Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-k7rjm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-30T03:19:18.375Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Small mammal owners’ experiences of housing challenges and animal welfare: A COM-B and word frequency analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 August 2025

Grace Carroll*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, https://ror.org/00hswnk62 Queen’s University Belfast , UK
Kerry Taylor
Affiliation:
Blue Cross, Oxfordshire, UK
Claire Stallard
Affiliation:
Blue Cross, Oxfordshire, UK
Alison Wills
Affiliation:
https://ror.org/020jfw620 Hartpury University and Hartpury College , UK
*
Corresponding author: Grace Carroll; Email: G.Carroll@qub.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Small mammals are particularly dependent on owner-provided housing and husbandry yet are frequently kept in conditions that do not meet their welfare needs. This study used the COM-B model (Capability, Opportunity, Motivation = Behaviour) to identify behavioural drivers influencing housing provision among 723 UK small mammal pet owners. This model of human behaviour proposes that behaviour occurs when individuals have the capability, opportunity, and motivation to act. Owners of the eight most commonly kept small mammal species were surveyed: rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus). Opportunity, particularly the availability of suitable enclosures, emerged as the primary barrier, while Capability and Motivation were identified as facilitators, with most pet owners willing and able to provide good levels of welfare. Owner approaches to assessing health and welfare at home were examined through qualitative word frequency analysis, with responses mapped to the Five Domains model. This analysis focused on rabbits, guinea pigs, rats, and hamsters due to limited data availability for other species. Overall, behavioural indicators were most commonly used to identify positive health and welfare, while nutritional and physical signs were cited most frequently for negative states. Changes in eating behaviour were the most frequently cited indicators of ill health or poor welfare across all four species, suggesting this may serve as a practical health and welfare indicator for owners. Improving access to suitable housing and further exploring eating behaviour as an early health and welfare indicator may together support better husbandry for small mammal pets.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Small mammal species are becoming increasingly popular as companion animals (McLaughlin & Strunk Reference McLaughlin and Strunk2016; Díaz-Berciano & Gallego-Agundez Reference Díaz-Berciano and Gallego-Agundez2024). A UK-wide pet census by Blue Cross (2024) found that 9% of respondents owned rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), and 9% owned other small pets, with guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) and hamsters being most popular, followed by rats (Rattus norvegicus), gerbils, chinchillas, degus and mice (Mus musculus), respectively. This represents hundreds of thousands of animals, including approximately 1 million rabbits, 700,000 guinea pigs, and 600,000 hamsters (UK Pet Food 2024). Despite their popularity, there is surprisingly limited information available regarding the natural behaviour, health, welfare, care and housing requirements of these species (Harrup & Rooney Reference Harrup and Rooney2020; Mee et al. Reference Mee, Tipton, Oxley and Westgarth2022; Hedley et al. Reference Hedley, Pettitt and Abeyesinghe2023; Gilhofer et al. Reference Gilhofer, Hebesberger, Waiblinger, Künzel, Rouha-Mülleder, Mariti and Windschnurer2024; Schneidewind et al. Reference Schneidewind, Lesch, Heizmann and Windschnurer2024). Small mammal pets belong to different orders; the order, Rodentia includes rodents like mice, rats, hamsters and gerbils, while rabbits belong to the order, Lagomorpha (Allaby Reference Allaby2003). Together, these orders make up the mammalian clade, Glires (Yeates & Baumans Reference Yeates, Baumans and McMillan2019). Within this clade, there are various wild and domesticated species, subspecies, and breeds, each with distinct needs (Lonstein & De Vries Reference Lonstein and De Vries2000; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Kim, Brodbelt, Church, Pegram and Baldrey2022). Some species were domesticated for use as a food source or for their fur, while others were bred for scientific research purposes, or purely for aesthetics (Mitchell Reference Mitchell2009; Yeates & Baumans Reference Yeates, Baumans and McMillan2019), further contributing to their diversity (Linderholm & Larson Reference Linderholm and Larson2013; Saré et al. Reference Saré, Lemons and Smith2021). Despite these differences, small mammal pets are often treated as a homogeneous group. For example, many small mammal enclosures are labelled for ‘rodents and small animals’ rather than a specific species (Bläske et al. Reference Bläske, Schwarzer, Ebner, Gerbig, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2022).

Small mammal housing

There is evidence to suggest that pet rabbits and small rodents are often housed in inappropriate enclosures. For example, Rooney et al. (Reference Rooney, Blackwell, Mullan, Saunders, Baker and Hill2014) surveyed 1,254 rabbit owners across South-West, North-West and Eastern England and found that 27.5% of rabbits were housed in enclosures that limit natural behaviour. Furthermore, 43.5% of rabbits, a social species, were housed singly, and a small number were housed with predator species (e.g. domestic cats [Felis catus]). Similarly, Mee et al. (Reference Mee, Tipton, Oxley and Westgarth2022) found that 31.2% of rabbits lived in inadequate housing, with half being housed alone. Harrup and Rooney (Reference Harrup and Rooney2020) also identified poor housing practices in guinea pig owners, with 21.4% of guinea pigs being housed alone, and 18.2% being housed in enclosures smaller than those recommended by the British Cavy Council (Neesam Reference Neesam2015). In addition, commercially available enclosures may not always meet optimal housing standards. For example, Bläske et al. (Reference Bläske, Schwarzer, Ebner, Gerbig, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2022) assessed the suitability of small pet products in Germany, including enclosures, bedding, and accessories. Criteria were created based on animal welfare legislation and animal welfare organisation guidelines. Between 50 to 100% of species-specific enclosures were evaluated as being unsuitable for the specific animal in question as regards their welfare (Bläske et al. Reference Bläske, Schwarzer, Ebner, Gerbig, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2022). These ongoing issues may stem, in part, from a lack of research into species-specific housing needs, making it difficult for both manufacturers and owners to make informed decisions. Much of the research on small mammal housing has been conducted for laboratory animals, where animal welfare competes with other priorities, including financial and scientific considerations (Mazhary & Hawkins Reference Mazhary and Hawkins2019; Harrup & Rooney Reference Harrup and Rooney2020; Neville et al. Reference Neville, Mounty, Benato, Hunter, Mendl and Paul2022). However, the limited evidence that does exist suggests that enclosures should be as big as is feasible; those that are too small may restrict the ability of the animal to perform natural behaviours and can increase inactivity (Dixon et al. Reference Dixon, Hardiman and Cooper2010; Hedley et al. Reference Hedley, Pettitt and Abeyesinghe2023). Furthermore, we know that many Glires are social animals and should be housed in pairs or small groups (Bläske et al. Reference Bläske, Schwarzer, Ebner, Gerbig, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2022; Gilhofer et al. Reference Gilhofer, Hebesberger, Waiblinger, Künzel, Rouha-Mülleder, Mariti and Windschnurer2024; Schneidewind et al. Reference Schneidewind, Lesch, Heizmann and Windschnurer2024), while recent evidence suggests that, although some species, such as Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus), have been traditionally housed alone, all hamster species may require solitary housing (Ross et al. Reference Ross, Norvelle, Choi, Walton, Albers and Huhman2017; Hedley et al. Reference Hedley, Pettitt and Abeyesinghe2023).

Appropriate housing is of concern to small mammal owners; in their Big Pet Census, Blue Cross (2024) found that 8% of respondents identified “ensuring pets have adequate housing” as their top welfare concern. However, this may relate to their own ability to provide housing, the availability of housing on the market, or something else. According to Section 9 of the UK Government Animal Welfare Act (2006), pet owners are responsible for meeting their animals’ day-to-day needs. This is especially important for animals kept in enclosures, where they rely entirely upon humans for food, water, and care. Furthermore, inappropriate husbandry in small mammals is often linked to poor health, meaning a lack of suitable housing has a significant impact on animal welfare (Wills Reference Wills2020). Considering this, it is important to understand the barriers that prevent owners from providing appropriate housing, and to determine whether these relate to intrinsic factors (e.g. knowledge, habits), external constraints (e.g. availability of suitable enclosures, social norms), or a mixture of both.

Understanding owner behaviour: Intrinsic and extrinsic barriers

In recent years, animal welfare science has moved away from focusing solely on the attitudes and intentions of those responsible for the care of animals, to adopting broader human behaviour change frameworks that consider a wide range of influencing factors (Carroll & Groarke Reference Carroll and Groarke2019; Cornish et al. Reference Cornish, Jamieson, Raubenheimer and McGreevy2019). For example, while pet owners may intend to provide suitable housing, habits, the social environment or the availability of resources may also determine their behaviour, factors which, although touched upon in earlier attitude models (via normative and control beliefs) are treated more explicitly and independently in newer frameworks. In order to change human behaviour to improve animal welfare, the barriers and facilitators associated with a desired outcome must be identified (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Van Stralen and West2011; Carroll et al. Reference Carroll, Groarke and Graham-Wisener2021). This can be done using the Behaviour Change Wheel (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Atkins and West2014); a framework that was originally used by Health Psychologists to change human behaviours, such as smoking and physical inactivity (e.g. Fulton et al. Reference Fulton, Brown, Kwah and Wild2016; Truelove et al. Reference Truelove, Vanderloo, Tucker, Di Sebastiano and Faulkner2020). Barriers and facilitators can be identified using the COM-B model (Capability-Opportunity-Motivation = Behaviour) which can, in turn, be mapped to viable solutions known to be successful in changing human behaviour (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Atkins and West2014). This model of human behaviour proposes that behaviour occurs when individuals have the Capability, Opportunity, and Motivation to act. Each of these components can be divided into two subtypes, resulting in six categories in total (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Atkins and West2014). Capability includes both physical capability (e.g. strength, dexterity) and psychological capability (e.g. knowledge, cognitive skills). Opportunity is divided into physical opportunity (e.g. time, resources) and social opportunity (e.g. cultural norms, social expectations). Motivation is comprised of reflective motivation (e.g. conscious planning, beliefs, intentions) and automatic motivation (e.g. habits, emotional responses, impulses). To the authors’ knowledge, this approach is yet to be used in the context of rabbit and small rodent companion animal housing.

Welfare assessment of small mammals

Similar to housing requirements, methods of assessing rabbit and small mammal welfare are under-developed. Cohen and Ho (Reference Cohen and Ho2023) conducted a systematic review of rat, mouse, guinea pig and rabbit welfare indicators and found there to be a lack of focus on direct welfare assessment methods. Similar to housing guidelines, most welfare measures are found within the grey literature (e.g. veterinary textbooks, animal welfare organisation materials), with few coming from the scientific literature. Welfare assessment measures that involve direct assessment of the animal more accurately reflect the welfare state than indirect resource-based measures alone (European Food Safety Authority [EFSA] 2012). For example, assessing enclosure size is a resource-based measure, while assessing coat quality or animal behaviour would be animal-based measures. While physiological measures are less practical to assess and often require a level of expertise and skill (Cohen & Ho Reference Cohen and Ho2023), physical and behavioural measures are more accessible to pet owners. Recently, James and Wills (Reference James and Wills2025) surveyed 1,700 guinea pig, hamster, rat, gerbil and mouse owners and found an association between owners’ perceived confidence in identifying illness, and their actual accuracy when asked to recognise clinical signs and behaviours indicative of ill health. Although this association was weak, it suggests that small mammal pet owners may have some capacity to accurately assess their animals’ welfare status. This is particularly important given that small mammals are less likely to receive veterinary care compared to species such as cats and dogs (Canis familiaris) (Fox & Neville Reference Fox and Neville2024). Furthermore, veterinarians often have limited training as regards the treatment of exotic species and have reduced confidence in their ability to provide adequate care (Grant et al. Reference Grant, Montrose and Wills2017; Wills & Holt Reference Wills and Holt2020; Espinosa García-San Román et al. Reference Espinosa García-San Román, Quesada-Canales, Arbelo Hernández, Déniz Suárez and Castro-Alonso2023). The brief time spent with each animal may also hinder thorough assessments (Robinson et al. Reference Robinson, Dean, Cobb and Brennan2014). In order to improve small mammal welfare, it is important to understand how owners assess their pets’ health and welfare in the home.

The aims of the current study were thus to: (a) assess barriers to, and facilitators of, provision of suitable housing for pet rabbits and rodents in the UK; and (b) determine key positive and negative health and welfare indicators in pet rabbits and rodents, according to their owners.

These aims allow for an assessment of both welfare inputs, such as the physical environment provided, and welfare outputs, such as the owners’ ability to recognise indicators of welfare status.

Materials and methods

Ethical considerations

Hartpury University Ethics Committee (ETHICS2021-113) approved this study on 27 July 2022.

Study design and recruitment

A cross-sectional descriptive study design was used, with quantitative and qualitative elements. An online survey was disseminated by Blue Cross through paid Facebook advertising. UK-based owners of the eight most commonly kept species were targeted: rabbits, guinea pigs, hamsters, gerbils, rats, mice, chinchillas and degus. Links to the survey were also shared via relevant Facebook groups. The survey was hosted on Qualtrics and was live between the 23 November 2022 and 6 January 2023.

The survey

A detailed survey was distributed to collect information regarding owner and pet demographics, housing, enrichment, diet, bedding, animal welfare, and barriers to appropriate housing. The survey was designed to enable reporting the key barriers to provision of suitable housing, and participants’ qualitative assessments of their pets’ welfare and to be completed for one animal only. Participants were instructed to select one species, and if they owned multiple animals of the same species, they were to choose the individual whose name appeared first in the alphabet. Participants were directed automatically to the branch of questions relevant to their selected species and could choose to leave certain questions blank should they so desire.

Barriers to provision of suitable housing

Participants were required to rate 51 statements on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 2 = agree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = disagree, 5 = strongly disagree). These statements were based on a review of the literature and the expert knowledge of the authors and were framed by the COM-B and Theoretical Domains Frameworks (Cane et al. Reference Cane, O’Connor and Michie2012; Michie et al. Reference Michie, Atkins and West2014). The COM-B model is used to gain an understanding of behaviour in-context. For a given behaviour to occur, there must be the ‘Capability’ to do it, the ‘Opportunity’ for it to occur, and the ‘Motivation’ to perform the behaviour (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Atkins and West2014). The Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) is made up of 14 domains that help explain what influences behaviour; ‘Knowledge’, ‘Skills’, ‘Memory, attention and decision processes’, ‘Behavioural regulation’, ‘Social/professional role and identity’, ‘Beliefs about capabilities’, ‘Optimism’, ‘Beliefs about consequences’, ‘Intentions’, ‘Goals’, ‘Reinforcement’, ‘Emotion’, ‘Environmental context and resources’, and ‘Social behaviour’. The TDF sits under the COM-B model (Cowdell & Dyson Reference Cowdell and Dyson2019). For example, ‘Knowledge’ and ‘Skills’ sit under ‘Capability’ and ‘Belief about consequences’ sits under ‘Motivation’. Questions were posed under each of the 14 domains. While each item was mapped to a single COM-B domain for clarity, we acknowledge that some constructs, particularly belief-based statements, may align with more than one domain. For example, normative beliefs shaped by social influence were classified under ‘Social opportunity’, though they also reflect aspects of ‘Motivation’ (Whittal et al. Reference Whittal, Atkins and Herber2021).

Descriptive statistical analyses were carried out in SPSS, version 29.

Qualitative health and welfare assessments

Participants were asked to write down up to three signs that they thought indicated their pet’s positive or negative health and welfare status (“I know when my [species] is happy/healthy when…” and “I know when my [species] is unhappy/ill when…”).

Word frequency analysis

Word frequency analysis was utilised to determine the most common words used to describe signs of positive and negative health and welfare. Word frequency analysis allows patterns to be easily identified and can decrease bias in interpretation of the data (Onwuegbuzie & Leech Reference Onwuegbuzie and Leech2007; Feng & Behar-Horenstein Reference Feng and Behar-Horenstein2019). Words were required to contain three or more letters, and were initially grouped with stemmed words, for example, ‘hide’, ‘hiding’ and ‘hides’). Stop-words like ‘a’, ‘for’, and ‘have’ were excluded (Baradad & Mugabushaka Reference Baradad and Mugabushaka2015) as they do not contribute meaning to the descriptions given by pet owners. The 25 most frequently used words were then assessed manually, with synonyms being grouped together (e.g. ‘lethargic’, ‘tired’). From this, the ten most frequently used words, and associated synonyms, were identified for each species.

Word frequency analysis was carried out using nVivo, version 12. For each species, sample size permitting, the ten most frequently used positive (happiness/good health) and negative (unhappiness/ill health) words were mapped to the Five Domains model of animal welfare, according to the expert opinions of two of the authors (GC and AW). The Five Domains include ‘Nutrition’, ‘Environment’, ‘Health’, ‘Behaviour’ and ‘Mental state’ (Mellor Reference Mellor2017). As ‘Environment’ is input-based and given the nature of the question directed to participants, this domain was not coded.

Results and Discussion

Participants

In total, n = 723 participants completed the survey; 238 responses were available for rabbits, 191 for guinea pigs, 163 for hamsters, 79 for rats, 22 for gerbils, 15 for mice, nine for degus and six for chinchillas.

Barriers to, and facilitators of, providing appropriate housing

While we did not conduct statistical comparisons, Opportunity emerged as the most commonly cited barrier across all species, while Motivation and Capability were typically facilitators. The level of agreement with statements related to barriers to provision of suitable housing related to Capability is shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Self-reported Capability of owners (n = 723) of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus)to provide appropriate housing for small mammals

* 1 = strongly agree, 2 = agree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = disagree, 5 = strongly disagree

Most owners reported high levels of psychological and physical capability, with many stating they understood their species’ needs and housing requirements. For example, 93.1% agreed they had researched housing prior to acquiring their pet, and 99.1% reported knowing their species’ social housing needs. Owners who are well-informed about species-specific needs may be better equipped to provide suitable housing. For example, McMahon and Wigham (Reference McMahon and Wigham2020) found that owners who had higher acknowledgement of rabbit sentience were more likely to provide suitable housing and a variety of environmental enrichment types. However, it is worth noting that in the current study, we used pet owner self-reported capability, which may not reflect actual capability. As part of a larger study, we collected enclosure images and data on social housing and enrichment use. This will allow for a direct comparison between owners’ reported capability and their actual husbandry practices (Wills et al. in prep).

This study identified Opportunity as the most significant barrier to the provision of suitable housing for pet rabbits and rodents. The level of agreement with statements relating to Opportunity is shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Self-reported Opportunity of owners (n = 723) of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus) to provide appropriate housing for small mammals

* 1 = strongly agree, 2 = agree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = disagree, 5 = strongly disagree

Despite high levels of self-reported capability, many owners indicated a lack of access to appropriate housing products, particularly in pet shops. In the current study, 84.3% of respondents either disagreed or strongly disagreed that suitable housing was available at pet shops. This is consistent with earlier research indicating that enclosures for rabbits and other small mammals are frequently undersized or otherwise fail to meet welfare requirements (Harrup & Rooney Reference Harrup and Rooney2020; Bläske et al. Reference Bläske, Schwarzer, Ebner, Gerbig, Reese, Erhard and Wöhr2022; Mee et al. Reference Mee, Tipton, Oxley and Westgarth2022). These findings suggest that the current market does not always support optimal animal welfare, likely due in part to limited evidence on species-specific housing needs and a lack of consistent guidelines. In the absence of consistent guidance or regulation, owners may be left to choose from a restricted range of housing options, some of which may not fully meet the animals’ behavioural and physical requirements. Furthermore, over one-third of participants reported insufficient space at home for provision of larger housing, reinforcing the role of environmental constraints in determining pet owner behaviour. Responses to this item were more mixed than others, possibly reflecting genuine variation in household space or the general perception of enclosure adequacy. Future analyses comparing perceived constraints with actual enclosure dimensions (collected as part of this wider study) may clarify whether those citing space limitations are already using larger enclosures or face genuine physical restrictions. Time was less frequently cited as a barrier; most owners disagreed that time limitations prevented them from selecting suitable housing. Only 1.3% of respondents agreed that they lacked the time to work out which housing was suitable, while 94% disagreed, suggesting that time was not a major limiting factor in owner decision-making. Social opportunity was also limited. Although 66.8% of participants agreed they had support in selecting housing, fewer than half felt able to ask professionals for advice, and fewer still viewed family and friends as reliable sources of advice. Notably, many respondents felt their pets were perceived by others as ‘starter pets’ or suitable only for children, attitudes which may reduce the perceived importance of providing optimal care (Rioja-Lang et al. Reference Rioja‐Lang, Bacon, Connor and Dwyer2019). Indeed, Skovlund et al. (Reference Skovlund, Forkman, Lund, Mistry, Nielsen and Sandøe2023) found that owners who viewed rabbits as starter pets were less likely to meet their basic welfare needs and housed their rabbits in more restricted enclosures.

Similar to Capability, Motivation of the surveyed pet owners was high and the level of agreement with statements relating to Motivation can be seen in Table 3.

Table 3. Self-reported Motivation of owners (n = 723) of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus) to provide appropriate housing for small mammals

* 1 strongly agree, 2 = agree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = disagree, 5 = strongly disagree

Most participants expressed a strong sense of responsibility for their pet’s welfare. For example, almost all agreed that they see themselves as someone who cares about animal welfare and agreed that it is their responsibility to monitor and adjust their pet’s welfare. This suggests that interventions aiming to enhance Opportunity are likely to be well received, as the underlying Motivation already exists (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Van Stralen and West2011).

Raw response patterns by species are provided in the Supplementary material.

Linking behavioural barriers to possible intervention strategies

Now that the sources of behaviour influencing small mammal housing behaviour in the UK have been identified, the specific barriers can be systematically linked to intervention functions shown to be effective in addressing them (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Atkins and West2014). For example, according to the Behaviour Change Wheel framework, barriers associated with opportunity are best addressed via the use of interventions that serve the functions of ‘Training’, ‘Restriction’, ‘Modelling’, ‘Environmental restructuring’ and ‘Enablement’ (Michie et al. Reference Michie, Van Stralen and West2011). Restricting sale of unsuitable housing (‘Restriction’) or increasing the availability of suitable housing (‘Environmental restructuring’), for instance, would make the desired behaviour more feasible and accessible to pet owners by targeting physical opportunity. Improved access to professional advice is another avenue for addressing lack of social opportunity. Given that many small pet owners are less likely to bring their animals for veterinary care (Fox & Neville Reference Fox and Neville2024), expert advice at the point of sale, for example, has the potential to reach a large number of pet owners.

Subjective animal health and welfare assessment: Word frequency analysis

This section explores owner perceptions of pet rabbit and rodent welfare using open text responses. The sample of gerbil, mouse, degu and chinchilla owners was too small to include in the analysis. Tables 4 to 7 show the top ten words that were perceived by owners to describe positive (happiness/good health) and negative (unhappiness/ill health) welfare status by species (rabbits, guinea pigs, hamsters and rats, respectively).

Table 4. The frequency of words used by rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) owners (n = 231) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

1 Binkys: “Spontaneous leaps into the air, sometimes with body twist (McMahon and Wigham, Reference McMahon and Wigham2020);

2 flops = “Flopping onto their side” (McMahon & Wigham Reference McMahon and Wigham2020);

3 zoomy: “Fast, excited running that doesn’t involve chasing to mount/bite (McMahon & Wigham Reference McMahon and Wigham2020);

4 thumps = “thump the ground with the hind feet” (adapted from Thurston & Ottensen Reference Thurston and Ottesen2020).

Table 5. The frequency of words used by guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) owners (n = 219) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

1 popcorns = “rapid locomotion in which the animal jumps into the air with all four limbs of the ground, often accompanied by rapid running and turning in multiple directions” (Harrup & Rooney Reference Harrup and Rooney2020);

2 wheeking = “high-pitched vocalisation usually performed in anticipation of food or other reward” (Harrup & Rooney Reference Harrup and Rooney2020).

Table 6. The frequency of words used by hamster (Cricetinae) owners (n = 154) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

Table 7. The frequency of words used by rat (Rattus norvegicus) owners (n = 77) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

1 boggling = “eyes ‘popping’ in and out” (Neville et al. Reference Neville, Mounty, Benato, Hunter, Mendl and Paul2022);

2 bruxing = “grinding teeth without movement of the eyes” (Neville et al. Reference Neville, Mounty, Benato, Hunter, Mendl and Paul2022).

Word frequency analysis revealed that, overall, behavioural indicators were most commonly used to identify positive health and welfare states, while nutritional and physical signs were cited most frequently for negative states.

Interestingly, in the current study, ‘eating’ was the most commonly used word referred to when indicating unhappiness/ill health across all four examined species, and ‘eats’ was the most commonly used word referred to when indicating happiness/good health for guinea pigs and hamsters, coming second for rabbits and rats.

A reduction in eating behaviour can serve as an early indicator of underlying issues, including pain, which prey species such as rabbits and rodents are generally considered to seek to conceal, while also reducing the need for physical interaction with animals that are often difficult to handle (Carbone Reference Carbone2020; Venkataraman & Raajkamal Reference Venkataraman and Raajkamal2021; James & Wills Reference James and Wills2025). Indeed, the UK Joint Working Group on Refinement (JWGR) identified food consumption as a general indicator of welfare that can reflect the physical, physiological and psychological state of laboratory animals such as rodents (Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Morton, Burman, Dennison, Honess and Jennings2011). Furthermore, as animals like rabbits and guinea pigs eat continuously throughout the day (Gidenne et al. Reference Gidenne, Lebas, Fortun-Lamothe, De Blas and Wiseman2010; Elfers et al. Reference Elfers, Armbrecht and Mazzuoli-Weber2021), changes to eating behaviour may be more noticeable to their owners than in species that typically eat less frequently. Together with the current study findings, this suggests that changes in eating behaviour may be a particularly salient and intuitive welfare cue for owners across multiple species and should be further explored as a potential iceberg indicator of small pet health and welfare.

In their systematic review of validated small mammal animal welfare assessment methods, Cohen and Ho (Reference Cohen and Ho2023) identified welfare indicators shared across rabbits, guinea pigs, rats and mice, including changes in faecal output, bodyweight changes, presence of discharge from the eyes and nose, and altered food and water consumption. Several of these were identified by owners in the current study. This suggests that pet owners do possess the ability to identify relevant signs of poor health and welfare in their animals. Furthermore, this highlights the possibility that small mammal pet species may share welfare indicators that could be used to develop practical, broadly applicable monitoring tools for pet owners.

Figures 1 and 2 display signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/ill health, as perceived by pet owners, mapped to four of the five welfare domains, Nutrition’, ‘Health’, ‘Behaviour’ and ‘Mental state’ (Mellor Reference Mellor2017). Overall, the most common domain used to signal happiness/good health across rats, hamsters, guinea pigs and rabbits was ‘Behavioural interactions’. The most common domain used to signal unhappiness/ill health in all species, apart from rats was ‘Nutrition’, with ‘Health’ being most common for rats.

Figure 1. Word frequency analysis to determine signs of happiness/good health, as perceived by pet owners (n = 723), mapped to four of the five welfare domains

Figure 2. Word frequency analysis to determine signs of unhappiness/ill health, as perceived by pet owners (n = 723), mapped to four of the five welfare domains.

Study limitations

This study has a number of limitations. Firstly, pet owners recruited through targeted advertising may not accurately represent the typical small mammal pet owner. Instead, they are more likely to have greater species-specific knowledge and greater competence in care provision (Hedley et al. Reference Hedley, Pettitt and Abeyesinghe2023; Fox & Neville Reference Fox and Neville2024). This may limit the generalisability of the findings. Future research could utilise a point-of-purchase design, to capture information from an unbiased sample of participants. Furthermore, the use of self-reported data may not offer an accurate reflection of actual owner knowledge or practices, as participants may overestimate their capability or respond in socially desirable ways. This limitation will be addressed by comparing self-reported capability with actual husbandry practices in the same sample of participants (Wills et al. in prep). It is also worth noting that while multiple small mammal species were included in this study, some were underrepresented, potentially reducing the applicability of the results across all commonly kept small mammal species. In particular, further research with degu and chinchilla owners is needed. It is also possible that social desirability played a role in this study. In particular, participants may have felt compelled to respond favourably to very direct and value-laden items such as “Providing good animal welfare is a priority for me”. One solution would be to use indirect questions (Ried et al. Reference Ried, Eckerd and Kaufmann2022), for instance, “Providing good animal welfare is a priority for small mammal pet owners”. This has been used previously in animal welfare research, with findings indicating differences between directly and indirectly worded questions (e.g. Lusk & Norwood Reference Lusk and Norwood2010). Follow-up work will compare self-reported responses to submitted images of housing and enrichment, allowing us to assess the extent of social desirability bias. Finally, this survey collected information from UK pet owners which may not translate directly to other countries, or to other contexts, such as laboratory settings.

Animal welfare implications

This study identified Opportunity as the principal barrier to providing suitable housing for small mammal pets, with most owners reporting challenges accessing appropriate enclosures. Capability and Motivation were reported as high, suggesting that many owners are both willing and able to provide good welfare, but are constrained by external factors. The findings also show that owners use observable cues, particularly eating behaviour, to assess animal health and welfare. These cues may offer a practical means of early detection of health or welfare concerns, especially in prey species, such as rabbits and rodents, which are known to conceal signs of pain or illness. Addressing the gap between owner motivation and the availability of suitable housing will require targeted interventions designed with industry stakeholders. Improvements in the design, promotion and accessibility of welfare-compliant housing, in conjunction with guidance that reflects species-specific needs, could support better husbandry across a wide range of small mammal pets.

Conclusion

This study used the COM-B model to examine the behavioural factors influencing the provision of suitable housing for small mammal pets. Lack of Opportunity was identified as the primary barrier for UK small mammal pet owners, while Capability and Motivation acted as facilitators. The results of the current study can now be mapped to suitable intervention strategies to increase the use of species-appropriate housing solutions. Indicators of ill health and poor welfare reported by pet owners often related to nutrition, particularly changes in eating behaviour. Further research should explore the potential of eating behaviour as an iceberg indicator of health and welfare issues in small mammal pets.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2025.10034.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Blue Cross for funding the data collection and study advertisement. Thanks to the pet owners who took the time to complete the survey.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contribution: Conceptualisation: GC, KT, CS, AW; Formal analysis: GC; Funding acquisition: KT, CS; Investigation: GC; Methodology: GC, KT, CS, AW; Project administration: GC, AW; Validation: AW; Software: GC; Visualisation: GC; Writing – original draft: GC; Writing – review and editing: GC, KT, CS, AW

References

Allaby, M 2003 A Dictionary of Zoology. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Baradad, VP and Mugabushaka, AM 2015 Corpus specific stop words to improve the textual analysis in scientometrics. ISSI Conference, June 2015. https://www.issi-society.org/proceedings/issi_2015/0999.pdf (accessed 3 August 2025).Google Scholar
Bläske, A, Schwarzer, A, Ebner, MV, Gerbig, H, Reese, S, Erhard, M and Wöhr, AC 2022 Evaluation of small mammal pet supplies offered in German retail under animal welfare aspects. PLOS One 17: e0262658. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0262658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blue Cross 2024 Blue Cross Pet Census 2024. https://www.bluecross.org.uk/pet-census (accessed 30 July 2025).Google Scholar
Cane, J, O’Connor, D and Michie, S 2012 Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implementation Science 7: 37. https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-7-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbone, L 2020 Do “prey species” hide their pain? Implications for ethical care and use of laboratory animals. Journal of Applied Animal Ethics Research 2: 216236. https://doi.org/10.1163/25889567-BJA10001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, GA and Groarke, JM 2019 The importance of the social sciences in reducing tail biting prevalence in pigs. Animals 9: 591. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9090591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, GA, Groarke, JM and Graham-Wisener, L 2021 Human behaviour change models for improving animal welfare. Bridging Research Disciplines to Advance Animal Welfare Science: A Practical Guide pp 91106. CABI: Wallingford, UK.Google Scholar
Cohen, S and Ho, C 2023 Review of rat (Rattus norvegicus), mouse (Mus musculus), guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) and rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) indicators for welfare assessment. Animals 13(13): 2167. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13132167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornish, A, Jamieson, J, Raubenheimer, D and McGreevy, P 2019 Applying the behavioural change wheel to encourage higher welfare food choices. Animals 9: 524. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9080524CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowdell, F and Dyson, J 2019 How is the theoretical domains framework applied to developing health behaviour interventions? A systematic search and narrative synthesis. BMC Public Health 19: 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7442-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Díaz-Berciano, C and Gallego-Agundez, M 2024 Abandonment and rehoming of rabbits and rodents in Madrid (Spain): a retrospective study (2008–2021). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 27: 712722. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2022.2162342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, LM, Hardiman, JR and Cooper, JJ 2010 The effects of spatial restriction on the behavior of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Journal of Veterinary Behavior 5: 302308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2010.07.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
EFSA Panel on Animal Health and Welfare 2012 Statement on the use of animal‐based measures to assess the welfare of animals. EFSA Journal 10: 2767. https://doi.org/10.2903/j.efsa.2012.2767Google Scholar
Elfers, K, Armbrecht, Y and Mazzuoli-Weber, G 2021 Good to know: Baseline data on feed intake, fecal pellet output and intestinal transit time in guinea pig as a frequently used model in gastrointestinal research. Animals 11(6): 1593. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espinosa García-San Román, J, Quesada-Canales, Ó, Arbelo Hernández, M, Déniz Suárez, S and Castro-Alonso, A 2023 Veterinary education and wtraining on non-traditional companion animals, exotic, zoo, and wild animals: Concepts review and challenging perspective on zoological medicine. Veterinary Sciences 10(5): 357. https://doi.org/10.3390/vetsci10050357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feng, X and Behar-Horenstein, LS 2019 Maximizing NVivo utilities to analyze open-ended responses. The Qualitative Report 24: 563571. https://doi.org/10.46743/2160-3715/2019.3692Google Scholar
Fox, A and Neville, V 2024 Burrowing for answers: Investigating Syrian hamster welfare through owner surveys. Veterinary Record 195(9): e4534. https://doi.org/10.1002/vetr.4534CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fulton, EA, Brown, KE, Kwah, KL and Wild, S 2016 StopApp: using the behaviour change wheel to develop an app to increase uptake and attendance at NHS Stop Smoking Services. Healthcare 4(2): 31. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare4020031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, RA, Montrose, VT and Wills, AP 2017 ExNOTic: Should we be keeping exotic pets? Animals 7: 47. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7060047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gidenne, T, Lebas, F and Fortun-Lamothe, L 2010 Chapter 13. Feeding behaviour of rabbits. In: De Blas, C and Wiseman, J (eds) Nutrition of the Rabbit, Second Edition pp 233252. CAB International: Wallingford, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhofer, EM, Hebesberger, DV, Waiblinger, S, Künzel, F, Rouha-Mülleder, C, Mariti, C and Windschnurer, I 2024 Husbandry conditions and welfare state of pet chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera) and caretakers’ perceptions of stress and emotional closeness to their animals. Animals 14: 3155. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani14213155CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrup, AJ and Rooney, NJ 2020 Current welfare state of pet guinea pigs in the UK. Veterinary Record 186: 282. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.105632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, P, Morton, DB, Burman, O, Dennison, N, Honess, P and Jennings, M 2011 A guide to defining and implementing protocols for the welfare assessment of laboratory animals: eleventh report of the BVAAWF/FRAME/RSPCA/UFAW Joint Working Group on Refinement. Laboratory Animals 45: 113. https://doi.org/10.1258/la.2010.010031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedley, JE, Pettitt, A and Abeyesinghe, SM 2023 Preliminary investigation into the housing of dwarf hamsters. Veterinary Record 193(4): e3170. https://doi.org/10.1002/vetr.3170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, L and Wills, AP 2025 Perception and utilisation of veterinary services by rodent owners in the United Kingdom. Veterinary Record 196(8): e4958. https://doi.org/10.1002/vetr.4958CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linderholm, A and Larson, G 2013 The role of humans in facilitating and sustaining coat colour variation in domestic animals. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology 24: 587593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.semcdb.2013.03.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonstein, JS and De Vries, GJ 2000 Sex differences in the parental behavior of rodents. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 24: 669686. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00036-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lusk, JL and Norwood, FB 2010 Direct versus indirect questioning: an application to the well-being of farm animals. Social Indicators Research 96(3): 551565. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9492-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazhary, H and Hawkins, P 2019 Applying the 3Rs: A case study on evidence and perceptions relating to rat cage height in the UK. Animals 9: 1104. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9121104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, A and Strunk, A 2016 Common emergencies in small rodents, hedgehogs, and sugar gliders. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice 19: 465499. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvex.2016.01.008Google ScholarPubMed
McMahon, SA and Wigham, E 2020 ‘All Ears’: A questionnaire of 1516 owner perceptions of the mental abilities of pet rabbits, subsequent resource provision, and the effect on welfare. Animals 10: 1730. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mee, G, Tipton, E, Oxley, JA and Westgarth, C 2022 Owner demographic factors are associated with suitable pet rabbit housing provision in the United Kingdom. Veterinary Record 190: e1736. https://doi.org/10.1002/vetr.1736CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, DJ 2017 Operational details of the five domains model and its key applications to the assessment and management of animal welfare. Animals 7(8): 60. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7080060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michie, S, Atkins, L and West, R 2014 The Behaviour Change Wheel. A Guide to Designing Interventions. Silverback Publishing: Sutton, UK.Google Scholar
Michie, S, Van Stralen, MM and West, R 2011 The behaviour change wheel: A new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implementation Science 6: 42. https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-6-42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, MA 2009 History of exotic pets. Manual of Exotic Pet Practice pp 13. WB Saunders: St Louis, MO, USA.Google Scholar
Neesam, S 2015 Welfare guidance for proper care of cavies. http://www.britishcavycouncil.org.uk/Welfare/ (accessed 1 August 2025).Google Scholar
Neville, V, Mounty, J, Benato, L, Hunter, K, Mendl, M and Paul, ES 2022 Thinking outside the lab: Can studies of pet rats inform pet and laboratory rat welfare? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 246: 105507. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, DG, Kim, K, Brodbelt, DC, Church, DB, Pegram, C and Baldrey, V 2022 Demography, disorders and mortality of pet hamsters under primary veterinary care in the United Kingdom in 2016. Journal of Small Animal Practice 63: 747755. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsap.13527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onwuegbuzie, AJ and Leech, NL 2007 Validity and qualitative research: An oxymoron? Quality & Quantity 41: 233249. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-006-9000-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ried, L, Eckerd, S and Kaufmann, L 2022 Social desirability bias in PSM surveys and behavioral experiments: Considerations for design development and data collectionJournal of Purchasing and Supply Management 28(1): 100743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pursup.2021.100743CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rioja‐Lang, F, Bacon, H, Connor, M and Dwyer, CM 2019 Rabbit welfare: Determining priority welfare issues for pet rabbits using a modified Delphi method. Veterinary Record Open 6: e000363. https://doi.org/10.1136/vetreco-2019-000363CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, NJ, Dean, RS, Cobb, M and Brennan, ML 2014 Consultation length in first opinion small animal practice. Veterinary Record 175: 486. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.102713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rooney, NJ, Blackwell, EJ, Mullan, SM, Saunders, R, Baker, PE and Hill, JM 2014 The current state of welfare, housing and husbandry of the English pet rabbit population. BMC Research Notes 7: 113. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-942CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, AP, Norvelle, A, Choi, DC, Walton, JC, Albers, HE and Huhman, KL 2017 Social housing and social isolation: Impact on stress indices and energy balance in male and female Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Physiology & Behavior 177: 264269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saré, RM, Lemons, A and Smith, CB 2021 Behavior testing in rodents: Highlighting potential confounds affecting variability and reproducibility. Brain Sciences 11: 522. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11040522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneidewind, S, Lesch, R, Heizmann, V and Windschnurer, I 2024 Exploring pet rat care: A comprehensive survey of husbandry, health, behavior, and the associations between caretaker attitudes, attachment, and husbandry practices. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 76: 4961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2024.06.009Google Scholar
Skovlund, CR, Forkman, B, Lund, TB, Mistry, BG, Nielsen, SS and Sandøe, P 2023 Perceptions of the rabbit as a low investment ’starter pet’ lead to negative impacts on its welfare: Results of two Danish surveys. Animal Welfare 32: e45. https://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2023.41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thurston, S and Ottesen, JL 2020 The rabbit. Animal-centric Care and Management pp 135148. CRC Press: USA.Google Scholar
Truelove, S, Vanderloo, LM, Tucker, P, Di Sebastiano, KM and Faulkner, G 2020 The use of the behaviour change wheel in the development of ParticipACTION’s physical activity app. Preventive Medicine Reports 20: 101224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmedr.2020.101224CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
UK Government 2006 Animal Welfare Act 2006. https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/2006/45/contents (accessed 20 May 2025).Google Scholar
UK Pet Food 2024 2024 Annual Report. https://ukpetfood-reports.co.uk/ (accessed 30 July 2025).Google Scholar
Venkataraman, K and Raajkamal, BS 2021 Clinical examination of laboratory rodents and rabbits. Essentials of Laboratory Animal Science: Principles and Practices pp 521539. Elsevier: London, UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whittal, A, Atkins, L and Herber, OR 2021 What the guide does not tell you: reflections on and lessons learned from applying the COM-B behavior model for designing real life interventionsTranslational Behavioral Medicine 11(5): 11221126. https://doi.org/10.1093/tbm/ibaa116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wills, A and Holt, S 2020 Confidence of veterinary surgeons in the United Kingdom in treating and diagnosing exotic pet species. Veterinary Record 186: e20. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.105664CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wills, AP 2020 Impact of husbandry on the welfare of pet guinea pigs in the UK. Veterinary Record 186(9): 279281. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.m743CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, J and Baumans, V 2019 Rodents. In: McMillan, FD (ed) Companion Animal Care and Welfare: The UFAW Companion Animal Handbook pp 145156. CABI: Boston, MA, USA.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Self-reported Capability of owners (n = 723) of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus)to provide appropriate housing for small mammals

Figure 1

Table 2. Self-reported Opportunity of owners (n = 723) of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus) to provide appropriate housing for small mammals

Figure 2

Table 3. Self-reported Motivation of owners (n = 723) of rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus), guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus), hamsters (Cricetinae), gerbils (Gerbillinae), rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), chinchillas (Chinchilla lanigera), and degus (Octodon degus) to provide appropriate housing for small mammals

Figure 3

Table 4. The frequency of words used by rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) owners (n = 231) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

Figure 4

Table 5. The frequency of words used by guinea pig (Cavia porcellus) owners (n = 219) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

Figure 5

Table 6. The frequency of words used by hamster (Cricetinae) owners (n = 154) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

Figure 6

Table 7. The frequency of words used by rat (Rattus norvegicus) owners (n = 77) in describing signs of happiness/good health and unhappiness/illness

Figure 7

Figure 1. Word frequency analysis to determine signs of happiness/good health, as perceived by pet owners (n = 723), mapped to four of the five welfare domains

Figure 8

Figure 2. Word frequency analysis to determine signs of unhappiness/ill health, as perceived by pet owners (n = 723), mapped to four of the five welfare domains.

Supplementary material: File

Carroll et al. supplementary material

Carroll et al. supplementary material
Download Carroll et al. supplementary material(File)
File 432.6 KB