Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-dbm8p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-26T09:03:43.180Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The non-linearity between populist attitudes and ideological extremism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2025

Eduardo Ryô Tamaki
Affiliation:
German Institute for Global and Area Studies, University of Erfurt, Erfurt, Germany
Yujin J. Jung*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Mount St. Mary’s University, Emmitsburg, MD, USA
*
Corresponding author: Yujin J. Jung; Email: yujinjuliajung@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The relationship between populist attitudes and ideological orientations remains an area of considerable academic interest, yet much is still unknown about the ideological inclinations associated with populist attitudes. While many scholars acknowledge the link between populist attitudes and political ideology, existing studies often treat this relationship as either a given or a peripheral concern. This paper represents an initial exploration into the association between populist attitudes and political ideology. Utilizing data from the fifth wave (2016–2021) of the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems, which encompasses 43 countries and 52 elections, this study aims to uncover how this relationship manifests cross-nationally. By employing a variety of rigorous methodological models, including the Generalized Additive Model, our results reveal a nonlinear relationship between populist attitudes and political ideology. Specifically, we find that political ideology and populist attitudes exhibit a U-shaped nonlinear relationship and that ideological extremism and populist attitudes demonstrate an exponential nonlinear relationship. These findings emphasize the nuanced interplay between ideological positions and populist attitudes, providing a deeper understanding of how they intersect.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nd/4.0), which permits re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of EPS Academic Ltd.

1. Introduction

The global surge in populist sentiments has revitalized research on populism (Canovan, Reference Canovan, Mény and Surel2002; Mudde, Reference Mudde2004; Moffitt, Reference Moffitt2016; Inglehart and Norris, Reference Inglehart and Norris2017; Rodrik, Reference Rodrik2018). Populism is often characterized by a dichotomous view of society, delineating “the pure people” versus “the corrupt elite.” This characterization defines populism as a “thin” ideology, capable of adhering to various “thicker” ideologies within an ideational approach (Canovan, Reference Canovan, Mény and Surel2002; Mudde, Reference Mudde2004; Stanley, Reference Stanley2008; Hawkins et al., Reference Hawkins, Riding and Mudde2012). Building on this ideational framework, scholars have increasingly sought to understand how these thin populist attitudes at the individual level may align with more substantiated, thick ideologies (Akkerman et al., Reference Akkerman, Mudde and Zaslove2014; Schulz et al., Reference Schulz, Müller, Schemer, Wirz, Wettstein and Wirth2018; Zaslove and Meijers, Reference Zaslove and Meijers2023). Despite the growing academic interest in the demand side of populism, there remains a paucity of direct investigation into the correlations between populist attitudes and ideological inclinations. Consequently, much is still unknown about these correlations. This paper represents a pioneering effort to specifically focus on the relationship between populist attitudes and political ideologies, seeking to fill this gap in current research.

The relationship between populist attitudes and ideologies has been implicitly explored in studies that sought an overall understanding of the nature of populist attitudes. While not exclusively focused on the relationship between populism and ideology, these studies often concentrated on a single country or several countries, providing insights into how various factors, including ideologies, correlate with populist attitudes. Most of these region-centric studies have suggested that the closer a country’s ideological leanings are to either the left or the right, the higher the prevalence of populist attitudes. For instance, some scholars argue for a linear relationship, suggesting a tendency where certain ideological orientations correspond to increasingly populist attitudes. According to this perspective, populist attitudes can be positively correlated with either right-leaning ideology (Hainmueller and Hiscox, Reference Hainmueller and Hiscox2007; Kriesi et al., Reference Kriesi, Grande, Lachat, Dolezal, Bornschier and Frey2008; Bechtel et al., Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller and Margalit2014) or left-leaning ideology (Rico et al., Reference Rico, Guinjoan and Anduiza2017; Tsatsanis et al., Reference Tsatsanis, Andreadis and Teperoglou2018; Marcos-Marne et al., Reference Marcos-Marne, Plaza-Colodro and Hawkins2020). Others who present the nonlinear nature of populist attitudes, starting from populist parties literature, have highlighted that the relationship between populism and ideology can be nonlinear, such as the U-shaped, where the extremities of the political spectrum are similarly inclined toward populist attitudes, potentially more so than moderate or centrist positions (Rooduijn and Akkerman, Reference Rooduijn and Akkerman2017; Meijers and Zaslove, Reference Meijers and Zaslove2021).

In our paper, we aim to make three contributions to the literature on populist attitudes. First, this study marks a pioneering effort to place political ideology at the center of research on populist attitudes, treating it as a critical element rather than a peripheral variable. While prior studies have acknowledged the link between ideology and populist attitudes, they often relegated this relationship to a secondary position, using it as one of many explanatory factors for understanding populist attitudes. These studies frequently stopped at the observation that individuals with strong populist attitudes in a given country tended to align with certain ideological leanings, leaving the deeper dynamics of this relationship underexplored. Moreover, few have systematically examined its potential nonlinear dimensions, often addressing them only as tangential considerations. In contrast, our research shifts the analytical focus entirely to this interplay, aiming to illuminate the broader patterns of how populist attitudes intersect with ideological positions. This paper thoroughly investigates not only the correlation between populist attitudes and political ideologies but also their intersection with ideological extremism, offering a comprehensive and nuanced examination of these critical relationships.

Second, our study provides a comprehensive examination of the relationship between populist attitudes and ideologies on a cross-national scale, utilizing data from the fifth wave (2016–2021) of the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems, encompassing 43 countries and 52 elections. By analyzing an extensive dataset, this research identifies overarching patterns and trends in the interplay between populist attitudes and ideological orientations across different countries. In contrast to prior research that predominantly focused on individual cases or regional clusters, our approach expands the scope of analysis to identify broader patterns and variations on a global scale. This cross-national perspective enables a deeper understanding of how populist attitudes correlate with ideological positions globally and advances the comparative analysis of populism on a global scale.

Moreover, our robustness tests reveal a subtle, context-dependent asymmetry: while the overall U-shaped relationship between ideology and populist attitudes is robust, in several cases, populist sentiments tend to be modestly more pronounced among right-leaning respondents than among their left-leaning counterparts. This is particularly the case in settings dominated by right-wing populist parties. Such asymmetry provides an important caveat to our findings, underscoring the moderating influence of political supply in the relationship between populism and ideology at the individual level.

Finally, our research offers a methodologically rigorous examination of the relationship between populist attitudes and ideologies. While much of the existing literature has relied primarily on Ordinary Least Squares regression to explore this relationship, our study incorporates both OLS and Generalized Additive Models (GAMs), enabling a more nuanced analysis that captures potential nonlinear dynamics. By employing these complementary methods and conducting detailed methodological analyses, we systematically investigate the intricate interplay between populist attitudes and ideological orientations. This approach provides a more robust framework for understanding these relationships, contributing to the growing body of research on populism by offering a deeper, empirically grounded perspective on how these critical dimensions interact across diverse contexts.

Our research approach is structured as follows. We begin by defining populism from an ideational perspective and then review previous studies on populism that have touched upon ideologies, examining how they have approached the topic. Subsequently, we will methodically explain how we use data to explore the correlation between ideologies and populist attitudes. Finally, we conclude our paper with robustness tests to confirm and validate our findings on the relationship between these two aspects. This comprehensive approach ensures a thorough and detailed examination of the interplay between populist attitudes and ideologies.

2. Populism and ideology

The academic discourse on populism has significantly evolved, with scholars consistently grappling over its definitional clarity. Researchers have conceptualized populism from various perspectives, describing it as a political strategy (Mény and Surel, Reference Mény and Surel2001; Weyland, Reference Weyland2001; Reference Weyland, Kaltwasser, Taggart, Espejo and Ostiguy2017; Reference Weyland2024; Barr, Reference Barr and Carlos2018), a political style (Jagers and Walgrave, Reference Jagers and Walgrave2007; Moffitt and Tormey, Reference Moffitt and Tormey2014), or an interpretation of representation and democracy (Taggart, Reference Taggart, Mény and Surel2002; Kriesi, Reference Kriesi2014; Urbinati, Reference Urbinati2019). In recent years, scholarly consensus has increasingly favored viewing populism as a constellation of ideas (Hawkins et al., Reference Hawkins, Riding and Mudde2012), with some treating it primarily as discourse (Laclau, Reference Laclau and Panizza2005; Aslanidis, Reference Aslanidis2016) and others as ideology (Canovan, Reference Canovan, Mény and Surel2002; Mudde, Reference Mudde2004; Stanley, Reference Stanley2008), yet uniformly acknowledging its essential dichotomy between a virtuous populace and a corrupt elite. Among these, Mudde’s (Reference Mudde2004: 543) definition has gained widespread acceptance, characterizing populism as a thin-centered ideology that divides society into two homogeneous and antagonistic groups—“the pure people” and “the corrupt elite”—and argues for politics to be an expression of the general will of the people. This conceptualization highlights people-centrism and anti-elitism as populism’s core components, positioning it as distinct from more comprehensive ideologies such as liberalism, socialism, and conservatism, which propose broader worldviews beyond merely the relationship between the populace and the elite (Canovan, Reference Canovan, Mény and Surel2002; Mudde, Reference Mudde2004).

From this ideational perspective, populism is understood as a thin-centered ideology that does not exist independently but rather coexists and interacts dynamically with thicker ideological frameworks. This coexistence highlights the necessity of examining how populism integrates with, and is articulated through, these broader ideological structures. Such an approach allows researchers to delve deeper into public sentiments toward populism, moving beyond analyzing mere voting behavior to exploring how fundamental populist principles, including anti-elitism and people-centrism, resonate within society at an individual level. Consequently, this framework has significantly enriched scholarly insights into how populist attitudes align with and express themselves through specific ideological paradigms, contributing to varied political manifestations (Hunger and Paxton, Reference Hunger and Paxton2022). Central to this perspective is the notion that populist ideologies are characterized by distinctive, identifiable sets of beliefs that can be measured quantitatively. As a result, there has been growing academic interest in exploring how these thick ideologies intersect with populist attitudes, particularly at the individual level. (Akkerman et al., Reference Akkerman, Mudde and Zaslove2014; Schulz et al., Reference Schulz, Müller, Schemer, Wirz, Wettstein and Wirth2018; Castanho Silva et al., Reference Silva, Sebastian Jungkunz, Helbling and Littvay2020; Zaslove and Meijers, Reference Zaslove and Meijers2023)

Despite recent advances, scholarly attention to citizens’ attitudes toward populism remains relatively nascent (Huber et al., Reference Huber, Jankowski and Wegscheider2023, 136), and empirical studies that systematically examine how populist attitudes align with specific ideological orientations are still limited. This gap is significant because understanding this alignment offers valuable insights into how populist sentiments interact with broader ideological frameworks.

While existing studies have often examined this relationship implicitly within broader analyses of populist attitudes, they tend to focus on individual countries or groups of countries, providing indirect insights rather than systematic, cross-national findings. Many of these region-specific studies suggest that the prevalence of populist attitudes increases as ideological leanings move toward either the extreme left or right. For instance, some scholars have observed a right-sided pattern in Western Europe, where countries are affluent, thus making identity politics a more significant issue (e.g., Hainmueller and Hiscox, Reference Hainmueller and Hiscox2007; Kriesi et al., Reference Kriesi, Grande, Lachat, Dolezal, Bornschier and Frey2008; Bechtel et al., Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller and Margalit2014). Bernhard and Hänggli Reference Bernhard and Hänggli(2018) also found that in Switzerland, where the radical right party has been tremendously successful, opposition to the EU and xenophobia are prevalent, resulting in a high correlation between populist attitudes and right-wing ideologies. Similarly, Marcos-Marne, Llamazares, and Schikano (2021) discovered through an original survey that individuals on the extreme right of the cultural dimension exhibit higher populist attitudes compared to those on the far left.

Concurrently with the right-leaning argument, other regional studies have also presented a left-leaning perspective. For example, Rico et al. Reference Rico, Guinjoan and Anduiza(2017) pointed to a strong correlation between populist attitudes and left-wing ideologies among the Spanish populace. Marcos-Marne, Llamazares, and Schikano (2021) also argued, based on their survey, that in Spain, individuals on the extreme left in economic and cultural dimensions exhibited higher populist attitudes. Tsatsanis et al. Reference Tsatsanis, Andreadis and Teperoglou(2018) observed a similar trend in Greece.

These findings, though not explicitly focused on the ideological correlates of populist attitudes, collectively hint at the existence of such dynamics. By synthesizing findings across these studies, a pattern emerges suggesting that populist attitudes may increase as ideological positions move toward either extreme of the left-right spectrum, pointing to a potential nonlinear relationship.

Building on the suggestion of a potential nonlinear relationship between populist attitudes and ideologies at the individual level, similar patterns emerge when examining populism from the perspective of its suppliers, such as populist parties and their ideological positions. Although these studies do not directly address individual-level populist attitudes, they provide critical insights into how the ideological orientations of parties relate to populism, frequently identifying nonlinear, U-shaped patterns that highlight the role of ideological extremity in shaping populist dynamics.

For instance, Rooduijn and Akkerman Reference Rooduijn and Akkerman(2017) analyzed election manifestos of 32 parties across five Western European countries and found that a party’s leaning toward the right or left did not significantly influence its populism. Instead, the radicalness of a party was a more significant factor, as both far-left and far-right parties commonly propagate the populist message that corrupt elites neglect the interests of ordinary people. Similarly, Meijers and Zaslove (Reference Meijers and Zaslove2021), in their elite survey of 250 political parties, found higher populist scores among parties with extreme left or right ideological tendencies. Their OLS regression analysis revealed correlations between leftist economic policies and rightist agendas, such as nativism and anti-EU integration, with the likelihood of being a populist party. Thus, Meijers and Zaslove Reference Meijers and Zaslove(2021) concluded that there is a U-shaped relationship between populist parties and ideologies. This aligns with Castanho Silva Reference Castanho Silva(2017), who, after analyzing manifestos of 92 parties across 14 Western European countries, found that populism is generally associated with extremism, not specifically with left or right politics. These discourses collectively hint at a potential relationship between populism itself and ideological radicalism on the supply side.

As mentioned, the papers discussed above predominantly focus on populism from the supply-side perspective, specifically on the relationship between parties and party ideologies. While research on populist attitudes has surged, and studies have explored the nature of these attitudes, there remains a lack of dedicated research on the demand level, specifically on the correlation between populist attitudes and individual ideologies, to the extent that party and party ideologies have been examined.

The most relevant research to date has predominantly focused on the broader nature of populist attitudes, offering valuable hints about their potential nonlinear relationship with ideology across various contexts, even if not explicitly examining this interplay. For example, Hawkins et al. Reference Hawkins, Riding and Mudde2012 utilized CCES and UCEP data and found that conservatives are more populist than liberals, with strong conservatives/liberals being more populist than their moderate counterparts. However, their study’s focus was more on quantitatively capturing populist attitudes and descriptively comparing groups of individual characteristics represented as binary variables. It did not delve deeply into this nonlinearity, and while it highlighted the association between populism and strong conservatism, it still hinted at a link between high populist attitudes and ideological extremism. Similarly, Bernhard and Hänggli Reference Bernhard and Hänggli(2018), using Switzerland as a case study, hypothesized a U-shaped relationship between populist attitudes and ideologies, but their findings suggested a W-shape, increasingly skewed to the right. In line with this, Tsatsanis et al. Reference Tsatsanis, Andreadis and Teperoglou(2018), who also anticipated a U-shape, found in their analysis of Greece that the trend generally declined in a linear fashion toward the left, with a slight rise in populist attitudes at the far right end of the spectrum. This suggests a linearity skewed to the left, resembling a U-shape.

Hence, existing research underscores the complexity of the relationship between populist attitudes and ideologies, suggesting that this dynamic may not be straightforward. Studies focusing on individual countries or specific regions have hinted that populist attitudes tend to increase toward the ideological extremes of both the left and right. Similarly, research on populist parties has suggested U-shaped patterns, with extreme ideological positions correlating with higher populism on the party level. However, few studies have systematically tested this relationship across national contexts, leaving a critical gap in understanding. Against this backdrop, this paper seeks to address the critical gap by systematically examining the relationship between ideological orientations and populist attitudes across diverse national contexts, with particular attention to their potential nonlinear dynamics.

3. Ideological extremism and populist attitudes

Building on the premise that the relationship between ideological orientations and populist attitudes is multifaceted and potentially nonlinear, it becomes imperative to examine how individuals situated at the ideological extremes engage with populist sentiments. Specifically, this raises critical questions: How do individuals positioned at the ideological extremes, on both the left and the right, manifest their relationship with populist attitudes? Moreover, do these ideological extremists, regardless of their alignment, share common traits that make them particularly susceptible to populist narratives?

Existing literature on political extremism highlights distinctive characteristics among individuals with extremist tendencies compared to those without. According to Van Prooijen and Krouwel Reference Van Prooijen and Krouwel(2019), ideological extremists are defined as those “strongly identifying with generic left- or right-wing ideological outlooks on society” (p. 159). To study these extremists, political psychology studies, for instance, have shown that ideological extremes are often associated with specific psychological traits (Krouwel et al., Reference Krouwel, Kutiyski, Van Prooijen, Martinsson and Markstedt2017). Regardless of their position on the left or right, these ideological extremists share certain common psychological characteristics that mark their extremism, suggesting a cross-ideological psychological profile (Greenberg and Jonas, Reference Greenberg and Jonas2003; Kruglanski et al., Reference Kruglanski, Gelfand, Bélanger, Sheveland, Hetiarachchi and Gunaratna2014; Van Prooijen and Krowel, 2017, Reference Van Prooijen and Krouwel2019). For example, whether on the far left or the far right, individuals with extreme ideological positions tend to reject opposing beliefs and perceive any differing ideology as inferior (Van Prooijen and Krowel, 2017, Reference Van Prooijen and Krouwel2019). Additionally, Zmigrod et al. Reference Zmigrod, Rentfrow and Robbins(2020) found that individuals at both ends of the ideological spectrum exhibit cognitive inflexibility, further reinforcing the similarities in psychological rigidity among extremists on either side.

Why, then, do ideologically extreme individuals, who exhibit these distinctive characteristics, display heightened populist attitudes? This observation invites further inquiry into why individuals at the ideological extremes, characterized by these distinctive traits, may be particularly drawn to populist attitudes. First, ideological extremism inherently aligns with populism’s Manichean worldview, characterized fundamentally by a cognitive division of society into clearly defined in-groups and out-groups. According to social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979), ideological extremism involves an identity-driven cognitive process wherein individuals distinctly categorize political and social groups based primarily on salient group identities. Such cognitive categorization is flexible, allowing the defining characteristics of these groups to shift depending on context (Berger, Reference Berger2018). Within this framework, in-group loyalty and perceived homogeneity become central cognitive elements, facilitating a simplified worldview structured around binary divisions (Van Hiel, Reference Van Hiel2012). When applied politically, these cognitive divisions encourage extreme ideologues to interpret social and political phenomena through an “us versus them” schema (Jost et al., Reference Jost, Baldassarri and Druckman2022), thus providing fertile cognitive grounds for populist reasoning, especially its dualistic worldview.

Second, ideologically extreme individuals with a Manichean worldview often exhibit this perspective through traits of oversimplification and overconfidence, which reinforce populist mindsets. Studies suggest that those with extremist ideologies have a pronounced tendency to oversimplify complex issues, approaching them with an epistemological rigidity that renders them more susceptible to binary thinking (Greenberg and Jonas, Reference Greenberg and Jonas2003; Kruglanski et al., Reference Kruglanski, Pierro, Mannetti and De Grada2006; Webber et al., Reference Webber, Babush, Schori-Eyal, Vazeou-Nieuwenhuis, Hettiarachchi, Bélanger, Moyano, Trujillo, Gunaratna and Kruglanski2018; Van Prooijen and Krouwel, Reference Van Prooijen and Krouwel2019). As a result, these individuals are more likely to categorize the world in homogenous, overly simplistic terms Lammers et al. Reference Lammers, Koch, Conway and Brandt(2017). This extreme simplification further entrenches their overconfidence (Van Prooijen and Krouwel, 2020), reinforcing their rigid worldview and solidifying their adherence to a fixed ideological perspective. Consequently, this entrenched, oversimplified outlook not only facilitates but also amplifies their populist, dualistic perspective on society and politics.

Third, the association between ideological extremism and populist attitudes emerges from the alignment of extremists’ preferences for direct participation in governance with the core tenets of populist thinking. Ideological extremists often reject the principles of representative democracy, favoring participatory democracy as a more authentic reflection of the people’s will (Wojcieszak, Reference Wojcieszak2014). This preference underpins their endorsement of people-centrism, as they perceive direct citizen involvement as a means to bypass the perceived inefficiencies or corruption of representative structures. Consequently, this alignment amplifies the populist notion that “the people” should hold direct power, reinforcing the connection between ideological extremism and populist attitudes.

Fourth, research on ideological extremism underscores a strong correlation between extreme ideological positions, low levels of political trust, and cognitive rigidity, all of which contribute to the adoption of a populist mindset. According to Kutiyski et al. Reference Kutiyski, Krouwel and van Prooijen(2021), individuals with rigid ideological beliefs often engage in black-and-white thinking, which provides simplified and intuitive solutions to complex societal issues (p. 2). This cognitive inflexibility fosters a closed-minded perspective, characterized by an “us vs. them” mentality that perpetuates distrust of others, including political elites. Such distrust, a defining feature of anti-elitism, is further reinforced by extremists’ rejection of representative structures and their tendency to view the world through overly simplistic and oppositional frameworks. These traits collectively intensify the populist sentiment among ideologically extreme individuals, anchoring their opposition to perceived elite dominance.

Lastly, ideologically extreme individuals not only adopt a cognitive “us versus them” worldview but also exhibit pronounced affective and behavioral hostility toward political opponents. Empirical research consistently documents that ideological extremists harbor higher levels of emotional animosity toward opposing parties compared to their own (Schmitt and Holmberg, Reference Schmitt, Holmberg, Klingemann and Fuchs1995; Berglund et al., Reference Berglund, Holmberg, Schmitt, Thomassen and Thomassen2005). Furthermore, their characteristic overconfidence leads to active rejection and intolerance of differing ideological perspectives (Van Prooijen and Krouwel, 2017, Reference Van Prooijen and Krouwel2019). Unlike the purely cognitive categorization described previously, this affective dimension manifests in explicit hostility, intolerance, and a refusal to engage constructively with opposing groups, thereby intensifying populist sentiments such as anti-elitism and people-centrism. Thus, ideological extremism not only promotes binary cognitive framing but also significantly enhances negative emotional and behavioral reactions toward opponents, amplifying populist attitudes. Consequently, we expect that ideological extremism significantly influences higher populist attitudes. However, this is not to suggest a causal relationship where ideology directly produces populist attitudes; rather, it indicates that individuals with extreme ideological views are more likely to exhibit populist behaviors.

Hence, in line with our hypothesis of a nonlinear relationship between political ideology and populist attitudes, we posit that there exists an exponential nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes. This extended framework anticipates that as ideological extremism intensifies, the association with populist attitudes becomes disproportionately stronger, further supporting a U-shaped model in which ideological extremes correspond with heightened populist sentiments.

Hypothesis 1. There is a nonlinear relationship between populist attitudes and political ideology.

4. Data

In this study, we utilize the cross-national survey data from the fifth wave of the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems (CSES), conducted from 2016 to 2021. The data encompasses face-to-face surveys conducted in 43 nations, spanning 53 elections,Footnote 1 and based on a random sample of respondents aged 16 and older. The data is representative of each country’s national voting-age population on gender, age, education level, and working sector. Detailed descriptive statistics for demographic characteristics are presented in Table A.1 of the Supplementary Material.

4.1. Populist attitudes

To test our hypothesis, we first construct a scale to measure populist attitudes. Recognizing populism as a multidimensional construct, we employ the populist/anti-establishment attitudes battery from the CSES, developed by Hobolt Reference Hobolt(2016). This scale comprises five items,Footnote 2 assessed on a 1 to 5 Likert-type scale (ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”Footnote 3). Each item reflects distinct dimensions of populism, with some items addressing multiple dimensions. Table 1 presents the original item wording alongside descriptive statistics for our sample.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the populist attitudes variables

Note: Total N = 69,355. All questions were originally asked on a 1-5 strongly agree-strongly disagree scale and have further been reverse-coded so that 1-5 means strongly disagree-strongly agree, with higher values denoting higher populism.

Next, we perform a one-factor Multiple Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MGCFA). Despite the diverse range of national and electoral contexts in our study, we implement a metric-invariant model to ensure consistency in factor loadings across countries.Footnote 4 Details on the full test for metric invariance, as well as factor loadings, are provided in Appendix B.

Our final populist attitudes scale comprises MC1, AE1, and PC1. “MC1” captures the Manichaean view of politics, while “PC1” reflects the belief in the “general will of the people.” To prevent the overrepresentation of anti-elitism or anti-establishment attitudes, which are assessed by three distinct items (“AE”), we opt for a single indicator to control for anti-elitism rather than using multiple items (Hayduk and Littvay, Reference Hayduk and Littvay2012). Specifically, we use “AE1,” as it holistically encapsulates anti-elite sentiment by explicitly linking elite misbehavior to the people.

Finally, we operationalize populist attitudes as an additive index, adopting a compensatory approach. By averaging respondents’ answers to the three selected items, we allow higher values in one dimension to offset lower values in another. Although this method lacks the stringent rigor of alternative scales, such as those proposed by Wuttke et al. Reference Wuttke, Schimpf and Schoen(2020) and Castanho Silva et al. Reference Silva, Bruno and Ryô Tamaki(2022), it produces results comparable to widely used scales, such as those developed by Akkerman et al. Reference Akkerman, Mudde and Zaslove(2014) and Schulz et al. Reference Schulz, Müller, Schemer, Wirz, Wettstein and Wirth(2018), with high correlations (r > 0.80). Thus, it serves as a valid measure for the purposes of our analysis.

4.2. Ideology and ideological extremism

We then turn to our primary independent variables: ideology and ideological extremism. Ideology is measured by respondents’ self-placement on the conventional left-right ideological scale (ranging from 0 to 10, left to right). To measure ideological extremism, we adopt the approach outlined by Torcal and Magalhães Reference Torcal and Magalhães(2022), which calculates the absolute difference between each respondent’s left-right self-placement and the average placement of respondents in the same country/election. Essentially, we define [ideological] extremism as:

\begin{equation*} Ext = |(Ideo_{ij} - \overline{Ideo_{j}})| \end{equation*}

where Ideoij denotes the left-right (0-10) ideological position of each individual i from country/election j, and $\overline{Ideo_{j}}$ the country j’s average individual ideological self-placement.

As previously discussed, this study focuses on the relationship between political ideology and populist attitudes. To ensure a more precise analysis, we also consider an additional factor: politically neutral moderates often lack a complete understanding of their political ideology compared to those with more defined political stances. Therefore, in exploring the relationship between political ideology and populist attitudes, we exclude individuals who identify as completely politically neutral to ensure a more accurate analysis of this relationship. This approach is informed by research such as Freeder et al. Reference Freeder, Lenz and Turney(2019), which highlights the lack of stable policy views among politically uninformed individuals, and Kinder and Kalmoe Reference Kinder and Kalmoe2017, who describe moderates as politically innocent and ideologically indifferent.Footnote 5

To test our hypothesis, we employ GAMs. Given our expectation of a nonlinear relationship between populist attitudes and ideology at the individual level, GAMs are particularly well-suited, as they allow for the estimation of smooth, nonparametric functional forms rather than imposing linear constraints (Pedersen et al., Reference Pedersen, Miller, Simpson and Ross2019). To test the robustness of our findings, we also utilize OLS regression models.

The development of our models followed an incremental approach. We began with simple univariate models to account for potential suppression effects (Pandey and Elliott, Reference Pandey and Elliott2010; Lenz and Sahn, Reference Lenz and Sahn2021). Subsequently, we gradually introduced exogenous variables, including sociodemographic controls and confounders. Throughout this process, we assessed improvements in model fit at each step. For conciseness, we present only the final models.Footnote 6

In addition to standard sociodemographic controls (e.g., Rovira Kaltwasser and Van Hauwaert, Reference Kaltwasser and Van Hauwaert2020), our models also incorporate individuals’ perceptions of corruption and the economy. These variables are essential for capturing the potential influence of contextual crises, such as widespread corruption and economic recession (e.g., Hawkins et al., 2020), on populist attitudes. Emphasizing the subjective nature of crises Tamaki et al. Reference Tamaki, Silva, Hawkins and Littvay(forthcoming), our approach prioritizes individuals’ subjective perceptions over broader contextual factors.Footnote 7

Finally, given the nested nature of our data, we adopted a fixed-effects approach. By incorporating fixed effects for countries and elections, we account for clustering effects and reduce omitted variable bias at the country and election levels (Allison, Reference Allison2009; Huang, Reference Huang2016).

5. Results

Starting with ideology, Model 1 in Table 2 presents the GAM results for the 52 countries/elections. The table is structured in two sections: the first presents the effective degrees of freedom (edf) of our smoothed term, and the second lists the conventional parametric terms, which in this case pertain to our control variables. Our primary focus is on the smoothed term for this analysis.

Table 2. Individual ideology and populist attitudes (generalized additive models with fixed effects)

Odds w/ 95% CIs in (parentheses). +p < 0.1; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. For both Model 1 and Re-estimated Model 1, concurvities levels for ideology are below 0.17 (worst case). In both models (p > 0.05), we cannot reject the null hypothesis that residuals are randomly distributed.

In a GAM, the edf measures the complexity of the smoothing function applied to model a nonlinear relationship. An edf of 1 indicates a linear relationship, while higher edf values suggest increasingly complex, nonlinear relationships.

Model 1 reveals an edf of 8.063 for ideology, significant at a p-value < 0.05. This result supports rejecting the null hypothesis of a linear relationship between individual ideology and populist attitudes. To further explore this relationship, we illustrate the smooth component of ideology in Figure 1. As hypothesized in H1, the relationship between individual ideology and populist attitudes is nonlinear. The observed “U” shaped pattern indicates that individuals identifying as either far left or far right exhibit higher levels of populist attitudes compared to those closer to the center. However, some caveats must be acknowledged.

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideology and populist attitudes from Model 1; 95%CIs.

Figure 1. Model 1 - populist attitudes and ideology

Initially, we observe a slight increase in populist attitudes near the center of the ideological scale, which may suggest a more complex relationship. However, upon further investigation, we attribute this to uninformed voters who tend to position themselves at the center of the scale, even when their socioeconomic positions suggest otherwise (Iversen and Soskice, Reference Iversen and Soskice2015: 1807). As Torcal and Magalhães (2022) argue, this pattern reflects low levels of political sophistication rather than true ideological moderation. Similarly, as noted above, Freeder, Lenz, and Turney (2019) highlight that politically uninformed individuals often lack stable policy preferences, and Kinder and Kalmoe (2017) describe moderates as politically innocent and ideologically indifferent.

Our initial assumption posits that centrist positioning on the ideological scale reflects a lack of political knowledge and/or apathy rather than genuine ideological moderation. To substantiate this, we employed the CSES variable on political interestFootnote 8 as a proxy for political knowledge, an approach recommended by Gidengil and Zechmeister Reference Gidengil and Zechmeister(2016) during the development of the 5th module of the CSES. Although not a perfect substitute, political interest is a robust predictor of political knowledge, making it a viable alternative.

We then compared the levels of political interest between respondents who positioned themselves at the neutral point of the ideological scale (5) and the rest of our sample. The results revealed that the lack of political interest is significantly higherFootnote 9 among those who identified as centrists. Additionally, we also examined the distribution of politically apathetic individuals across countries and ideological positions. On average, the majority of those with no interest in politics clustered at the midpoint (5) on the 0-to-10 ideological scale, accounting for approximately 23.8% of politically disinterested respondents. These findings challenge the notion of ideological moderation and provide further validity for our initial assumptions.

Consequently, we re-estimated Model 1, this time excluding respondents who positioned themselves at the exact center on the ideological scale (5) and who also demonstrated no interest in politics. The results in Table 2 and Figure 2 reinforce our initial hypothesis.Footnote 10.

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideology and populist attitudes from Re-estimate Model 1; 95%CIs.

Figure 2. Re-estimated model 1 - populist attitudes and ideology

The refined Model 1 offers a deeper understanding of this dynamic. While the “U”-shaped pattern remains, it now displays a more consistent trend as one moves further from the center in either direction. Notably, Figure 2 reveals a subtle asymmetry in which populist attitudes are slightly more pronounced among right-wing identifiers. This pattern suggests that, even within a broadly nonlinear relationship, there may be modest differences in how populist attitudes are expressed at the ideological extremes. As our robustness tests further demonstrate, this asymmetry is conditioned by the supply-side context of populism within each political system. The psychological mechanisms linking extremism to populist attitudes appear to be moderated by the presence and ideological orientation of populist parties, with right-wing extremism more strongly associated with populist attitudes in contexts where right-wing populist parties predominate

Transitioning to ideological extremism, our findings in Model 2, as shown in Table 3, indicate a nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes (edf = 7.272, p < 0.05). Similar to the analysis of ideology, Figure 3 unveils complexities in the relationship that surpass our initial expectations.

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes from Model 2; 95%CIs.

Figure 3. Model 2: populist attitudes and ideological extremism

Table 3. Ideological extremism and populist attitudes (generalized additive models with fixed effects)

Odds w/ 95% CIs in (parentheses). +p < 0.1; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. For both Model 2 and Re-estimated Model 2, concurvities levels for extremism are below 0.20 (worst case). In both models (p > 0.05), we cannot reject the null hypothesis that residuals are randomly distributed.

Initial observations indicate a noticeable increase in populist attitudes among individuals identified as holding moderate views (ideological extremism of 0). Furthermore, it appears that the impact of extremism on populist attitudes reaches its limit among those scoring five or higher on the ideological extremism scale; beyond this point, the influence of extremism on populist attitudes diminishes. However, upon closer examination, we attribute these trends mainly to (i) individuals inaccurately self-identifying as ideologically centrist, as previously discussed, and (ii) the constraints imposed by the limited number of observations at the high levels of extremism (see the rug plot at the bottom of Figure 3 and the confidence intervals around x-axis 5 to 7). To address these issues, following the same strategy as above, we excluded respondents who positioned themselves at the neutral point of the ideological scale (5) and who are not interested in politics.Footnote 11 The re-estimated model yields a more discernible pattern, as illustrated by the re-estimated Model 2 in Table 3 and Figure 4.Footnote 12

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes from Re-estimate Model 2; 95%CIs.

Figure 4. Re-estimated model 2: populist attitudes and ideological extremism

The association between ideological extremism and populist attitudes is now more consistent with the outcomes of the re-estimated Model 1. As individuals radicalize, their populist attitudes tend to intensify. However, this escalation manifests solely among individuals exhibiting moderate to high levels of extremism (relative to the mean position within their country); only in these cases does extremism correlate with elevated levels of populist attitudes.

In summary, our analyses compel us to reject the null hypotheses pertaining to the effects of H1. Specifically, we observe a notable upsurge in populist attitudes at both extremes of the ideological spectrum, irrespective of their left or right orientation. When examining ideological extremism, the relationship is as expected. While the overall association between individual ideology and populism resembles a “U” shape, with heightened levels of populist attitudes at both ends of the spectrum, a closer examination of these extremes reveals a somewhat exponential rise in populist attitudes for each increment in ideological extremism, discernible solely among those possessing at least average levels of ideological extremism.

6. Robustness tests

To validate the primary findings of our preceding analysis, we turn to alternative models that adhere to more conventional approaches. First, we replicate the Re-estimated Model 1, which initially provided supportive evidence for Hypothesis 1. To ensure the robustness of our results, we employ Ordinary Least Squares models, estimating distinct models to further examine the nonlinear relationship between ideology and populist attitudes. Model 1.2 assumes linearity in the ideology-populist attitudes relationship, while Model 1.3 introduces a quadratic polynomial term to capture a U-shaped curve. While Model 1.2 tests the linearity assumption, Model 1.3 serves as an alternative to the GAM. Theoretically, the quadratic transformation in Model 1.3 should better fit the data if individuals at both ideological extremes, left and right, exhibit higher populist attitudes than those at the center. Detailed results are presented in Supplementary Material E.1.

Table 4 compares Re-estimated Model 1 with Re-estimated Models 1.2 and 1.3. The results highlight notable differences in model fit, as indicated by the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) values. The GAM achieves a lower AIC (120536.8) and BIC (121126) compared to the linear and quadratic polynomial OLS models, reinforcing the nonlinear relationship initially suggested by Re-estimated Model 1.

Table 4. AIC & bic comparison - ideology

Using a similar approach, we extend our analysis to the Re-estimated Model 2. Initial findings suggest that the relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes exhibits features of an exponential curve while also demonstrating linear elements, as illustrated in Figure 4. To ensure robustness, we construct two additional models, with results presented in Supplementary Material E.2. Re-estimated Model 2.2 assumes a linear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes, while Re-estimated Model 2.3 incorporates an exponential term for extremism. Table 5 compares their AIC and BIC values.

Table 5. AIC & bic comparison - ideological extremism

The findings are twofold. While the GAM yields the lowest AIC (120646.7), the linear OLS model has the lowest BIC (121292.9). This reflects the inherent trade-off between the two criteria: AIC prioritizes model fit, whereas BIC emphasizes parsimony (Claeskens, Reference Claeskens2016), particularly penalizing models with larger sample sizes. These results suggest that while GAM provides a better overall fit to the data, the linear OLS model is more parsimonious. Ultimately, as anticipated, both models can be deemed acceptable, with the choice depending on the researcher’s priorities and the specific nature of the data.

Next, we examine distinct strategies for operationalizing our populist attitudes variable, ensuring the robustness of our findings against potential biases arising from operationalization choices. Departing from the notion that populism, and consequently populist attitudes, comprises a configurable concept that necessitates the presence of all dimensions in a non-compensatory manner Castanho Silva et al. (Reference Silva, Bruno and Littvay2017, as argued by), we explore two alternative approaches. Initially, we adopt a multiplicative index, as advocated by Castanho Silva et al. Reference Silva, Bruno and Ryô Tamaki(2022). This method involves normalizing the items on a 0-1 scale before computing their product, ensuring that elevated levels on the populist attitudes scale exclusively encompass individuals who consistently score high across each distinct dimension (Manichaeism, Anti-elitism, and People-centrism).

Subsequently, we implement what Wuttke et al. Reference Wuttke, Schimpf and Schoen(2020) refer to as the “Goertz” approach. Unlike the previous method, this approach utilizes the minimum response among the three items to ascertain that only respondents exhibiting high scores across all populist dimensions are categorized as populists. Supplementary Materials E.3. and D.4. present the results of GAMs following both Re-estimated Models 1 and 2. The findings remain consistent with those of previous models, as evidenced by the edfs greater than 1 and significance levels of p-value < 0.05.

To further validate our findings, we test an alternative specification of the populist attitudes scale comprising only people-centrism and anti-elitism dimensions. This additional test addresses potential concerns regarding conceptual overlap between the Manichaean dimension and ideological extremism. While our empirical analysis already demonstrates a minimal relationship between these constructs (Spearman’s ρ = 0.10), removing the Manichean item (MC1) from our scale allows us to explicitly test whether the observed relationships between ideological positions and populist attitudes persist independently of any potential confounding effects. Results from GAMs following our Re-estimated Models 1 and 2 (presented in Supplementary Material E.5) demonstrate remarkable consistency with our main findings, further underscoring the robustness of our conclusions.

As an additional robustness check, we incorporate socioeconomic status as a control variable. Although previous research indicates socioeconomic factors may influence populist attitudes e.g., Rico and Anduiza Reference Rico and Anduiza(2019), including these variables reduces our sample by 13 countries due to missing data in the fifth wave of the CSES. To preserve comprehensive cross-national coverage of our primary analysis while still testing for socioeconomic effects, we present these supplementary models in Material E.6. The results once again confirm the stability of our findings, with no substantial changes to our core conclusions regarding the relationship between ideology and populist attitudes.

Finally, to address potential contextual variation in the relationship between populist attitudes and ideology, we conduct an additional analysis that incorporates the supply-side dynamics of populism. Drawing on data from the V-Party (Lindberg et al., Reference Lindberg, Düpont, Higashijima, Kavasoglu, Marquardt, Bernhard, Döring, Hicken, Laebens, Medzihorsky, Neundorf, Reuter, Ruth–Lovell, Weghorst, Wiesehomeier, Wright, Alizada, Bederke, Gastaldi, Grahn, Hindle, Ilchenko, von Römer, Wilson, Pemstein and Seim2022), we classify countries based on the ideological orientation of their populist parties. We first identify populist parties using a threshold score of 0.5 on V-Party’s populism measure - a relatively inclusive criterion that allows us to capture a broad range of populist actors. We then categorize countries according to the ideological distribution of their populist parties using V-Party’s left-right scores: countries with exclusively right-wing populist parties are labeled as “right,” those with only left-wing populist parties as “left,” those with centrist populist parties as “center,” and countries featuring populist parties spanning multiple ideological positions as “diverse.” We conduct a sub-group analysis by dividing the sample based on these classifications and fitting GAMs to each subgroup. This approach allows us to investigate whether the relationship between individual-level ideology and populist attitudes systematically varies according to the ideological orientation of populist supply in each country, thereby addressing how political context conditions the interplay between citizens’ ideological predispositions and their populist attitudes.

The sub-group analysis, presented in Table 6 and Figure 5, reinforces our primary findings regarding the nonlinear relationship between ideology and populist attitudes. Across varying supply-side configurations, the data consistently exhibit nonlinear patterns in the relationship between ideological positions and populist attitudes. In countries where right-wing populist parties predominate, the U-shaped pattern is especially pronounced (edf = 5.115, p < 0.05), with respondents on the right reporting generally higher levels of populist attitudes. In contrast, this asymmetry notably diminishes in contexts where the populist supply is exclusively left-wing, though it remains nonlinear (edf = 3.402, p < 0.05). Systems featuring ideologically diverse populist actors reveal more complex patterns (edf = 6.489, p < 0.05), yet they uphold the fundamental nonlinear relationship identified in our analysis.

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes from Model 2; 95%CIs.

Figure 5. Sub-group analysis: Populist attitudes and ideology

Table 6. Sub-group analysis - ideology and populist attitudes (generalized additive models with fixed effects)

Coefficients with 95% CIs in (parentheses). * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001 All models include country fixed effects. Concurvities levels for extremism are below 0.20 (worst case). We cannot reject the null hypothesis that residuals are randomly distributed (p > 0.05).

7. Discussion and conclusion

This study presents a nuanced exploration into the relationship between populist attitudes and political ideologies, advancing our understanding of populism from both ideational and empirical perspectives. Our analysis, based on cross-national data from the fifth wave of the Comparative Study of Electoral Systems, reveals intricate patterns in how populist attitudes are associated with ideological orientations.

Our findings highlight a significant nonlinear relationship between populist attitudes and ideological positions. This complexity is evident in the -shaped pattern observed in the association between individual ideology and populism, with heightened levels of populist attitudes at both ideological extremes. The analysis employing GAMs robustly supports our initial hypothesis that the relationship is nonlinear, thus rejecting a simplistic linear interpretation.

Further, the study delves into the nuances of ideological extremism and its correlation with populist attitudes. Here, our results confirm our second hypothesis, indicating that higher degrees of ideological extremism, irrespective of whether they lean toward the extreme left or right, are associated with greater populist attitudes.

An important caveat emerges, however, when examining cross-national variation. While the nonlinear, U-shaped pattern is consistent across diverse political contexts, we observe a subtle asymmetry that is conditioned by the supply-side characteristics of each political system. In contexts dominated by right-wing populist parties, populist attitudes are more pronounced among right-leaning respondents, whereas this asymmetry diminishes in systems with left-wing or ideologically diverse populist actors. This contextual conditioning effect underscores the complex interplay between individual predispositions and political opportunity structures, suggesting that the psychological mechanisms linking extremism to populist attitudes are moderated by the available political expressions of populism within each system. Future research should further investigate these contextual dynamics to develop a more comprehensive understanding of how supply and demand factors interact in shaping populist sentiments across different political environments.

The robustness of our findings is further validated through alternative modeling approaches and operationalizations of the populist attitudes variable. Across various model specifications and operationalizations, the core insights remain consistent, affirming the nonlinear nature of the relationship between ideology and populist attitudes and the significant role of ideological extremism.

In light of these findings, our study makes three primary contributions to the field. First, it provides a comprehensive cross-national examination of the interplay between populist attitudes and political ideologies, addressing a critical gap in the literature. While existing studies have often examined populist attitudes within single-country or region-specific contexts, there has been a notable absence of research investigating how this relationship manifests across a diverse set of national settings. Our study is the first to systematically explore the extent to which the relationship between populist attitudes and political ideologies exhibits universal trends or country-specific variations. By utilizing cross-national data, we offer new insights into whether populist attitudes consistently correlate with ideological extremity across different political systems, cultures, and levels of democratic development, thereby enhancing the field’s understanding of populism as a global phenomenon.

Second, it applies methodologically rigorous approaches to uncover these patterns. Our study employs both Ordinary Least Squares regression and GAMs, which allow us to identify and robustly model nonlinear relationships that may be obscured by conventional linear approaches. By integrating these advanced methods, we provide a more precise and nuanced understanding of the relationship between populist attitudes and political ideologies, avoiding oversimplified interpretations. The use of multiple operationalizations of populist attitudes further ensures the reliability and validity of our findings, demonstrating their applicability across diverse measurement strategies.

Third, our research offers a novel conceptual contribution by highlighting the significant role of ideological extremism as a driving factor behind populist attitudes. While the ideological foundations of populism have been widely debated, few studies have explicitly focused on the intersection of ideological extremity and populist attitudes at the individual level. Our analysis reveals that ideological extremism, whether aligned with the far left or far right, intensifies the salience of populist attitudes, thereby elucidating the psychological and behavioral underpinnings of this relationship.

Nonetheless, this relationship operates within contextually bounded parameters. Our empirical findings demonstrate that the manifestation and intensity of this relationship vary systematically across different political configurations, particularly contingent upon the ideological composition of populist supply within each system. This contextual conditionality suggests that theoretical frameworks must integrate both psychological dispositions and institutional opportunity structures to fully explicate the dynamics of populist attitude formation. These nuanced insights not only advance theoretical debates on populism but also provide a foundation for future studies to investigate the broader implications of ideological polarization in shaping democratic processes and populist mobilization across diverse political environments.

Ultimately, this study not only fills a gap in the literature by addressing the intricate relationship between populist attitudes and political ideologies on a global scale but also opens new avenues for further research. While our focus has not been on establishing causal mechanisms, the findings presented here provide a foundational framework for subsequent studies to explore the dynamics of causality in greater depth. Future research could build on this pioneering work to investigate the causal pathways that link ideological extremity and populist attitudes, examining how these relationships evolve over time and across diverse political contexts.

Additionally, our findings invite further in-depth exploration of why these dynamics occur. By delving into the psychological, cultural, and structural factors underlying the observed relationships, subsequent studies could offer a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanisms at play. We hope that this study serves as a catalyst for such investigations, inspiring future scholarship to advance the field and address the complexities of populism and ideology with greater nuance. Understanding these dynamics is not only academically significant but also essential for addressing the broader implications of populism in shaping political behavior and democratic resilience.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2025.10045. To obtain replication material for this article, https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/BNIRXP.

Footnotes

1 The original number of countries included in the fifth wave of the CSES was 55. Some were dropped due to inconsistencies in data and measurement.

2 We excluded the item “Having a strong leader in government is good for [COUNTRY] even if the leader bends the rules to get things done,” based on findings by Castanho Silva et al. Reference Silva, Sebastian Jungkunz, Helbling and Littvay(2020), who noted that this item neither correlated nor loaded with the others. We observed a similar pattern in our data. Conceptually, this item aligns more closely with authoritarian preferences than populism, at least in its ideational conception.

We also excluded “Most politicians are trustworthy” due to its negative wording. Although retaining such items can mitigate acquiescence bias e.g., Castanho Silva et al. Reference Castanho Silva, Andreadis, Anduiza, Blanusa, Corti, Delfino, Rico, RuthLovell, Spruyt, Steenbergen, Littvay, Hawkins and Carlin2018, the questionnaire includes only one negatively worded item (for “anti-elitism”).

3 We reverse-coded the scale for interpretive clarity. In the revised scale, “1” represents “Strongly Disagree” and “5” represents “Strongly Agree.”

4 CFI = 0.94, TLI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.086 (90% CI: 0.084 - 0.089), SRMR = 0.064. Ultimately, we consider the good fit of the metric-invariant model to be enough for us to consider the scale’s invariance across countries.

5 In considering alternative measures of ideology for this analysis, it is important to clarify that no suitable variable exists in the CSES dataset that could replace self-placement on the left-right scale. Furthermore, our primary objective was to address individuals who positioned themselves exactly at the midpoint of the scale (5) due to concerns about their political sophistication. We specifically excluded them from our analysis to focus on the ideologically sophisticated respondents, for whom the relationship between ideology and populist attitudes is clearer.

6 Previous models are provided in Supplementary Material C.1.

7 While concerns about endogeneity in the relationship between corruption, economic perceptions, and populism are valid, prior research suggests that the causal pathway primarily runs from these issues to populist attitudes rather than the reverse. For further discussion, see Rico and Anduiza, Reference Rico and Anduiza2019.

8 Original wording: “How interested would you say you are in politics? Are you: (1) very interested, (2) somewhat interested, (3) not very interested, or (4) not at all interested?” We treat “not interested” as those who answered 3 (not very interested) and 4 (not at all interested).

9 Two-proportion Z-test.

10 We also tested political interest as a control variable, as suggested by the reviewers. The results were nearly identical to those of Model 1, which aligns with expectations since the splines, which would allow for varying effects of ideology across levels of political interest, were not interacted with. The full results are in Supplementary Material D.

11 Individuals self-positioning at 5 on the traditional left-right scale tend to occupy a middle ground that reflects an absence of clear political engagement or firm stances. This centrist positioning often corresponds to a lack of political knowledge or interest, as such individuals may not actively participate in political discourse, leading to their categorization as politically indifferent.

12 Similar to Model 1, we also tested political interest here as a control variable. The results were also nearly identical to those of Model 2, which aligns with expectations since the splines, which would allow for varying effects of ideological extremism across levels of political interest, were not interacted with. Results are in the Supplementary Material D.

References

Akkerman, A, Mudde, C and Zaslove, A (2014) How populist are the people? Measuring populist attitudes in voters. Comparative Political Studies 47, 13241353.10.1177/0010414013512600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, PD (2009) Fixed Effects Regression Models, Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE publications.10.4135/9781412993869CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aslanidis, P (2016) Is populism an ideology? A refutation and a new perspective. Political Studies 64, 88104.10.1111/1467-9248.12224Google Scholar
Barr, RR (2018) Populism as a Political Strategy. In Routledge Handbook of Global Populism. Carlos, de la Torre, pp.4456 London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315226446-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, MM, Hainmueller, J and Margalit, Y (2014) Preferences for international redistribution: The divide over the Eurozone bailouts. American Journal of Political Science 58, 835856.10.1111/ajps.12079CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, JM (2018) Extremism, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/11688.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berglund, F, Holmberg, S, Schmitt, H and Thomassen, J (2005) The European Voter: A Comparative Study of Modern Democracies Thomassen, Jacques 105140. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bernhard, L and Hänggli, R (2018) Who holds populist attitudes? Evidence from Switzerland. Swiss Political Science Review 24, 510524.10.1111/spsr.12326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canovan, M (2002) Taking Politics to the People: Populism as the Ideology of Democracy. Democracies and the Populist Challenge Mény, Yves, and Surel, Yves, New York: Springer.10.1057/9781403920072_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanho Silva, B, Andreadis, I, Anduiza, E, Blanusa, N, Corti, YM, Delfino, G, Rico, G, RuthLovell, SP, Spruyt, B, Steenbergen, M and Littvay, L (2018) Public opinion surveys: A new scale. In Hawkins, KA, and Carlin, RE (eds.) The Ideational Approach to Populism: Concept, Theory, and Analysis, London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Castanho Silva, C (2017) Contemporary Populism: Actors, Causes, and Consequences Across 28 Democracies. Budapest, Budapest: Central European University, PhD.Google Scholar
Claeskens, G (2016) Statistical Model Choice. Annual Review of Statistics and Its Application 3, 233256.10.1146/annurev-statistics-041715-033413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeder, S, Lenz, GS and Turney, S (2019) The importance of knowing “what goes with what”: Reinterpreting the evidence on policy attitude stability. The Journal of Politics 81, 274290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gidengil, E and Zechmeister, E (2016) The tough decision to remove political knowledge from the CSES module 5. Comparative Study of Electoral Systems (CSES) https://cses.org/data-download/cses-module-5-2016-2021/funding-sources-and-how-to-cite-module-5/.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J and Jonas, E (2003) Psychological motives and political orientation–the left, the right, and the rigid: comment on Jost et al., Psychological Bulletin 129, 376382.10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.376CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hainmueller, J and Hiscox, MJ (2007) Educated preferences: Explaining attitudes toward immigration in Europe. International Organization 61, 399442.10.1017/S0020818307070142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, KA, Riding, S and Mudde, C (2012) Measuring Populist Attitudes Committee on Concepts and Methods Working Paper Series 55 Mexico City: International Political Science Association, Committee on Concepts and Methods, 135.Google Scholar
Hayduk, LA and Littvay, L (2012) Should researchers use single indicators, best indicators, or multiple indicators in structural equation models?. BMC Medical Research Methodology 12, 117.10.1186/1471-2288-12-159CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobolt, SB (2016) The Brexit vote: A divided nation, a divided continent. Journal of European Public Policy 23, 12591277.10.1080/13501763.2016.1225785CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, FL (2016) Alternatives to multilevel modeling for the analysis of clustered data. The Journal of Experimental Education 84, 175196.10.1080/00220973.2014.952397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, RA, Jankowski, M and Wegscheider, C (2023) Explaining populist attitudes: The impact of policy discontent and representation. Politische Vierteljahresschrift 64, 133154.10.1007/s11615-022-00422-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunger, S and Paxton, F (2022) What’s in a buzzword? A systematic review of the state of populism research in political science. Political Science Research and Methods 10, 617633.10.1017/psrm.2021.44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R and Norris, P (2017) Trump and the populist authoritarian parties: The silent revolution in reverse. Perspectives on Politics 15, 443454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iversen, T and Soskice, D (2015) Information, inequality, and mass polarization: Ideology in advanced democracies. Comparative Political Studies 48, 17811813.10.1177/0010414015592643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jagers, J and Walgrave, S (2007) Populism as political communication style: An empirical study of political parties’ discourse in Belgium. European Journal of Political Research 46, 319345.10.1111/j.1475-6765.2006.00690.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, JT, Baldassarri, DS and Druckman, JN (2022) Cognitive–motivational mechanisms of political polarization in social-communicative contexts. Nature Reviews Psychology 1, 560576.10.1038/s44159-022-00093-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaltwasser, CR and Van Hauwaert, SM (2020) The populist citizen: Empirical evidence from Europe and Latin America. European Political Science Review 12, 118.10.1017/S1755773919000262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinder, DR and Kalmoe, NP (2017) Neither Liberal nor Conservative: Ideological Innocence in the American public, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriesi, H (2014) The populist challenge. West European Politics 37, 361378.10.1080/01402382.2014.887879CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kriesi, H, Grande, E, Lachat, R, Dolezal, M, Bornschier, S and Frey, T (2008) West European Politics in the Age of Globalization, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511790720CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krouwel, A, Kutiyski, Y, Van Prooijen, J-W, Martinsson, J and Markstedt, E (2017) Does extreme political ideology predict conspiracy beliefs, economic evaluations and political trust? Evidence from Sweden. Journal of Social and Political Psychology 5, 435462.10.5964/jspp.v5i2.745CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, AW, Gelfand, MJ, Bélanger, JJ, Sheveland, A, Hetiarachchi, M and Gunaratna, R (2014) The psychology of radicalization and deradicalization: How significance quest impacts violent extremism. Political Psychology 35, 6993.10.1111/pops.12163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, AW, Pierro, A, Mannetti, L and De Grada, E (2006) Groups as epistemic providers: need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. Psychological Review 113, 84.10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutiyski, Y, Krouwel, A and van Prooijen, J-W (2021) Political extremism and distrust: Does radical political orientation predict political distrust and negative attitudes towards European integration? The Social Science Journal 58, 116.10.1016/j.soscij.2019.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laclau, E (2005). In Populism and the Mirror of Democracy Panizza, Francisco (ed), 103114, London: Verso.Google Scholar
Lammers, J, Koch, A, Conway, P and Brandt, MJ (2017) The political domain appears simpler to the politically extreme than to political moderates. Social Psychological and Personality Science 8, 612622.10.1177/1948550616678456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenz, GS and Sahn, A (2021) Achieving statistical significance with control variables and without transparency. Political Analysis 29, 356369.10.1017/pan.2020.31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindberg, SI, Düpont, N, Higashijima, M, Kavasoglu, YB, Marquardt, KL, Bernhard, M, Döring, H, Hicken, A, Laebens, M, Medzihorsky, J, Neundorf, A, Reuter, OJ, Ruth–Lovell, S, Weghorst, KR, Wiesehomeier, N, Wright, J, Alizada, N, Bederke, P, Gastaldi, L, Grahn, S, Hindle, G, Ilchenko, N, von Römer, J, Wilson, S, Pemstein, D and Seim, B (2022) Codebook Varieties of Party Identity and Organization (V–Party) V2. https://doi.org/10.23696/vpartydsv2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcos-Marne, H, Plaza-Colodro, C and Hawkins, KA (2020) Is populism the third dimension? The quest for political alliances in post-crisis Spain. Electoral Studies 63, 102112.10.1016/j.electstud.2019.102112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meijers, MJ and Zaslove, A (2021) Measuring Populism in Political Parties: Appraisal of a New Approach. Comparative Political Studies, 54, SAGE Publications Inc, Publisher, . https://doi.org/10.1177/0010414020938081.Google Scholar
Mény, Y and Surel, Y (2001) Democracies and the Populist Challenge, New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Moffitt, B (2016) The Global Rise of Populism: Performance, Political Style, and Representation, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Moffitt, B and Tormey, S (2014) Rethinking populism: Politics, mediatisation and political style. Political Studies 62, 381397.10.1111/1467-9248.12032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mudde, C (2004) The populist zeitgeist. Government and Opposition 39, 541563.10.1111/j.1477-7053.2004.00135.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pandey, S and Elliott, W (2010) Suppressor variables in social work research: Ways to identify in multiple regression models. Journal of the Society for Social Work and Research 1, 2840.10.5243/jsswr.2010.2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, EJ, Miller, DL, Simpson, GL and Ross, N (2019) Hierarchical generalized additive models in ecology: an introduction with mgcv. PeerJ 7, e6876.10.7717/peerj.6876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rico, G and Anduiza, E (2019) Economic correlates of populist attitudes: an analysis of nine European countries in the aftermath of the great recession. Acta Politica 54, 371397.10.1057/s41269-017-0068-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rico, G, Guinjoan, M and Anduiza, E (2017) The emotional underpinnings of populism: How anger and fear affect populist attitudes. Swiss Political Science Review 23, 444461.10.1111/spsr.12261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodrik, D (2018) Populism and the economics of globalization. Journal of International Business Policy 1, 1233.10.1057/s42214-018-0001-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooduijn, M and Akkerman, T (2017) Flank attacks: Populism and left-right radicalism in Western Europe. Party Politics 23, 193204.10.1177/1354068815596514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, H and Holmberg, S (1995) Political Parties in Decline? In Klingemann, H-D Fuchs, D (eds.) Citizens and the State, Oxford University Press, Oxford, .Google Scholar
Schulz, A, Müller, P, Schemer, C, Wirz, DS, Wettstein, M and Wirth, W (2018) Measuring populist attitudes on three dimensions. International Journal of Public Opinion Research 30, 316326.10.1093/ijpor/edw037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, C, Bruno, FV and Littvay, L (2017) The elite is up to something: Exploring the relation between populism and belief in conspiracy theories. Swiss Political Science Review 23, 423443.10.1111/spsr.12270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, C, Bruno, MF and Ryô Tamaki, E (2022) So thin it’s almost invisible: Populist attitudes and voting behavior in Brazil. Electoral Studies 75, 102434.10.1016/j.electstud.2021.102434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, C, Sebastian Jungkunz, B, Helbling, M and Littvay, L (2020) An empirical comparison of seven populist attitudes scales. Political Research Quarterly 73, 409424.10.1177/1065912919833176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanley, B (2008) The thin ideology of populism. Journal of Political Ideologies 13, 95110.10.1080/13569310701822289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taggart, P (2002) Populism and the pathology of representative politics. In Democracies and the Populist Challenge Mény, Yves, and Surel, Yves (eds), New York: Springer, .10.1057/9781403920072_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H and Turner, JC (1979) An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Conflict. In Austin, WG Worchel, S (eds.) The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations, Brooks/Cole, Monterey, CA, .Google Scholar
Tamaki, ER, Silva, BC, Hawkins, K and Littvay, L (forthcoming) Contextual Determinants of Populist Attitudes’ Activation Across 55 Countries.Google Scholar
Torcal, M and Magalhães, PC (2022) Ideological extremism, perceived party system polarization, and support for democracy. European Political Science Review 14, 188205.10.1017/S1755773922000066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsatsanis, E, Andreadis, I and Teperoglou, E (2018) Populism from below: Socio-economic and ideological correlates of mass attitudes in Greece. South European Society and Politics 23, 429450.10.1080/13608746.2018.1510635CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Urbinati, N (2019) Political theory of populism. Annual Review of Political Science 22, 111127.10.1146/annurev-polisci-050317-070753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hiel, Alain (2012) A psycho-political profile of party activists and left-wing and right-wing extremists. European Journal of Political Research 51, 166203.10.1111/j.1475-6765.2011.01991.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J-W and Krouwel, APM (2019) Psychological features of extreme political ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science 28, 159163.10.1177/0963721418817755CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webber, D, Babush, M, Schori-Eyal, N, Vazeou-Nieuwenhuis, A, Hettiarachchi, M, Bélanger, JJ, Moyano, M, Trujillo, HM, Gunaratna, R and Kruglanski, AW et al. (2018) The road to extremism: Field and experimental evidence that significance loss-induced need for closure fosters radicalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 114, 270.10.1037/pspi0000111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weyland, K (2001) Clarifying a contested concept: Populism in the study of Latin American politics. Comparative Politics 34, 122.10.2307/422412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weyland, K (2017) A Political-Strategic Approach. In The Oxford Handbook of Populism, edited by Kaltwasser, Cristóbal Rovira, Taggart, Paul, Espejo, Paulina Ochoa, and Ostiguy, Pierre, 4872. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198803560.013.2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weyland, K (2024) Democracy’s Resilience to Populism’s Threat: Countering Global Alarmism, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781009432504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wojcieszak, M (2014) Preferences for political decision-making processes and issue publics. Public Opinion Quarterly 78, 917939.10.1093/poq/nfu039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wuttke, A, Schimpf, C and Schoen, H (2020) When the whole is greater than the sum of its parts: On the conceptualization and measurement of populist attitudes and other multidimensional constructs. American Political Science Review 114, 356374.10.1017/S0003055419000807CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaslove, A and Meijers, M (2023) Populist democrats? Unpacking the relationship between populist and democratic attitudes at the citizen level. Political Studies 72, 00323217231173800.Google Scholar
Zmigrod, L, Rentfrow, PJ and Robbins, TW (2020) The partisan mind: Is extreme political partisanship related to cognitive inflexibility?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 149, 407.10.1037/xge0000661CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the populist attitudes variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Individual ideology and populist attitudes (generalized additive models with fixed effects)

Figure 2

Figure 1. Model 1 - populist attitudes and ideology

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideology and populist attitudes from Model 1; 95%CIs.
Figure 3

Figure 2. Re-estimated model 1 - populist attitudes and ideology

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideology and populist attitudes from Re-estimate Model 1; 95%CIs.
Figure 4

Figure 3. Model 2: populist attitudes and ideological extremism

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes from Model 2; 95%CIs.
Figure 5

Table 3. Ideological extremism and populist attitudes (generalized additive models with fixed effects)

Figure 6

Figure 4. Re-estimated model 2: populist attitudes and ideological extremism

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes from Re-estimate Model 2; 95%CIs.
Figure 7

Table 4. AIC & bic comparison - ideology

Figure 8

Table 5. AIC & bic comparison - ideological extremism

Figure 9

Figure 5. Sub-group analysis: Populist attitudes and ideology

Note: Nonlinear relationship between ideological extremism and populist attitudes from Model 2; 95%CIs.
Figure 10

Table 6. Sub-group analysis - ideology and populist attitudes (generalized additive models with fixed effects)

Supplementary material: File

Tamaki and Jung supplementary material

Tamaki and Jung supplementary material
Download Tamaki and Jung supplementary material(File)
File 667.5 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Tamaki and Jung Dataset

Link