1. Introduction
Household food waste is increasingly pointed out by several reports as a major issue. The United Nations Environment Program (United Nations Environment Programme, 2024) reported that households were responsible for 60% of the total food wasted in 2022 and that ‘on average, each person wastes 79 kg of food annually.’ While the literature examined several strategies to reduce food waste like increasing consumers’ awareness and adapting portions of food products (e.g., Hamerman, Rudell & Martins, Reference Hamerman, Rudell and Martins2018), an overlooked dimension relates to the used numbers and their framing in communications and reports.
For instance, is wasting 1,000 kg of food equivalent to wasting 1 ton? Does a waste of one thousand and eight hundred and fifty grams (1,850 g) more important than a waste of two thousand grams (2,000 g)? For a rational mind, there is no debate: In the first question, the quantity is identical, regardless of the used unit, while in the second one, 1,850 g is obviously lower than 2,000 g. Nevertheless, these seemingly simple situations can be disturbed by the ‘numerosity bias,’ that is, the human tendency to over-infer quantities when represented with higher numeric values or phonetically longer denominations, that is, they have more syllables (Pelham et al., Reference Pelham, Sumarta and Myaskovsky1994; Shrivastava et al., Reference Shrivastava, Jain, Nayakankuppam, Gaeth and Levin2017; West et al., Reference West, Azab, Ma and Bitter2020).
This bias has been studied in various settings such us resource allocation (Pelham et al., Reference Pelham, Sumarta and Myaskovsky1994; Shrivastava et al., Reference Shrivastava, Jain, Nayakankuppam, Gaeth and Levin2017), discounts (Pandelaere, Briers & Lembregts, Reference Pandelaere, Briers and Lembregts2011), money estimation (Raghubir, Capizzani & Srivastava, Reference Raghubir, Capizzani and Srivastava2017), and stock splits (West et al., Reference West, Azab, Ma and Bitter2020). Nevertheless, while it can be potentially leveraged to design more effective food waste messages, no study has examined its importance in this context. Thus, the objective of this short communication is to fill this gap. First, we test the effect of using a higher numeric value vs. a lower numeric value on individuals’ perceptions of an identical food waste quantity (Study 1). Second, following other scholars (e.g., Coulter et al., Reference Coulter, Choi and Monroe2012; Shrivastava et al., Reference Shrivastava, Jain, Nayakankuppam, Gaeth and Levin2017), we also test whether a lower food waste quantity presented in a phonetically longer denomination is perceived as higher than a lower quantity presented in a phonetically smaller denomination (Study 2).
The next section develops the conceptual framework and draws hypotheses. Sections 3 and 4 are devoted to Study 1 and Study 2, respectively. Section 5 is devoted to the discussion and implications. Section 6 concludes.
2. Background and hypotheses
The seminal works of Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky have demonstrated that perceptions and decision-making are frequently influenced by various heuristics and biases, leading to systematic and predictable errors (e.g., Tversky & Kahneman, Reference Tversky and Kahneman1974, Reference Tversky and Kahneman1981). Actually, because individuals often have a difficulty in processing numeric data, they rely on the numerosity heuristic (Pelham et al., Reference Pelham, Sumarta and Myaskovsky1994; Shrivastava et al., Reference Shrivastava, Jain, Nayakankuppam, Gaeth and Levin2017; West et al., Reference West, Azab, Ma and Bitter2020). Accordingly, individuals are influenced by (i) larger numbers and distracted by unit manipulations (120 minutes are perceived greater than 2 hours) and (ii) phonetically longer numbers that are perceived greater than phonetically smaller ones (two thousand eight hundred seventy-five greater than three thousand). Thus, we formulate the following hypotheses:
- •
H1. Food waste expressed in a higher numeric value is perceived greater than an equivalent quantity in a lower numeric value.
H2. Individuals are more likely to reduce food waste when it is framed in a higher numeric value, compared to its lower equivalent.
H3: Lower food waste expressed with a phonetically longer denomination is perceived greater than a higher food waste with a phonetically smaller denomination.
H4. Individuals are more likely to reduce food waste when it is framed in a phonetically-longer denomination, compared to a phonetically lower one.
In order to test our hypotheses, we conducted two experimental surveys. In Study 1 (S1), there are two treatments (T1 and T2) with an identical and realistic food waste quantity expressed in different units (2.5 kg in S1T1 versus 2500 g in S1T2). In Study 2 (S2), we compare a higher quantity expressed with few syllables (three kilograms in T1) and a smaller quantity expressed with more syllables (two kilos eight hundred seventy-five grams in T2). Table 1 below depicts the flow across studies.
Table 1. Experimental design

3. Study 1
3.1. Participants and design
147 participants (73% female, M = 21 years old) from a French school of agricultural engineering received an email invitation to join the experiment via a link. Participation was anonymous, voluntary, and without any compensation.
We designed a between-subjects experimental survey with random assignment across treatments. Specifically, participants read a realistic statement about food waste in France, framed either in a low numeric value (T1: 2.5 kg) or a higher numeric value (T2: 2500 g), precisely:
According to recent data from the environment and energy management agency (ADEME) and the Ministry of ecological transition and territorial cohesion, food waste in France during the consumption phase is 2.5 kg [versus 2500 g in T2] per person per month (https://www.ecologie.gouv.fr/gaspillage-alimentaire).
Participants were then asked to evaluate this quantity of food waste on a 7-point Likert scale (1: very low; 7: very high). They were also asked to indicate whether the statement above encourages them to reduce their food waste (1: does not encourage them at all; 7: strongly encourages them). Finally, in addition to age and gender, individuals were also asked to indicate their own practice in terms of food waste on a 7-point Likert scale (1: I waste very little; 7: I waste a lot).
3.2. Results
Mean responses (Table 2) suggest that perception of food waste volume and likelihood to reduce it were not affected by our numerosity manipulation. Analyzing the effect of the treatment on the likelihood to reduce food waste in a regression controlling for the perceived level of food waste, participants’ own practice, age, and gender (Table 3), confirms that H1 and H2 are not supported. Interestingly, individuals who perceive the food waste volume as high are more likely to reduce it, regardless of the treatment (the interaction between the two variables is not significant).
Table 2. Mean responses for food waste quantity and likelihood to reduce it

Table 3. Effect of larger numbers on food waste reduction

*** and ** stand for parameter significance at the 1% and 5% levels, respectively.
4. Study 2
4.1. Participants and design
144 other participants (74% female, Mage = 21 years old) were recruited the same way as in Study 1.
We used a similar between-subjects design as in the first study and asked participants to answer the same questions. The only change compared to Study 1 relates to the vignettes that are framed by using phonetically smaller versus longer denominations, precisely:
According to recent data from the environment and energy management agency (ADEME) and the Ministry of ecological transition and territorial cohesion, food waste in France during the consumption phase is around three kilograms [two kilos eight hundred seventy-five grams in T2] per person per month (https://www.ecologie.gouv.fr/gaspillage-alimentaire).
4.2. Results
Unlike our prediction (H3), two kilos eight hundred seventy-five grams of food waste are perceived lower than three kilograms and intentions to reduce food waste do not vary significantly across treatments (Table 4). Examining the effect of the treatment on the likelihood to reduce food waste reveals that H4 is not supported (Table 5).
Table 4. Mean responses for FW quantity and likelihood to reduce it

Table 5. Estimation of the effect of phonetical denomination on food waste reduction

*** stands for parameter significance at the 1% level.
5. Conclusion
Despite its promising features to address food waste issues, our manipulations based on the numerosity bias failed to change food waste perceptions and raise awareness. Unlike previous studies, we considered the numerosity bias in a domain unrelated to money. In addition, our sample consisted of participants with backgrounds in agriculture and environmental matters, likely to hold strong normative beliefs, which may have reduced their susceptibility to numerosity bias related to food waste.
The ineffectiveness of our manipulation may also be attributed to two other key factors.Footnote 1 Firstly, the numerosity bias, typically observed in evaluative situations, may not apply to food waste scenarios that focus on divesting rather than assessing value. Interestingly, the domain analyzed in our study pertains to a very concrete ‘bad’ (Grolleau et al., Reference Grolleau, Mzoughi and Weber2025), whereas previous studies often examined more abstract ‘goods’ or less negatively connoted items, such as price discounts or sharing. Secondly, food waste is likely associated with disgust – an instinctual reaction that serves as a protective mechanism to avoid potential sickness. Discarded food may evoke feelings of contamination or decay, reinforcing negative perceptions (see, e.g., Davey, Reference Davey2011; Horberg et al., Reference Horberg, Oveis, Keltner and Cohen2009; Rozin & Fallon, Reference Rozin and Fallon1987; Rozin et al., Reference Rozin, Haidt, McCauley, Lewis and Haviland-Jones2000). Disgust is often linked to the experience of nausea and prompts a need to expel possible harmful objects, potentially overriding numerosity appraisals. Moreover, when considered through the lens of affects-as-information or as-cognitive feedback models, negative emotions serve as a ‘Stop!’ signal and significantly influence decision-making (Huntsinger et al., Reference Huntsinger, Isbell and Clore2014; Schwarz & Clore, Reference Schwarz and Clore1983, Reference Schwarz, Clore, Fiedler and Forgas1988). This mechanism may explain why disgust exerts a stronger influence on food waste judgments than numerosity assessments. In sum, our findings suggest that the numerosity bias may have well-delineated scopes and that strong emotional responses can significantly shape decision-making processes, often superseding more abstract cognitive mechanisms.
Funding statement
None.
Competing interests
None.