Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-smtgx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-08T07:19:35.444Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Calling a trade-off a trade-off in arguments for cat confinement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2025

Carmen Glanville
Affiliation:
Faculty of Science, University of Melbourne , Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia
Jordan O. Hampton*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Science, University of Melbourne , Parkville, Victoria 3010, Australia School of Veterinary Medicine and Harry Butler Institute, Murdoch University , Murdoch, Western Australia 6150, Australia
Peter Sandoe
Affiliation:
Department of Food and Resource Economics and Department of Veterinary and Animal Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Frederiksberg, Denmark
*
Corresponding author: Jordan O. Hampton; Email: jordan.hampton@murdoch.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In different parts of the world the claim is increasingly being made that continuous confinement of pet cats (Felis catus) is beneficial for both wildlife conservation and cat welfare. The first part of the claim is almost incontrovertible, but the second is misleading. The assertion that confined animals have superior welfare is rooted in thinking pre-dating the 1960s that equates welfare with physical health. By contemporary accounts of animal welfare, confinement of animals presents major welfare risks, and this recognition has been a major driver of refinement in livestock industries, e.g. moves towards free-range systems. Yet, these risks have not been widely acknowledged in debates over pet cat management. We argue that the current pervasive rhetoric from conservationists and some regulators that cat confinement is beneficial for wildlife and cats is, at best, confusing health with welfare. At worst, it is a deliberate attempt to mislead the public through portraying a win-win scenario where, instead, a trade-off must be navigated. Failure to recognise this trade-off undermines conservation goals three-fold. First, it limits the efficacy of behaviour change interventions to increase confinement. Second, it erodes public trust in organisations perceived as knowingly misleading the public. Finally, it reduces the incentive to make the one decision yielding long-term benefits for both cats and ecosystems; ceasing to own cats at all. Policy-makers should be wary of the allure of false win-win narratives when tackling contentious issues that require trade-offs to be made.

Information

Type
Opinion paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Cats (Felis catus) are one of the most popular pets, with around 600 million pet cats worldwide (Ramakrishnan et al. Reference Ramakrishnan, Maier, Walker, Rehrauer, Joekel, Winiger, Basso, Grigg, Hehl and Deplazes2019) across all inhabited continents (Crawford et al. Reference Crawford, Calver and Fleming2019). While pet cats may provide benefits to their owners and by extension, society (McNicholas et al. Reference McNicholas, Gilbey, Rennie, Ahmedzai, J-A and Ormerod2005), there is increasing concern about the negative impact of cats’ natural hunting propensity on wildlife and biodiversity (Calver et al. Reference Calver, Cherkassky, Cove, Fleming, Lepczyk, Longcore, Marzluff, Rich and Sizemore2023). Disagreements regarding whether and how to manage these impacts has led to intense ethical discourse, typified by the interactions between Loss and Marra (Reference Loss and Marra2018) and Lynn et al. (Reference Lynn, Santiago-Ávila, Lindenmayer, Hadidian, Wallach and King2019) regarding cats in North America. The debate in Europe is quite different (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Nørspang, Kondrup, Bjørnvad, Forkman and Lund2018; Palmer & Thomas Reference Palmer and Thomas2023), given that cats have been abundant in this global region for around 2,000 years (Baca et al. Reference Baca, Popović, Panagiotopoulou, Marciszak, Krajcarz, Krajcarz, Makowiecki, Węgleński and Nadachowski2018), while there has been limited examination of pet cat husbandry in South America (Escobar-Aguirre et al. Reference Escobar-Aguirre, Alegría-Morán, Calderón-Amor and Tadich2019), Africa, and Asia. Globally, focus on managing free-ranging cats has perhaps been most intense in recent years in Oceania: namely Aotearoa, New Zealand (Palmer & Thomas Reference Palmer and Thomas2023; Ovenden et al. Reference Ovenden, Bassett and Sumner2024) and Australia (Legge et al. Reference Legge, Woinarski, Dickman, Murphy, L-A and Calver2020; Calver et al. Reference Calver, Cherkassky, Cove, Fleming, Lepczyk, Longcore, Marzluff, Rich and Sizemore2023).

Here, we focus on one interesting development in this debate: the recent proliferation of public education messages in Australia (echoing previous messaging campaigns from North America, reviewed in Palmer and Sandøe [Reference Palmer, Sandøe and Gruen2014]) that continuous confinement of pet cats benefits both wildlife and cats. To be clear from the outset, we do not oppose cat confinement per se, our concern (and the focus of this paper) is with the lack of recognition of the risks it poses to cat welfare, and subsequent issues this omission from public messaging is likely to cause.

Ethical statement

No approvals were required for the writing of this manuscript.

Pet cats in contemporary Australia

While free-roaming cats come in varying forms with respect to their level of socialisation and status relating to humans (Calver et al. Reference Calver, Cherkassky, Cove, Fleming, Lepczyk, Longcore, Marzluff, Rich and Sizemore2023), here, we will focus on cats which people have a degree of control over, i.e. owned or semi-owned. All references to ‘cats’ should be understood with this restriction.

Concerns over the environmental impacts of cats have been particularly elevated in Australia (Legge et al. Reference Legge, Woinarski, Dickman, Murphy, L-A and Calver2020), where native mammals, birds, and reptiles are highly susceptible to eutherian mammal predators (Radford et al. Reference Radford, Woinarski, Legge, Baseler, Bentley, Burbidge, Bode, Copley, Dexter and Dickman2018). Indeed, Australia has lost > 10% of native mammal species to extinction in the past two centuries, with predation by introduced mammalian predators (including cats) largely to blame (Woinarski et al. Reference Woinarski, Burbidge and Harrison2015). Consequently, it is unsurprising that Australia is making a concerted effort to reduce the impacts of cat predation on its remaining extant native fauna (Legge et al. Reference Legge, Murphy, McGregor, Woinarski, Augusteyn, Ballard, Baseler, Buckmaster, Dickman and Doherty2017). This is perhaps best illustrated by the Australian Government commissioning a formal Parliamentary Inquiry into “the problem of feral and domestic cats in Australia” in 2020 (Parliament of Australia 2020).

Despite this concern regarding the impacts of cats in Australia, cat ownership continues to increase, with an estimated 5.3 million pet cats nationally in 2022 (Animal Medicines Australia 2022). In the face of this ongoing popularity, there have been no serious moves to ban cat ownership. Instead, there has been a concerted effort to encourage cat owners to confine their cats.

Confinement

The idea of confinement (also called ‘containment’; van Eeden et al. [Reference van Eeden, Hames, Faulkner, Geschke, Squires and McLeod2021]) for cats is to restrict them to areas in which they cannot hunt native animals; either indoors or in contained areas (e.g. ‘cat runs’). Initial confinement strategies involved night-time curfews (i.e. cats confined from dusk until dawn) (Legge et al. Reference Legge, Woinarski, Dickman, Murphy, L-A and Calver2020). However, night-time curfews have been criticised for being insufficient to protect wildlife and therefore, continuous confinement (24 h per day) has been increasingly recommended (van Heezik Reference van Heezik2010; Legge et al. Reference Legge, Woinarski, Dickman, Murphy, L-A and Calver2020). Consequently, mandated continuous confinement is the direction many regulatory bodies are moving towards, including the Australian Government (2022).

Accordingly, public education campaigns encouraging owners to implement continuous cat confinement have increased (Zoos Victoria and RSPCA Victoria 2018; Invasive Species Council 2023). These campaigns originally focused on the benefit of cat confinement for wildlife (Grayson & Calver Reference Grayson, Calver, Lunney and Burgin2004). However, recent campaigns have adopted new messaging: that cat confinement is beneficial for wildlife conservation and cat welfare. Confinement is hence being presented as a ‘win-win’ solution for wildlife and cats (Wahlquist Reference Wahlquist2023). This phenomenon represents the culmination of a public relations campaign that envisioned “cat containment campaigns could be improved by appealing to owners’ concerns about cat well-being” (van Eeden et al. Reference van Eeden, Hames, Faulkner, Geschke, Squires and McLeod2021).

Various stakeholders have adopted this stance, including conservation advocacy groups (Invasive Species Council 2023), and the mass-media (Wahlquist Reference Wahlquist2023). Influential journal articles have also referred to “the benefits of containment to pet cat welfare” (Legge et al. Reference Legge, Woinarski, Dickman, Murphy, L-A and Calver2020). While the benefit to wildlife is undeniable in Australia, that confinement is good for cats is a major leap in the argument, and a highly dubious assertion.

Cat welfare

The recent public relations campaign in Australia has appealed to the concerns of cat owners regarding cat well-being (van Eeden et al. Reference van Eeden, Hames, Faulkner, Geschke, Squires and McLeod2021). Such a strategy is heavily reliant upon how welfare (or ‘well-being’) is defined. We contend that how animal welfare has been defined in this context does not align with contemporary conceptions.

There are both welfare benefits and risks for cats associated with either confinement or free-roaming, which have been summarised elsewhere (Stella & Croney Reference Stella and Croney2016; Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019; Tan et al. Reference Tan, Stellato and Niel2020). In short, cats allowed to roam face greater risks to health and safety through infectious diseases, fighting injuries, road traffic accidents, predation, parasites, and persecution (such as poisoning) by disgruntled community members (Tan et al. Reference Tan, Stellato and Niel2020). Consequently, one key argument made by confinement proponents is that indoor cats live longer lives and, therefore, confinement is good for them (Fischer Reference Fischer2020). However, even if it is true that confined cats live longer (for which there is some conflicting evidence [Calver et al. Reference Calver, Cherkassky, Cove, Fleming, Lepczyk, Longcore, Marzluff, Rich and Sizemore2023]), lifespan alone is not a robust measure of animal welfare by modern standards.

The claim that confinement is only good for cat welfare stems from a narrow and outdated view of what animal welfare is. This view equates safety and physical health with welfare, or at least heavily prioritises them. Yet science moved on from conceiving of animal welfare in this way in the 1960s, when the importance of natural behaviour and positive mental experiences was recognised (Hemsworth et al. Reference Hemsworth, Mellor, Cronin and Tilbrook2015). Indeed, confinement and the restrictions it places upon animal behaviour was a driving factor behind the animal welfare movement (Harrison Reference Harrison1964; Brambell Reference Brambell1965). Precipitated by the Five Freedoms, modern conceptions of animal welfare now recognise and prioritise the importance of animal’s ability to perform natural behaviour, and their access to positive experiences (Mellor Reference Mellor2015). As such, it is well-recognised in almost all animal use or care settings, that confinement is a major risk to animal welfare (Barnett Reference Barnett2007).

The tyranny of confinement

“A ship in a harbour is safe but that is not what ships are built for” (Shedd Reference Shedd1928).

Confined cats, while generally being safer, face significant welfare risks, just of a different nature. In considering these risks, it is important to recognise that, in contrast to other domestic species (e.g. dogs [Canis familiaris]), cats’ genotype and phenotype, including their behavioural motivations, remain relatively unchanged (Cecchetti et al. Reference Cecchetti, Crowley and McDonald2021). Confinement of a species adapted to roaming and hunting is a clear deprivation of natural behaviour. The median home range sizes for free-ranging cats have been reported as 2.5 km2 for females and 5.1 km2 for males (Bengsen et al. Reference Bengsen, Algar, Ballard, Buckmaster, Comer, Fleming, Friend, Johnston, McGregor and Moseby2016). A recent Danish study of privately owned cats allowed to roam found that the median time cats spent away from their homes was 5 h per day (Jensen et al. Reference Jensen, Meilby, Nielsen and Sandøe2022). In stark contrast, many cats in Australia are confined to apartments, with an average area in the order of 130 m2 (Kendall & Ley Reference Kendall and Ley2006).

Continuous confinement and the associated thwarting of behavioural motivations can lead to frustration, chronic stress, and subsequent impacts on an animal’s behaviour and physiology (Lawson et al. Reference Lawson, Langford and Harvey2020). Indeed, indoor cats are reported to have more behavioural problems (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Nørspang, Forkman, Bjørnvad, Kondrup and Lund2017). These can be indicative of frustration and chronic stress and associated with higher hair cortisol levels (an indicator of chronic physiological stress) (Wojtas Reference Wojtas2023). Contained cats invariably receive less exercise, predisposing them to obesity and related health conditions (Rochlitz Reference Rochlitz2005). Contained cats are also more likely to develop urologic conditions (Segev et al. Reference Segev, Livne, Ranen and Lavy2011), hyperthyroidism (Scarlett et al. Reference Scarlett, Moise and Rayl1988), and odontoclastic resorptive lesions (Scarlett et al. Reference Scarlett, Saidla and Hess1999).

That said, it is clear that there are things owners of indoor-only cats can do to improve the welfare of indoor living (Ellis Reference Ellis2009). Namely, the provision of enrichment. Firstly, they can improve the physical indoor environment, e.g. by having different kinds of scratching posts (Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, Rodan, Carney, Heath, Rochlitz, Shearburn, Sundahl and Westropp2013). Secondly, it is possible to activate indoor cats via the provision of various forms of toys or via play and training (Dantas et al. Reference Dantas, Delgado, Johnson and Buffington2016). Third, walking the cat outside on a leash may also serve as a form of enrichment (Elford et al. Reference Elford, Cooke and Ventura2025). However, it is uncertain the extent to which these initiatives will compensate for the welfare loss linked to lack of freedom to roam (Foreman-Worsley & Farnworth Reference Foreman-Worsley and Farnworth2019).

While proponents of continuous confinement often claim that, with appropriate management, the indoor environment can fulfil a cat’s natural behavioural instincts (Foreman-Worsley et al. Reference Foreman-Worsley, Finka, Ward and Farnworth2021), the reality of whether the average owner is equipped or motivated to do so is debatable. Lawson et al. (Reference Lawson, Langford and Harvey2020) surveyed more than 12,000 Australian cat owners, reporting that 46% of households contained indoor cats. Based on a range of factors, they concluded that the environmental needs of most Australian pet cats are not being met. Put simply, there are positive experiences associated with roaming, hunting, and socialising that are unlikely to be able to be replicated indoors (Crowley et al. Reference Crowley, Cecchetti and McDonald2019).

Only by an antiquated and reductionist account of welfare (welfare = health) (Hampton et al. Reference Hampton, Hyndman, Barnes and Collins2015), or by some overly optimistic assumptions about the willingness of owners to invest what is required to enrich the lives of confined cats, is it credible to say that cat confinement yields clear net benefits for cat welfare. A similar thinking, that confinement reduces infectious disease and mortality, was used to support the development and continued use of farrowing crates for sows (Sus scrofa) (Barnett et al. Reference Barnett, Hemsworth, Cronin, Jongman and Hutson2001) and battery cages for laying hens (Gallus gallus domesticus) (Savory Reference Savory2004). Obviously, farrowing pens and battery cages impose a much greater level of confinement than confining cats to houses, but this difference is only a matter of degree. If one considers that confining cats should be mandatory, a logical extension of this idea would be not only allowance of farrowing pens and battery cages in farming but making their use mandatory. Today, very few stakeholders, including the general public and regulators, would see this as a win for animal welfare (Kipperman Reference Kipperman2015).

There may also be interesting lessons to be learnt from efforts within the zoo community to improve the welfare of captive wild animals, notably non-domesticated felid species, through enrichment (Goswami et al. Reference Goswami, Patel, Kadivar, Tyagi, Malik and Mondol2021). Here, positive effects, but also limitations, have been documented (Chan et al. Reference Chan, Khong, McWhorter and Fernandez2025). Interestingly, it has also been documented that some of these animals, notably lions (Panthera leo), live much longer in captivity than in the wild (Tidière et al. Reference Tidière, Gaillard, Berger, Müller, Bingaman Lackey, Gimenez, Clauss and Lemaître2016). However, very few people argue that lion welfare would be improved if all wild lions were moved into captivity (Peng et al. Reference Peng, Yeh and Broom2025). If one again logically extends the idea that confining pet cats indoors should be mandatory, one would likely arrive at the conclusion of mandatory housing of zoo animals in enclosed cages, rather than open-range settings, a proposition offering highly dubious overall welfare benefits (Clubb & Mason Reference Clubb and Mason2007).

In all, the welfare risks of roaming versus confinement for cats can be summarised as a trade-off that is familiar in animal welfare: that between greater health and safety versus the freedom to express natural behaviour and experience positive welfare states (Lassen et al. Reference Lassen, Sandøe and Forkman2006; Browning & Veit Reference Browning and Veit2021). This tension can be posed as a question: is it better to have a more enriched life with a greater risk of mortality, or a longer, safer life of arguably reduced quality (Abbate Reference Abbate2020)? Regardless of the answer, the assertion that confined cats have categorically better welfare, and therefore denying that the trade-off exists, is misleading.

The unintended consequences of a false narrative

Even for stakeholders that value conservation more than cat welfare, there is one pragmatic reason to care about this trade-off: owner behaviour change. Ultimately, cat confinement requires cat owners change their behaviour (Macdonald et al. Reference Macdonald, Milfont and Gavin2015), and failure to recognise the welfare risks of cat confinement is likely to impact the efficacy of education campaigns (Linklater et al. Reference Linklater, Farnworth, van Heezik, Stafford and MacDonald2019).

Multiple studies have examined owner-related predictors of cat confinement, primarily focusing on beliefs and attitudes (Toukhsati et al. Reference Toukhsati, Young, Bennett and Coleman2012; Macdonald et al. Reference Macdonald, Milfont and Gavin2015; McLeod et al. Reference McLeod, Hine and Bengsen2015, Reference McLeod, Evans, Jones, Paterson and Zito2020; Hall et al. Reference Hall, Adams, Bradley, Bryant, Davis, Dickman, Fujita, Kobayashi, Lepczyk, McBride, Pollock, Styles, van Heezik, Wang and Calver2016; Ma & McLeod Reference Ma and McLeod2023). These studies have largely found that owner beliefs about cat safety and perceived benefits of confinement for cat welfare, are most strongly predictive of existing cat confinement behaviour. Consequently, many of these studies have recommended that campaigns to increase cat containment would be more effective if they use ‘cat-benefit’ messaging over a ‘wildlife-protection’ argument (Macdonald et al. Reference Macdonald, Milfont and Gavin2015; Hall et al. Reference Hall, Adams, Bradley, Bryant, Davis, Dickman, Fujita, Kobayashi, Lepczyk, McBride, Pollock, Styles, van Heezik, Wang and Calver2016; van Eeden et al. Reference van Eeden, Hames, Faulkner, Geschke, Squires and McLeod2021). However, to our knowledge, only one published study has evaluated the efficacy of these different messages in changing behaviour and found they elicited similar effects on confinement intentions and behaviour (McLeod et al. Reference McLeod, Hine, Bengsen and Driver2017). Importantly, with small effect sizes and self-reported behavioural measures, it remains unclear how successful either message was at actually creating behaviour change. Therefore, there is no clear evidence that a ‘cat-benefit’ message is effective in changing owner behaviour, and we consider there are several risks associated.

In the work on owner attitudes and recommendations for ‘cat benefit’ messaging, a major factor has been seemingly overlooked: the dynamic relationship between animal behaviour and human behaviour. That is, as much as human behaviour influences animal behaviour and welfare, there is an important feedback loop whereby animal behaviour influences human attitudes and behaviour (Hemsworth & Coleman Reference Hemsworth and Coleman2011). The cat behavioural problems caused by confinement, including inconvenient toileting, aggression, and destruction of furniture, are highly objectionable to owners (Heidenberger Reference Heidenberger1997), and may result in owners allowing the cat to roam again because they are too hard to manage. In one study, several participants highlighted the cat’s behaviour (e.g. constant meowing, scratching furniture) as a problem they had to overcome with respect to confinement (McLeod et al. Reference McLeod, Evans, Jones, Paterson and Zito2020). In implementing any behaviour change intervention, preparing people for known or highly likely challenges they may encounter is critical to success and maintenance of behaviour change (Glanville et al. Reference Glanville, Abraham and Coleman2020). Therefore, cat owners must be prepared to manage behavioural problems as they arise or risk the intervention failing (Jongman Reference Jongman2007).

Reputational risk and source credibility

The wholesale promotion of confinement being good for cats also poses a reputational risk for organisations involved. Cat owners generally understand that confinement will impact cat welfare, as evident from several surveys identifying owner concern for their cat’s welfare as a major barrier to them implementing confinement (Crowley et al. Reference Crowley, Cecchetti and McDonald2019; van Eeden et al. Reference van Eeden, Hames, Faulkner, Geschke, Squires and McLeod2021). The Australian public relations campaign promoting cat confinement openly relies upon “engaging respected messengers that align with these concerns” (van Eeden et al. Reference van Eeden, Hames, Faulkner, Geschke, Squires and McLeod2021) to dissuade cat owners of the legitimacy of their concerns about cat welfare impacts. However, there are likely to be unintended consequences relating to the long-term trust of the target audience in messengers if ‘bad faith arguments’ are used in an attempt to affect targeted behaviour change (Gregg et al. Reference Gregg, Kidd, Bekessy, Martin, Robinson and Garrard2022).

If messaging presents confinement as categorically good for cat welfare, without at least warning about the need to ensure effective enrichment, cat owners may consider these ‘respected messengers’ to be untrustworthy or not giving due consideration to their animal’s interests. That is, if pet owners already believe that their cat’s welfare will be compromised (as demonstrated by aforementioned surveys), yet they are told the opposite without clear evidence, then they will naturally question this messaging and, by extension, the messenger (Gregg et al. Reference Gregg, Kidd, Bekessy, Martin, Robinson and Garrard2022). Alternatively, if owners do trust the advice initially, confine their cat, and then observe negative impacts on their cat’s welfare, this would be likely to prompt similar questioning and distrust (Cotterell et al. Reference Cotterell, Rand and Scotney2025). This type of insincere appeal to the animal welfare concerns of stakeholders has a name: ‘welfare washing’ (Bjørkdahl & Syse Reference Bjørkdahl and Syse2021). We argue this is a risk to the messenger’s ‘source credibility’ which is based on their perceived trustworthiness, expertise, and care (Bull et al. Reference Bull, Mills, Byrne-Davis and Hart2021).

Source credibility is central to the efficacy of persuasive messages and behaviour change interventions (Pornpitakpan Reference Pornpitakpan2004). Consequently, not only could this dubious narrative risk the efficacy of the present campaign to confine cats but it may also jeopardise effectiveness in other campaigns or business activities. Where scientists are involved, or a spurious scientific basis is used to justify confinement, this may also threaten public trust in science (Young et al. Reference Young, Searle, Butler, Simmons, Watt and Jordan2016). Such ‘white lies’ having a nasty habit of being found out and causing greater long-term problems than being honest in the first place would have caused (Olsen & Menkhorst Reference Olsen and Menkhorst2020).

Calling a trade-off a trade-off

Cat confinement is not an example of a win-win for biodiversity and animal welfare – it is a trade-off. To claim otherwise is, at best, presenting a one-sided account of animal welfare (Abbate Reference Abbate2020), and at worst, downright deceptive. Understandably, scientists and people involved in public relations often feel uncomfortable with trade-offs. When values conflict, the murky water of ethics must be navigated. This can be uncomfortable ground for scientists, who often lack the training or terminology from philosophy to engage in protracted ethical debates (Lynn et al. Reference Lynn, Santiago-Ávila, Lindenmayer, Hadidian, Wallach and King2019). To avoid this mire, claims of scientific consensus are sometimes touted (McIlroy-Young et al. Reference McIlroy-Young, Öberg and Leopold2021), or claims of science denialism levelled at those who point out the existence of a value trade-off (Loss & Marra Reference Loss and Marra2018).

Ethical dilemmas are obviously unappealing from a public relations viewpoint, and public education efforts dictate that straightforward messages (such as a win-win) are often sought (Masterson et al. Reference Masterson, Spierenburg and Tengö2019). Unfortunately, the odyssey of pet cat management carries with it conflicts that cannot be avoided or denied (as hard as we try). This would not be the first time in recent years that attempts have been made in vain to portray complex ethical trade-offs as straightforward win-win situations. Similar claims have been made regarding proposed One Health solutions that invariably favour (without acknowledgment) anthropogenic interests over those of individual animals or ecosystems (Palmer et al. Reference Palmer, Fischer, Gamborg, Hampton and Sandøe2023). This amounts to a disingenuous message to the public that they can both have their cake (a happy cat) and eat it too (an intact urban ecosystem). The most important risk associated with this, beyond the dishonesty involved, is that it takes away the incentive for people to make the one decision that is likely to yield long-term benefits for both cats and ecosystems in areas with vulnerable wildlife: ceasing to own cats (Lilith et al. Reference Lilith, Calver, Styles and Garkaklis2006).

Conclusion

We do not question the magnitude of ecological impacts that pet cats may create in areas with vulnerable wildlife. Nor do we question the efficacy of confinement in mitigating these impacts. What we question is the assertion that confinement is categorically good for cat welfare. In light of developments in animal welfare science over the past 60 years, it is not. Commentators making this assertion either lack knowledge of the tenets of contemporary animal welfare science or are deliberately misleading the public for political reasons. While this exploration of the issue has focused upon Australia, we wish to direct our critique to all those globally who claim that confinement is a benefit for cat welfare. As with many decisions facing humans in the Anthropocene, deciding what to do with cats involves trade-offs and conflicts that cannot be denied or avoided. Effective communication from conservationists to cat owners requires an acknowledgement of there being a real dilemma between cat welfare on the one hand, and wildlife protection on the other.

Acknowledgements

PS had the time spent on this manuscript paid for by a grant from Skibsreder Per Henriksen, R og Hustrus Fond. The funder had no say in how the study was conducted.

Competing interests

JH is currently a Section Editor at Animal Welfare and PS was previously a Section Editor at Animal Welfare. JH is the President of the Australasian Wildlife Management Society, a member of the Zoos Victoria Scientific Advisory Committee, and a member of the Australian Feral Cat Taskforce. The authors have no other potential competing interests to report.

Footnotes

Author contribution: Conceptualisation: CG, JH; Supervision: JH; Validation: PS; Visualisation: PS; Writing – original draft: JH; Writing – review & editing: CG, JH, PS.

References

Abbate, CE 2020 A defense of free-roaming cats from a hedonist account of feline well-being. Acta Analytica 35(3): 439461. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12136-019-00408-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Animal Medicines Australia 2022 Pets in Australia: a national survey of pets and people. Canberra, Australia: Animal Medicines Australia. https://animalmedicinesaustralia.org.au/wp-content/uploads/2022/11/AMAU008-Pet-Ownership22-Report_v1.6_WEB.pdf (accessed 2 September 2025).Google Scholar
Australian Government 2022 Australian Government response to the House of Representatives Standing Committee on the Environment and Energy report: tackling the feral cat pandemic. https://www.aph.gov.au/Parliamentary_Business/Committees/House/Former_Committees/Environment_and_Energy/Feralanddomesticcats/Government_Response (accessed 2 September 2025).Google Scholar
Baca, M, Popović, D, Panagiotopoulou, H, Marciszak, A, Krajcarz, M, Krajcarz, MT, Makowiecki, D, Węgleński, P and Nadachowski, A 2018 Human-mediated dispersal of cats in the Neolithic Central Europe. Heredity 121(6): 557563. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41437-018-0071-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, JL 2007 Effects of confinement and research needs to underpin welfare standards. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 2(6): 213218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2007.08.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, JL, Hemsworth, PH, Cronin, GM, Jongman, EC and Hutson, G 2001 A review of the welfare issues for sows and piglets in relation to housing. Australian Journal of Agricultural Research 52(1): 128. https://doi.org/10.1071/AR00057CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bengsen, A, Algar, D, Ballard, G, Buckmaster, T, Comer, S, Fleming, PJ, Friend, J, Johnston, M, McGregor, H and Moseby, K 2016 Feral cat home-range size varies predictably with landscape productivity and population density. Journal of Zoology 298(2): 112120. https://doi.org/10.1111/jzo.12290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjørkdahl, K and Syse, KVL 2021 Welfare washing: disseminating disinformation in meat marketing. Society & Animals 32(1): 3755. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-BJA10032Google Scholar
Brambell, FWR 1965 Report of the technical committee to enquire into the welfare of animals kept under intensive livestock husbandry systems. HMSO: London, UK.Google Scholar
Browning, H and Veit, W 2021 Freedom and animal welfare. Animals 11(4): 1148. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11041148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, ER, Mills, M, Byrne-Davis, LMT and Hart, JK 2021 Who is a credible source of preventive advice? An experimental vignette study of general public attitudes towards role expansion in health and social care. British Journal of Health Psychology 26(1): 198213. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calver, MC, Cherkassky, L, Cove, MV, Fleming, PA, Lepczyk, CA, Longcore, T, Marzluff, J, Rich, C and Sizemore, G 2023 The animal welfare, environmental impact, pest control functions, and disease effects of free-ranging cats can be generalized and all are grounds for humanely reducing their numbers. Conservation Science and Practice 5(10): e13018. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.13018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cecchetti, M, Crowley, SL and McDonald, RA 2021 Drivers and facilitators of hunting behaviour in domestic cats and options for management. Mammal Review 51(3): 307322. https://doi.org/10.1111/mam.12230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, TY, Khong, HYC, McWhorter, TJ and Fernandez, EJ 2025 Feline flourish: a scoping review of enrichment effects on zoo-housed felids. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 286: 106620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2025.106620CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clubb, R and Mason, GJ 2007 Natural behavioural biology as a risk factor in carnivore welfare: How analysing species differences could help zoos improve enclosures. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102(3–4): 303328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cotterell, J, Rand, J and Scotney, R 2025 Rethinking urban cat management—limitations and unintended consequences of traditional cat management. Animals 15(7): 1005. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani15071005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, HM, Calver, MC and Fleming, PA 2019 A case of letting the cat out of the bag - why Trap-Neuter-Return is not an ethical solution for stray cat (Felis catus) management. Animals 9(4): 171. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9040171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, SL, Cecchetti, M and McDonald, RA 2019 Hunting behaviour in domestic cats: an exploratory study of risk and responsibility among cat owners. People and Nature 1(1): 1830. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dantas, LM, Delgado, MM, Johnson, I and Buffington, CT 2016 Food puzzles for cats: feeding for physical and emotional wellbeing. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 18(9): 723732. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X16643753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elford, A, Cooke, AS and Ventura, BA 2025I want them to live their best lives:” A qualitative exploration of owner experiences with walking their cats. Animal Welfare 34: e56. https://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2025.10030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, SL 2009 Environmental enrichment: practical strategies for improving feline welfare. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 11(11): 901912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfms.2009.09.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, SL, Rodan, I, Carney, HC, Heath, S, Rochlitz, I, Shearburn, LD, Sundahl, E and Westropp, JL 2013 AAFP and ISFM feline environmental needs guidelines. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 15(3): 219230. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X13477537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Escobar-Aguirre, S, Alegría-Morán, RA, Calderón-Amor, J and Tadich, TA 2019 Can responsible ownership practices influence hunting behavior of owned cats? Results from a survey of cat owners in Chile. Animals 9(10): 745. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9100745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, B 2020 Keep your cats indoors: a reply to Abbate. Acta Analytica 35: 463468. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12136-020-00431-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foreman-Worsley, R and Farnworth, MJ 2019 A systematic review of social and environmental factors and their implications for indoor cat welfare. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 220: 104841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104841CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foreman-Worsley, R, Finka, LR, Ward, SJ and Farnworth, MJ 2021 Indoors or outdoors? An international exploration of owner demographics and decision making associated with lifestyle of pet cats. Animals 11(2): 253. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanville, C, Abraham, C and Coleman, G 2020 Human behaviour change interventions in animal care and interactive settings: a review and framework for design and evaluation. Animals 10(12): 2333. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10122333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, S, Patel, SK, Kadivar, R, Tyagi, PC, Malik, PK and Mondol, S 2021 Effects of a combined enrichment intervention on the behavioural and physiological welfare of captive Asiatic lions (Panthera leo persica). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 236: 105222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grayson, J and Calver, M 2004 Regulation of domestic cat ownership to protect urban wildlife: a justification based on the precautionary principle. In: Lunney, D and Burgin, S (eds) Urban Wildlife: More than Meets the Eye pp 169178. Royal Zoological Society of New South Wales: Sydney, NSW, Australia.10.7882/FS.2004.094CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregg, EA, Kidd, LR, Bekessy, SA, Martin, JK, Robinson, JA and Garrard, GE 2022 Ethical considerations for conservation messaging research and practice. People and Nature 4(5): 10981112. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, CM, Adams, NA, Bradley, JS, Bryant, KA, Davis, AA, Dickman, CR, Fujita, T, Kobayashi, S, Lepczyk, CA, McBride, EA, Pollock, KH, Styles, IM, van Heezik, Y, Wang, F and Calver, MC 2016 Community attitudes and practices of urban residents regarding predation by pet cats on wildlife: an international comparison. PloS ONE 11(4): e0151962. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151962CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, JO, Hyndman, TH, Barnes, A and Collins, T 2015 Is wildlife fertility control always humane? Animals 5(4): 10471071. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani5040398CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, R 1964 Animal Machines. Vincent Stuart Publishers: London, UK.Google Scholar
Heidenberger, E 1997 Housing conditions and behavioural problems of indoor cats as assessed by their owners. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 52(3–4): 345364. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01134-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemsworth, P, Mellor, DJ, Cronin, GM and Tilbrook, AJ 2015 Scientific assessment of animal welfare. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 63(1): 2430. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.966167CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemsworth, PH and Coleman, GJ 2011 Human-Livestock Interactions: The Stockperson and the Productivity and Welfare of Intensively Farmed Animals, Second Edition. CABI: Wallingford, UK.Google Scholar
Invasive Species Council 2023 Pet cats in Australia. https://invasives.org.au/our-work/feral-animals/cats-in-australia/pet-cats/ (accessed 2 September 2025).Google Scholar
Jensen, HA, Meilby, H, Nielsen, SS and Sandøe, P 2022 Movement patterns of roaming companion cats in denmark—a study based on GPS tracking. Animals 12(14): 1748. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12141748CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jongman, EC 2007 Adaptation of domestic cats to confinement. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 2(6): 193196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2007.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendall, K and Ley, J 2006 Cat ownership in Australia: Barriers to ownership and behavior. Journal of Veterinary Behavior 1(1): 516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2006.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kipperman, BS 2015 The role of the veterinary profession in promoting animal welfare. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 246(5): 502504. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.246.5.502CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lassen, J, Sandøe, P and Forkman, B 2006 Happy pigs are dirty!–conflicting perspectives on animal welfare. Livestock Science 103(3): 221230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.livsci.2006.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawson, GT, Langford, FM and Harvey, AM 2020 The environmental needs of many Australian pet cats are not being met. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 22(10): 898906. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X19890189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legge, S, Murphy, BP, McGregor, H, Woinarski, JC, Augusteyn, J, Ballard, G, Baseler, M, Buckmaster, T, Dickman, CR and Doherty, T 2017 Enumerating a continental-scale threat: how many feral cats are in Australia? Biological Conservation 206: 293303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2016.11.032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legge, S, Woinarski, JC, Dickman, CR, Murphy, BP, L-A, Woolley and Calver, MC 2020 We need to worry about Bella and Charlie: the impacts of pet cats on Australian wildlife. Wildlife Research 47(8): 523539. https://doi.org/10.1071/WR19174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lilith, M, Calver, M, Styles, I and Garkaklis, M 2006 Protecting wildlife from predation by owned domestic cats: application of a precautionary approach to the acceptability of proposed cat regulations. Austral Ecology 31(2): 176189. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-9993.2006.01582.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linklater, WL, Farnworth, MJ, van Heezik, Y, Stafford, KJ and MacDonald, EA 2019 Prioritizing cat-owner behaviors for a campaign to reduce wildlife depredation. Conservation Science and Practice 1(5): e29. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loss, SR and Marra, PP 2018 Merchants of doubt in the free-ranging cat conflict. Conservation Biology 32(2): 265266. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13085CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynn, WS, Santiago-Ávila, F, Lindenmayer, J, Hadidian, J, Wallach, A and King, BJ 2019 A moral panic over cats. Conservation Biology 33(4): 769776. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ma, GC and McLeod, LJ 2023 Understanding the factors influencing cat containment: identifying opportunities for behaviour change. Animals 13(10): 1630. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13101630CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macdonald, E, Milfont, T and Gavin, M 2015 What drives cat-owner behaviour? First steps towards limiting domestic-cat impacts on native wildlife. Wildlife Research 42(3): 257265. https://doi.org/10.1071/WR14164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson, VA, Spierenburg, M and Tengö, M 2019 The trade-offs of win–win conservation rhetoric: exploring place meanings in community conservation on the Wild Coast, South Africa. Sustainability Science 14(3): 639654. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-019-00696-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McIlroy-Young, B, Öberg, G and Leopold, A 2021 The manufacturing of consensus: a struggle for epistemic authority in chemical risk evaluation. Environmental Science & Policy 122: 2534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envsci.2021.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, LJ, Evans, D, Jones, B, Paterson, M and Zito, S 2020 Understanding the relationship between intention and cat containment behaviour: a case study of kitten and cat adopters from RSPCA Queensland. Animals 10(7): 1214. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLeod, LJ, Hine, DW and Bengsen, AJ 2015 Born to roam? Surveying cat owners in Tasmania, Australia, to identify the drivers and barriers to cat containment. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 122(3): 339344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2015.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLeod, LJ, Hine, DW, Bengsen, AJ and Driver, AB 2017 Assessing the impact of different persuasive messages on the intentions and behaviour of cat owners: a randomised control trial. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 146: 136142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prevetmed.2017.08.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNicholas, J, Gilbey, A, Rennie, A, Ahmedzai, S, J-A, Dono and Ormerod, E 2005 Pet ownership and human health: a brief review of evidence and issues. British Medical Journal 331(7527): 12521254. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.331.7527.1252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, D (2015) Positive animal welfare states and encouraging environment-focused and animal-to-animal interactive behaviours. New Zealand Veterinary Journal 63(1): 916. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.926800CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen, P and Menkhorst, P 2020 A recent investigation highlights the importance of honesty in ornithology and conservation. Emu-Austral Ornithology 120(2): 178180. https://doi.org/10.1080/01584197.2020.1758570CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ovenden, K, Bassett, I and Sumner, CL 2024 ‘I want you to want me’: how owners value cats’ choices has implications for cat containment. People and Nature 6(2): 548561. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, A and Thomas, V 2023 Categorisation of cats: managing boundary felids in Aotearoa New Zealand and Britain. People and Nature 5(5): 15391551. https://doi.org/10.1002/pan3.10519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, C, Fischer, B, Gamborg, C, Hampton, JO and Sandøe, P 2023 Zoonotic disease: bad bats? In: Wildlife Ethics: Ethics in Wildlife Management and Conservation pp 230244. Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK.10.1002/9781119611332.ch16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, C and Sandøe, P 2014 For their own good: captive cats and routine confinement. In: Gruen, L (ed) The Ethics of Captivity pp 135155. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Parliament of Australia 2020 Inquiry into the problem of feral and domestic cats in Australia. Parliament of Australia: Canberra, ACT, Australia. https://www.aph.gov.au/feralanddomesticcats#:~:text=Following%20a%20referral%20from%20the,and%20domestic%20cats%20in%20Australia (accessed 2 September 2025).Google Scholar
Peng, S, Yeh, HM and Broom, DM 2025 Zoos and other organisations with living world impacts should have honest policies. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 286: 106618. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2025.106618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pornpitakpan, C 2004 The persuasiveness of source credibility: a critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 34(2): 243281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radford, JQ, Woinarski, JC, Legge, S, Baseler, M, Bentley, J, Burbidge, AA, Bode, M, Copley, P, Dexter, N and Dickman, CR 2018 Degrees of population-level susceptibility of Australian terrestrial non-volant mammal species to predation by the introduced red fox (Vulpes vulpes) and feral cat (Felis catus). Wildlife Research 45(7): 645657. https://doi.org/10.1071/WR18008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramakrishnan, C, Maier, S, Walker, RA, Rehrauer, H, Joekel, DE, Winiger, RR, Basso, WU, Grigg, ME, Hehl, AB and Deplazes, P 2019 An experimental genetically attenuated live vaccine to prevent transmission of Toxoplasma gondii by cats. Scientific Reports 9(1): 1474. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-37671-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochlitz, I 2005 A review of the housing requirements of domestic cats (Felis silvestris catus) kept in the home. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 93(1): 97109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandøe, P, Nørspang, AP, Forkman, B, Bjørnvad, CR, Kondrup, SV and Lund, TB 2017 The burden of domestication: a representative study of welfare in privately owned cats in Denmark. Animal Welfare 26(1): 110. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.1.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandøe, P, Nørspang, AP, Kondrup, SV, Bjørnvad, CR, Forkman, B and Lund, TB 2018 Roaming companion cats as potential causes of conflict and controversy: a representative questionnaire study of the Danish public. Anthrozoös 31(4): 459473. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2018.1483870CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savory, C 2004 Laying hen welfare standards: a classic case of ‘power to the people’. Animal Welfare 13(S1): S153S158. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600014512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarlett, JM, Moise, NS and Rayl, J 1988 Feline hyperthyroidism: a descriptive and case-control study. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 6(4): 295309.10.1016/0167-5877(88)90041-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarlett, JM, Saidla, J and Hess, J 1999 Risk factors for odontoclastic resorptive lesions in cats. Journal of the American Animal Hospital Association 35(3): 188192. https://doi.org/10.5326/15473317-35-3-188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segev, G, Livne, H, Ranen, E and Lavy, E 2011 Urethral obstruction in cats: predisposing factors, clinical, clinicopathological characteristics and prognosis. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 13(2): 101108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfms.2010.10.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shedd, JA 1928 Salt from my Attic. Mosher Press: Ephrata, USA.Google Scholar
Stella, JL and Croney, CC 2016 Environmental aspects of domestic cat care and management: implications for cat welfare. The Scientific World Journal 2016: 6296315. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/6296315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, SML, Stellato, AC and Niel, L 2020 Uncontrolled outdoor access for cats: an assessment of risks and benefits. Animals 10(2): 258. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10020258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tidière, M, Gaillard, J-M, Berger, V, Müller, DW, Bingaman Lackey, L, Gimenez, O, Clauss, M and Lemaître, J-F 2016 Comparative analyses of longevity and senescence reveal variable survival benefits of living in zoos across mammals. Scientific Reports 6(1): 36361. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep36361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toukhsati, SR, Young, E, Bennett, PC and Coleman, GJ 2012 Wandering cats: attitudes and behaviors towards cat containment in Australia. Anthrozoös 25(1): 6174. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303712X13240472427195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Eeden, LM, Hames, F, Faulkner, R, Geschke, A, Squires, ZE and McLeod, EM 2021 Putting the cat before the wildlife: exploring cat owners’ beliefs about cat containment as predictors of owner behavior. Conservation Science and Practice 3(10): e502. https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Heezik, Y 2010 Pussyfooting around the issue of cat predation in urban areas. Oryx 44(2): 153154. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003060531000027XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlquist, C 2023 Keeping cats indoors is a rare solution where everybody wins. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/australia-news/2023/sep/12/keeping-cats-indoors-training-raising-how-tips-guide?CMP=share_btn_tw (accessed 2 September 2025).Google Scholar
Woinarski, JC, Burbidge, AA and Harrison, PL 2015 Ongoing unraveling of a continental fauna: decline and extinction of Australian mammals since European settlement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112(15): 45314540. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1417301112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wojtas, J 2023 Hair cortisol levels in cats with and without behavioural problems. Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery 25(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/1098612X221150624CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, JC, Searle, K, Butler, A, Simmons, P, Watt, AD and Jordan, A 2016 The role of trust in the resolution of conservation conflicts. Biological Conservation 195: 196202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2015.12.030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zoos Victoria and RSPCA Victoria 2018 Safe cat, safe wildlife. https://www.safecat.org.au/ (accessed 2 September 2025).Google Scholar