Brown, NE (2022) Intersectionality and Political Participation. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 725743.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, DA (2004) Framing Processes, Ideology, and Discursive Fields. In Snow, DA, Soule, S and Kriesi, H (eds), The Blackwell Companion to Social Movements. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 380412.10.1002/9780470999103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, DA, Vliegenthart, R and Ketelaars, P (2019) The Framing Perspective on Social Movements: Its Conceptual Roots and Architecture. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 392410.Google Scholar
Tarrow, S (1989) Democracy and Disorder: Protest and Politics in Italy, 1965–1975. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Theocharis, Y and van Deth, JW (2017) Political Participation in a Changing World. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203728673CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilly, C (1986) The Contentious French: Four Centuries of Popular Struggle. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.10.4159/harvard.9780674433984CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilly, C (1995) Popular Contention in Great Britain, 1758–1834. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tilly, C and Tarrow, S (2015 [2006]) Contentious Politics, 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trejo, G (2014) The Ballot and the Street: An Electoral Theory of Social Protest in Autocracies. Perspectives on Politics 12, 332352.10.1017/S1537592714000863CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, NA, Brader, T, Groenendyk, EW, Gregorowicz, K and Hutchings, VL (2011) Election Night’s Alright for Fighting: The Role of Emotions in Political Participation. Journal of Politics 73, 156170. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022381610000939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, NA, Gregorowicz, K and Groenendyk, EW (2009) Efficacy, Emotions and the Habit of Participation. Political Behavior 31, 307330. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-008-9076-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, NA, Hutchings, VL, Banks, AJ and Davis, AK (2008) Is a Worried Citizen a Good Citizen? Emotions, Political Information Seeking, and Learning via the Internet. Political Psychology 29, 247273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00625.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Deth, , Jan, W and Scarbrough, E (1998) The Impact of Values. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/0198294751.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Ness, J and Summers‐Effler, E (2019) Emotions and Social Movements. In Snow, DA, Soule, SA, Kriesi, H and McCammon, HJ (eds), The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Social Movements, 2nd edn. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell, pp. 411428.Google Scholar
van Stekelenburg, J and Klandermans, B (2007) Individuals in Movements: A Social Psychology of Contention. In Klandermans, B and Roggeband, C (eds), The Handbook of Social Movements across Disciplines. New York: Springer, pp. 157204.Google Scholar
van Stekelenburg, J and Klandermans, B (2013) The Social Psychology of Protest. Current Sociology 61, 886905. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011392113479314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Zomeren, M and Iyer, A (2009) Introduction to the Social and Psychological Dynamics of Collective Action. Journal of Social Issues 65, 645660. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01618.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Zomeren, M, Leach, CW and Spears, R (2012) Protesters as ‘Passionate Economists’: A Dynamic Dual Pathway Model of Approach Coping with Collective Disadvantage. Personality and Social Psychology Review 16, 180199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311430835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Zomeren, M, Spears, R, Fischer, AH and Leach, CW (2004) Put Your Money Where Your Mouth Is! Explaining Collective Action Tendencies through Group-Based Anger and Group Efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87, 649664. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vráblíková, K (2013) How Context Matters? Mobilization, Political Opportunity Structures, and Nonelectoral Political Participation in Old and New Democracies. Comparative Political Studies 47, 203229. https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414013488538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Way, BM and Masters, RD (1996) Political Attitudes: Interactions of Cognition and Affect. Motivation and Emotion 20, 205236. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02251887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, C (2013) Emotions, Campaigns and Political Participation. Political Research Quarterly 66, 414428. https://doi.org/10.1177/1065912912449697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiteley, PF (2022) Rational Choice Theory and Political Participation. In Giugni, M and Grasso, M (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Participation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 135151.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198861126.013.8CrossRefGoogle Scholar