Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-10T22:09:24.618Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations among rearing environment and the infant gut microbiome with early-life neurodevelopment and cognitive development in a nonhuman primate model (Macaca mulatta)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2025

Katherine Daiy*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Kyle Wiley
Affiliation:
Department of Sociology and Anthropology, University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX, USA
Jacob Allen
Affiliation:
Department of Kinesiology and Community Health, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
Michael T. Bailey
Affiliation:
The Research Institute at Nationwide Children’s Hospital, Center for Microbial Pathogenesis, Columbus, OH, USA Department of Pediatrics, The Ohio State University College of Medicine, Columbus, OH, USA
Amanda M. Dettmer
Affiliation:
Yale School of Medicine, Yale Child Study Center, New Haven, CT, USA
*
Corresponding author: Katherine Daiy; Email: katherine.daiy@yale.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Early gut microbiome development may impact brain and behavioral development. Using a nonhuman primate model (Macaca mulatta), we investigated the association between social environments and the gut microbiome on infant neurodevelopment and cognitive function. Infant rhesus monkeys (n = 33) were either mother-peer-reared (MPR) or nursery-reared (NR). Neurodevelopmental outcomes, namely emotional responsivity, visual orientation, and motor maturity, were assessed with the Primate Neonatal Neurobehavioral Assessment (PNNA) at 14–30 days. Cognitive development was assessed through tasks evaluating infant reward association, cognitive flexibility, and impulsivity at 6–8 months. The fecal microbiome was quantified from rectal swabs via 16S rRNA sequencing. Factor analysis was used to identify “co-abundance factors” describing patterns of microbial composition. We used multiple linear regressions with AIC Model Selection and differential abundance analysis (MaAsLin2) to evaluate relationships between co-abundance factors, microbiome diversity, and neuro-/cognitive development outcomes. At 30 days of age, a gut microbiome co-abundance factor, or pattern, with high Prevotella and Lactobacillus (β = −0.88, p = 0.04, AIC Weight = 68%) and gut microbiome alpha diversity as measured by Shannon diversity (β = −1.33, p = 0.02, AIC Weight = 80%) were both negatively associated with infant emotional responsivity. At 30 days of age, being NR was also associated with lower emotional responsivity (Factor 1 model: β = −3.13, p < 0.01; Shannon diversity model: β = −3.77, p < 0.01). The infant gut microbiome, along with early-rearing environments, may shape domains of neuro-/cognitive development related to temperament.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press in association with The International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD)

Introduction

Infancy is a critical period when early-life conditions shape developmental trajectories with consequences for later-life health. Reference Kuzawa and Quinn1 The gut microbiome, a commensal community of bacteria and other microorganisms that reside in the gastrointestinal tract, is central to human and nonhuman primate growth and development. The first few years of life are sensitive periods for the development of the gut microbiome. Initially sterile, the infant gut is gradually colonized by maternal and environmental microbiota during birth, breastfeeding, and from environmental microbiota. Reference Allen-Blevins, Sela and Hinde2,Reference Stinson3 Because of its low microbial diversity – often described as a “blank slate” after birth – the infant gut microbiome is particularly sensitive to such exposures. Previous research in human and nonhuman primates suggests that the infant gut microbiome is shaped by infant diet, Reference Thompson4 early social environments, Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 and exposure to antibiotics, Reference Reyman, van Houten and Watson6 among other factors. Reference Lloyd-Price, Abu-Ali and Huttenhower7 In humans, the infant gut microbiome continues to develop until it reaches a stable, adult-like state at approximately 2–3 years of age. Reference Arrieta, Stiemsma, Amenyogbe, Brown and Finlay8

The assembly and maturation of the gut microbiome occurs concordantly with the development of the host, including neurodevelopment and cognitive development in the first few years of life via the gut-brain axis. Reference Kelsey, Dreisbach, Alhusen and Grossmann9 Evidence for this co-development comes from germ-free mice, which exhibit different behavioral and cognitive outcomes than their counterparts. Compared to “colonized” mice, germ-free mice exhibit reduced anxiety-like behavior Reference Heijtz, Wang and Anuar10,Reference Neufeld, Kang, Bienenstock and Foster11 and increased motor activity, Reference Sordillo, Korrick and Laranjo12 suggesting that the gut microbiota holds a role in brain function. In line with the rodent literature, observational studies in humans have identified correlational relationships between the microbiome and cognition. For example, in a large cohort study, gut microbiome composition from 3–6 months of age was associated with fine motor skills and communication, personal, and social skills at 3 years. Reference Sordillo, Korrick and Laranjo12 Human studies have also explored the role of specific taxa in this association. In U.S infants, Faecalibacterium abundance and greater alpha diversity were associated with lower cognitive scores on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning at one year of age. Reference Carlson, Xia and Azcarate-Peril13 Another study found that in rural China, scores on Bayley Scales of Infant Development were positively associated with Faecalibacterium, Sutterella, and Clostridium abundance, while scores were not significantly associated with alpha diversity in this same study. Reference Rothenberg, Chen and Shen14 However, studies with human cohorts thus far lack data on the influence of different early-life social environments because such studies are complex and difficult to carry out in a controlled manner. Rodent studies provide careful control, but the majority of studies are carried out in environments that do not recapitulate human experiences; moreover, rodent models differ substantially from humans in a number of physiological, neurological, and behavioral traits. Reference Phillips, Bales and Capitanio15

Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) can fill these gaps. As an evolutionarily and translationally relevant animal model, macaques have been widely utilized to investigate infant growth and development because they allow the opportunity to study the relationships between early-life environments and developmental outcomes in a controlled manner. Reference Phillips, Bales and Capitanio15,Reference Dettmer, Suomi and Hinde16 Rhesus macaques are a widely dispersed and adaptable primate, second only to humans in their global population size and widespread distribution. Rhesus macaques live in large troops of both sexes, largely composed of related females and immigrant males. Reference Altmann17 Infants are highly attached to their mothers until 1 month of age, after which they gradually socialize more with peers and become increasingly distant from their mothers; by 4–5 months, play with peers becomes a main form of social interaction, growing more complex with time. Reference Dettmer, Suomi and Hinde16,Reference Harlow and Lauersdorf18,Reference Suomi19

Research in the past two decades has illustrated typical patterns of neurodevelopment, cognitive development, and the early-life social factors that shape these patterns in captive rhesus macaques. For instance, a descriptive analysis of cognitive development found that nursery-reared (NR) infants, which were reared with other infants by human caregivers in a highly enriched environment but absent species-typical caregiver interactions, had no gross cognitive differences as compared to mother-peer-reared (MPR) infants (though MPR infants showed greater initial reactivity to stimuli). Reference Murphy and Dettmer20 Additional work with this population of infant rhesus macaques found that NR and MPR infants differed in gut microbiome composition across early development. Specifically, though NR and MPR did not differ at birth, MPR infants had higher Bacteroides, Clostridium, and lower Bifidobacterium at Day 14; higher Lactobacillus and Streptococcus at Day 30; higher Lactobacillus, Bacteroides, Clostridium, and Prevotella, as well as lower Bifidobacterium and Streptococcus at Day 90; and lastly, no differences at Day 180. Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 Given the evidence in rodent literature highlighting links between the gut and brain, as well as the observed associations between early-life microbiomes and later cognitive outcomes in large-scale epidemiological studies, Reference Sordillo, Korrick and Laranjo12Reference Rothenberg, Chen and Shen14,Reference Laue, Korrick, Baker, Karagas and Madan21 it is of interest to delineate how the infant gut microbiome is associated with neuro and cognitive development. Therefore, this study investigates the relationships between the infant gut microbiome, early neurodevelopment, and cognitive development in rhesus macaques as a function of early social and caregiving experiences. We hypothesize that gut microbiome composition and diversity, in conjunction with early-rearing environments, may partly shape infant neurodevelopment and cognitive development in the first year of life. In captive NR and MPR infant macaques, we measured the gut microbiome via 16S rRNA sequencing of infant rectal swabs on Day 14, Day 30, and Day 180. In the first 30 days of life, we assessed neurodevelopment by measuring infant reactivity via the Primate Neonatal Neurobehavioral Assessment. Reference Schneider, Moore, Suomi and Champoux22 We assessed cognitive development by measuring a) reward association and cognitive flexibility by using a black/white discrimination (BW) and reversal (BWR) task, respectively, and b) impulsivity by using an Object Detour Reach task. Reference Murphy and Dettmer20,Reference Dettmer, Murphy and Suomi23 Utilizing controlled, experimental conditions not possible in humans, this study will contribute to our understanding of relationships between the composition of the gut microbiome, neurodevelopment, and cognition in sensitive periods of development as a function of early-rearing environment.

Methods

Subjects

This research was approved by the NICHD Animal Care and Use Committee and adhered to the American Society of Primatologists Principles for the Ethical Treatment of Nonhuman Primates. The sample included 33 infant rhesus macaques (M. mulatta), all of which were born and reared at the Laboratory of Comparative Ethology in Poolesville, Maryland, USA. Subjects were pseudo-randomly assigned to one of two rearing conditions: mother-peer rearing (MPR) or nursery rearing (NR). Sex-balancing was ensured to the degree possible. Because early rearing was a primary predictor of outcomes and the rearing of nonhuman primates occurred in different settings, researchers were aware of group allocation at all stages.

MPR infants were born and reared in indoor/outdoor pens in social groups consisting of their mothers, adult females (8–10), half-siblings (3–5), and one adult male. MPR infants were breastfed ad libitum from birth to approximately 8 months of age when they were relocated to be housed with NR infants in another part of the facility. MPR infants were also continuously exposed to foods their mothers ate, including commercial monkey chow (#5045; Purina, St Louis, MO), seeds, nuts, fruits, and other foraged items. NR infants were reared indoors by human caregivers with other infants and had visual and auditory contact with peers daily. From Day 37, NR infants were randomly assigned to either peer rearing, where they spent 24 h per day in contact with three other same-aged peers (peer groups were sex-balanced), or surrogate peer rearing, where they lived in single cages with cloth surrogates and had two hours of same-age peer contact daily. Because the microbiome composition did not differ between peer-reared and surrogate peer-reared (see supplementary information in Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 ), nor did overall early cognitive development, Reference Murphy and Dettmer20 we combined these sub-groups into a single group for analyses, “NR.” NR infants were formula-fed (Similac Advance Complete Nutrition formula, Chicago, IL) until Day 180, after which they ate monkey chow and foraged seeds, nuts, and fruits. Rearing protocols have been extensively described elsewhere. Reference Dettmer, Suomi and Hinde16,Reference Murphy and Dettmer20

Biospecimen collection

Our analysis centers on rectal swabs collected around the time of neurodevelopmental and cognitive assessments (Days 14, 30, and 180). Swabs were collected during routine neonatal assessments to prevent unnecessary separation of infants from social groups and/or mothers. Samples were collected between 0900–1100 h by gently washing the exterior surface of the infant rectum with a sterile saline solution and gauze, then gently inserting a sterile swab and spinning the swab three times in each direction and against the walls of the rectum before extraction (BD CultureSwab, Becton, Dickinson, and Company, Franklin Lakes, NJ). Microbiome sampling was conducted on a standardized schedule for all infants based on their date of birth (e.g., postnatal days 14 and 30). If the sampling or assessment day fell on a weekend, the collection date was shifted to either Friday or Monday (to accommodate for Saturday or Sunday, respectively). Rectal swabs were then placed on dry ice and stored at −80°C until they were shipped and analyzed at the Bailey Laboratory at Nationwide Children’s Hospital in Columbus, Ohio. Due to institutional constraints in 2015 that affected the project schedule, samples were unavailable for each infant and each age (Supplemental Table S1a–b shows the number of samples available by age and by neurodevelopmental/cognitive assessment).

Microbiome analysis

The QIAamp Fast DNA Stool Mini Kit (Qiagen, Germantown, MD) was used to extract DNA from rectal swabs for microbiome analysis; slight modifications were made to the manufacturer’s instructions. Swabs were incubated at 37°C for 45 min in lysozyme buffer (22 mg/ml lysozyme, 20 mM Tris-HCl, 2 mM E5/3/23TA, 1.2% Triton-x, pH 8.0), then bead-beat for 150 s with 0.1 mm zirconia beads. Samples were incubated at 95°C for 5 min with InhibitEX Buffer, then incubated at 70°C for 10 min with Proteinase K and Buffer AL. Following this step, the QIAamp Fast DNA Stool Mini Kit isolation protocol was followed, beginning with the ethanol step. DNA was quantified with the Qubit 2.0 Fluorometer (Life Technologies, Carlsbad, CA) using the dsDNA Broad Range Assay Kit (Carlsbad, CA). Samples were standardized to at least 5 ng/µl before being sent to the Molecular and Cellular Imaging Center in Wooster, OH, for library preparation. Amplified polymerase chain reaction libraries were sequenced from both ends of the 250 nt region of the V4–V5 16S rRNA hypervariable region using an Illumina MiSeq, Illumina, Inc. (San Diego, CA). Illinois Mayo Taxonomy Operations for RNA Database Operations (IM-TORNADO-2) workflow integrated with Mothur V1.40.0 was utilized for quality (> 30), and operational taxonomic unit binning of paired-end reads using Mothur and Greengenes (version 13.8) databases. Reference Schloss Patrick, Westcott Sarah and Ryabin24

Experiment 1: early-life neurodevelopment

Infants were given the Primate Neonatal Neurobehavioral Assessment (PNNA) to assess early neurodevelopment. The PNNA was administered on 14 and 30 ± 2 days (details about the PNNA can be found in previous work Reference Murphy and Dettmer20,Reference Schneider, Moore, Suomi and Champoux22 ). As with the microbiome sampling, PNNA assessments were conducted on a standardized schedule for all infants based on their date of birth (e.g., postnatal days 14, 30), and if the sampling or assessment day fell on a weekend, the collection date was shifted to either Friday or Monday (to accommodate for Saturday or Sunday, respectively). The PNNA assessed infants on 52 reflexes, behaviors, and developmental milestones by a trained researcher. For each item, infants were scored on a scale from 0 (absent), to 1 (weak) and 2 (strong) based on their reaction to stimuli or behavior. Half-values were also possible. From the PNNA, we calculated three developmental domains. Emotional responsivity was a composite measure of behavioral reactivity or emotional responsivity and was computed as the sum of the following measures: Irritability (amount of distress), Consolability (ease of researcher to console infant), Struggle During Test (amount of movement/wriggling), and Predominant State (amount of vigilance and agitation). The visual orientation score was the sum of Visual Orientation, Visual Follow, Duration of Looking, and Attention Span. Motor maturity was the sum of Head Posture in the prone position, Head Posture in the supine position, Response Speed, Coordination, and Labyrinthine Righting.

Experiment 2: cognitive development

We administered tasks developed specifically for infant macaques to assess infant cognitive development. Reference Murphy and Dettmer20,Reference Dettmer, Murphy and Suomi23,Reference Sackett, Ruppenthal, Hewitson, Simerly and Schatten25 Procedures for acclimating infants to cognitive testing have been described previously. Reference Murphy and Dettmer20 The main period of cognitive testing was 6-8 months of age. Scores (i.e., % correct response) on cognitive tests were averaged over all sessions; these averages or means were then utilized in the main statistical analyses. In this analysis, we focus on three measures of infant cognition – reward association, cognitive flexibility, and impulsivity, all detailed below. Cognitive training and assessments were initiated on the Monday closest to the infant’s postnatal at 120 days of age. Tasks were presented to infants in the following order: B/W Discrimination, B/W Reversal, and ODR, and each infant progressed to the next task once the criteria for passing the preceding task were achieved.

Reward association was evaluated with the Black/White Discrimination Task (BW). In this task, infants’ abilities to associate a color (black/white) with a food reward were tested. This task involved infants having to push aside one of two blocks (one black, one white) to receive a treat placed underneath the box in a well. For each infant, one color (black or white) was always associated/rewarded with a treat, whether presented on the left or right side. Infants had 60 s in each trial to make a choice, and criterion was reached when the infant made a certain number of correct responses (23 out of 25 trials in a session were correct, or 32 out of 35 correct responses over two days of testing). Averages of % correct responses (“average % correct”) were used in analyses to measure infant reward association ability. Reference Murphy and Dettmer20

Cognitive flexibility was measured with the Black/White Reversal task (BWR). This task measured how quickly infants could reverse a previously learned response from the Discrimination task. The colored block previously associated with a treat was “reversed” to the other color – the previously unrewarded block was now always rewarded, whether on the right or left side of the test board. Infants had 60 s to complete the task. Criterion was 23 out of 25 correctly answered trials. Averages of % correct responses (“average % correct”) were utilized as measures of infant cognitive flexibility. Reference Murphy and Dettmer20

Impulsivity was measured with the Object Detour Reach (“ODR”) task, which involved placing a clear plastic box with a treat inside in front of the infant, with an opening to the side of the box. An infant could “bonk” the box by incorrectly reaching for the treat through the front or could make the “correct” choice by reaching into the opening on the side of the box to obtain the treat. We calculated the z-scores of the average percentages of “bonks” (errors where the infant used a straight line-of-sight reach when the box was open to one side), of straight errors (where the infant used an incorrect side detour when the box was open to the front), and of side errors (where the infant used an incorrect side detour when the box was open to one side). We summed these three z-scores to create a composite “impulsivity” measure. Infants that displayed these errors were deemed to exhibit greater impulsivity. Reference Dettmer, Murphy and Suomi23,Reference Dettmer, Murphy and Guitarra26 We also ran analyses with the average percent correct responses of the ODR task as the outcome, presented in the supplementary material (Supplementary Table S4–5).

Statistical analysis

Our statistical analysis involved a multi-pronged exploratory approach: 1) exploring the within- and between-sample diversity in the microbiome data via alpha and beta diversity analyses, respectively; 2) using dimensionality reduction to collapse the microbiome data into “co-abundance factors”; 3) examining bivariate relationships between “co-abundance factors,” alpha diversity of the microbiome, and neurodevelopmental/cognitive outcomes via Spearman’s correlations (corrected for multiple comparisons); 4) assessing relationships between microbiome variables and neurodevelopmental and cognitive outcomes in multiple linear regression models separated by age group, while controlling for covariates (infant sex, growth rate); 5) comparing the predictive power of rearing environment vs. microbiome-related characteristics through information theory approaches; and, 6) exploring additional relationships between individual microbial taxa and outcomes of interest via FDR-corrected Multivariate Association with Linear Models (MaAsLin2). Reference Mallick, Rahnavard and Mclever27 Analyses were conducted in R using phyloseq (version 1.46), Reference McMurdie and Holmes28 psych (version 2.4.1), Reference Revelle, W29 ccrepe (version 1.1.3), Reference Schwager, Weingart, Bielski and Huttenhower30 vegan (version 2.6.4), Reference Oksanen, Blanchet, Friendly, Kindt, Legendre and McGlinn31 and MaAsLin2 (version 1.16.0) packages Reference Mallick, Rahnavard and Mclever27 ; read counts were normalized through total sum scaling prior to analysis. Reference Bullard, Purdom, Hansen and Dudoit32 We ran analyses such that every independent variable temporally preceded outcomes of interest. The sample size was based on data availability since not all infants had completed the experimental tasks successfully, and not all infants had a fecal swab available at certain time points. Samples with missing data were omitted pairwise to maximize sample size for each analysis. In Experiment 1, the sample sizes were as follows: 18 infants had both Day 14 fecal samples and neurodevelopmental measures (emotional responsivity, visual orientation, and motor maturity), and 19 infants had both Day 30 fecal samples and neurodevelopmental measures. The sample sizes for Experiment 2 were as follows: 18 infants had both Day 180 fecal samples and reward association scores, 15 had both Day 180 fecal samples and cognitive flexibility scores, and 20 had both Day 180 fecal samples and impulsivity scores.

Alpha and beta diversity

We calculated alpha diversity to describe within-sample microbial diversity. We selected the Shannon diversity index as our alpha diversity measure because of its robusticity in small sample sizes. Reference Casals-Pascual, González, Vázquez-Baeza, Song, Jiang and Knight33 We tested for sex- and rearing environment-based differences in alpha diversity, co-abundance factors, and neurodevelopmental and cognitive outcomes with Mann-Whitney U tests and utilizing Cliff’s Delta as the effect size estimates. We conducted a permutational analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) to measure differences in overall gut microbiome composition according to variables of interest (i.e., rearing environment, growth rate, and infant sex). We utilized Bray-Curtis dissimilarity as the beta diversity metric for the PERMANOVAs; Bray-Curtis quantifies the dissimilarity between samples (with values ranging from 0, meaning that two samples share all species and 1, which indicates that two samples do not share any species). We used the R adonis2 function (vegan package) for PERMANOVA, with the standard 999 permutations. Reference Oksanen, Blanchet, Friendly, Kindt, Legendre and McGlinn31 Principal Coordinates Analysis (PCoA) plots were used to visualize beta diversity analyses in two-dimensional space.

Principal components analysis (PCA) to generate microbiome “co-abundance factors”

To evaluate infant gut microbiome composition on a continuous scale and to reduce the dimensionality of the microbiome data, we obtained co-abundance factors describing groupings of bacterial taxa in the infant gut microbiome following previously published protocols. Reference Sordillo, Korrick and Laranjo12 Briefly, we determined Spearman’s correlation coefficients for the top 10 most abundant taxa (based on mean relative abundance) for the following age groups: Days 14, 30, and 180. Using Spearman’s correlation matrices, we conducted Principal Components Analysis (PCA; with varimax rotation) using the psych package in R. For all groups, a 3-factor solution was determined using the scree plot method. Factor/Principal Component loadings were used to generate factor scores. Individual factor scores, which we name Factors 1, 2 and 3 (for all ages), were utilized as independent variables in multiple linear regression models, with neurodevelopmental and cognitive measures as outcomes.

Spearman’s correlations

We calculated Spearman’s correlations between covariates, microbiome variables (co-abundance factors and Shannon diversity), and neurodevelopmental and cognitive variables. We also calculated p-values adjusted for the False-Discovery Rate.

Multiple linear regression models and AIC model selection

We ran separate multiple linear regressions by time point, with microbiome alpha diversity (Shannon diversity), “co-abundance” Factors 1, 2 and 3, and rearing environment predicting PNNA and cognitive scores. Each model included infant sex and infant growth rate (g/day) as covariates. Following previous protocol, Reference Sordillo, Korrick and Laranjo12 we opted out of using corrections for multiple comparisons in the multiple linear regression models (e.g., Bonferroni), in that these adjustments would be too conservative for our approach. Reference Casals-Pascual, González, Vázquez-Baeza, Song, Jiang and Knight33 We checked that each model met regression assumptions beforehand by examining data linearity, normality of residuals, and homoscedasticity and by looking for data points with high leverage with diagnostic plots; each model met the assumptions within reason. We used AIC model selection to identify the best-fit model among models with rearing environment, alpha diversity, and both rearing environment and alpha diversity as predictors.

Multivariate association with linear models (MaAsLin2)

To investigate possible associations between less abundant taxa not captured by co-abundance groupings (following previously established methods Reference Sordillo, Korrick and Laranjo12 ), we utilized Multivariate Association with Linear Models (MaAsLin2) to assess the relationship between individual genus-level taxa and cognitive and neurodevelopmental outcomes. MaAsLin2 employs general linear models to assess associations between specific microbial taxa and variables of interest while controlling for false discoveries, using p and q values (FDR-adjusted p-values). Using our normalized microbiome data, we ran the MaAsLin2 analysis with the following parameters: minimum abundance = 0; minimum prevalence = 10%; normalization = none; transformation = none; standardize = false; q-value threshold = 0.25. We selected the program’s default q-value threshold of 0.25 because of our exploratory aims and because of our inclusion of covariates in the models and the additional load they have on the burden of multiple testing. We included various cognitive and neurodevelopmental outcomes as the “fixed effects” in each model. All models also included covariates – rearing environment, infant sex, and growth rate. To avoid reporting non-meaningful relationships in our small sample sizes, we only report statistically significant associations for taxa found at detectable levels in greater than 10 subjects.

Results

Descriptive statistics

This analysis used data from 33 infant rhesus macaque subjects. Most infants did not have a fecal swab for all three time points. For example, a portion (12/20) of the infants with data at Day 180 did not have any fecal swabs for either Day 14 or Day 30. Moreover, 11 of the 18 infants with Day 14 samples did not have a fecal swab for Day 180, and 11 of the 19 infants with Day 30 samples similarly did not have a Day 180 swab (Supplementary Table S1a–b). Forty-two percent of the infant subjects included in this analysis were were raised in an MPR environment (14/33), and 48% (16/33) were female (Table 1).

Table 1. Sample characteristics by infant’s age at microbiome sampling (mean [standard deviation] unless noted;* indicates % [proportion of sample])

MPR and NR infants differed in markers of early neurodevelopment in the first 30 days of life. In the Day 14 and Day 30 age groups, MPR infants had higher emotional responsivity scores than their NR peers, indicative of greater emotionality (Day 14: Cliff’s Delta = 0.84, p < 0.01; Day 30: Cliff’s Delta = 2.67, p < 0.01). NR infants had higher visual orientation scores in Day 14 and Day 30 age groups (Day 14: Cliff’s Delta = -0.86, p < 0.01; Day 30: Cliff’s Delta = −0.81, p < 0.01); however, there were no significant differences in motor maturity by rearing environment at either age group (Day 14: Cliff’s Delta = 0.27, p = 0.35; Day 30: Cliff’s Delta = 0.27, p = 0.33). Infant sex was not statistically associated with either emotional responsivity scores or with visual orientation (effect sizes not shown; p > 0.05). Surrogate- and peer-reared infants did not differ in most neurodevelopmental and cognitive outcomes, except that at Day 30, SPR infants had higher visual orientation (Cliff’s Delta: −1.0, p = 0.02), and at Day 180, SPR infants had lower cognitive flexibility (Cliff’s Delta = 0.77, p = 0.03; Supplemental Table S6).

MPR and NR infants did not differ regarding later cognitive outcomes, except for one measure on the Object Detour Reach or impulsivity task. In the Day 180 age group, MPR infants had lower percent correct responses than NR infants (Cliff’s Delta = −0.68, p = 0.01); this measure does not necessarily encapsulate impulsivity but instead captures the number of correct trials where the animal was given up to 60 s to correctly complete the trial in as many attempts as it wanted. We note that for these later cognitive outcomes, fewer infants completed the tasks, and as a result, smaller sample sizes of data were available for statistical analysis (Supplementary Table S1a–b).

Fecal microbiome composition and co-abundance factors

Figures 1–3 show taxonomic variation in the fecal microbiome across age groups and rearing environments. There were qualitative age-and-rearing-environment-specific patterns of taxonomic composition. In MPR infants, at Days 14, 30, and 180, the predominant phylum was Firmicutes at each time point (41.6 ± 8.2%, 43.7 ± 16.6%, and 57.9 ± 20.0%, respectively). In NR infants, the predominant phylum at Day 14 and 30 were Actinobacteria (36.5 ± 28.5% and 41.4 ± 30.7%, respectively), and the predominant phylum at Day 180 was Firmicutes (61.3 ± 20.3%). At the family level, MPR infants had gut microbiota enriched in Prevotellaceae at Days 14 and 30 (27.6 ± 13.6 and 28.8 ± 14.0%, respectively); at Day 180, Ruminococcaceae was the predominant bacterial family in the MPR group (19.3 ± 10.1%). In NR infants, Bifidobacteriaceae was predominant at Days 14 and 30 (34.5 ± 28.3% and 37.7 ± 33.3%, respectively); at Day 180, however, Prevotellaceae was predominant (28.6 ± 16.9%). At the genus level, age-specific trends mirrored those at the phylum and family levels. In MPR infants, Prevotella was the predominant genus at Days 14 (27.6 ±13.6%), 30 (28.8 ± 13.9%,) and 180 (18.8 ± 7.9%). In contrast, NR infants exhibited gut microbiota enriched in Bifidobacterium at Days 14 (34.6 ± 28.3%) and 30 (37.7 ± 33.3%); at Day 180, Prevotella was the most abundant genus in NR infants (28.6 ± 16.9%; Figs 13).

Figure 1. Gut microbiome composition at phylum level (top 4 phyla)1.

Figure 2. Gut microbiome composition at family level (top 10 families)1.

Figure 3. Gut microbiome composition at genus level (top 10 genera)1.

At each age group, factor analysis yielded three co-abundance factors, or general patterns of fecal microbiome composition. These general patterns of fecal microbiome composition tended to vary in representation of key bacterial genera such as Prevotella, Lactobacillus, and Bifidobacterium, which were also variably abundant across ages and rearing environments (Figs 13). At Day 14, Factor 1 had high Catenibacterium and Lactobacillus (40% of variance); Factor 2 represented low Bifidobacterium and high Blautia (36% of variance); Factor 3 represented low Prevotella (Prevotellaceae family; 24% of variance). At Day 30, Factor 1 was heavily loaded by Prevotella (Paraprevotellaceae family) and Lactobacillus, as well as low Faecalibacterium (41% of variance); Factor 2 had high Eubacterium, Blautia, and low Catenibacterium (37% of variance); Factor 3 had high Collinsella, Ruminococcus and low Bifidobacterium (22% of variance). At Day 180, Factor 1 had high Roseburia, Blautia, and low Ruminococcus and Prevotella (36% of variance); Factor 2 had low Bifidobacterium and low abundance of an unclassified genus (32% of variance); Factor 3 had high Faecalibacterium, and low Prevotella, Oscillospira, and Lactobacillus (32% of variance). The factor loadings for each factor are represented in Supplemental Tables 2a–c.

Alpha and beta diversity

Microbiome alpha diversity varied by age and rearing environment but not by infant sex. Infants across the two rearing conditions differed in their overall community composition, and this held across every age group. Specifically, Shannon diversity in the gut microbiome increased with age (ANOVA: F-statistic = 10.84, p < 0.01). Monkeys who were NR had significantly lower Shannon diversity on Day 14 (Cliff’s Delta = 0.79, p < 0.01), Day 30 (Cliff’s Delta = 0.60, p = 0.02), but not at Day 180 (Cliff’s Delta = 0.29, p = 0.30; Supplementary Figures S1a–c).

Figures 4–6 show Principal Coordinates Analysis (PCoA) plots of beta diversity estimates and results of the PERMANOVA models. According to PERMANOVA, rearing environment was associated with microbiome beta diversity, or overall fecal microbiome composition, at Day 14 (R2 = 0.16, p = 0.01), Day 30 (R2 = 0.17, p = 0.01), and Day 180 (R2 = 0.13, p = 0.01); rearing environment differences in beta diversity were also found in this cohort in 2019. Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 Infant sex was not associated with microbiome composition at any time point (p > 0.05; Figs 46).

Figure 4. Day 14 (MPR: n = 7; NR: n = 11; total: n = 18)1.

Figure 5. Day 30 (MPR: n = 9; NR: n = 10; total: n=19)1.

Figure 6. Day 180 (MPR: n = 8; NR: n = 12; total: n = 20)1.

Correlations between microbiome characteristics and neurodevelopmental/cognitive outcomes

After exploring Spearman’s correlations between variables and FDR adjustment of p-values, we found that Shannon diversity and Factor 2 at Day 14 were positively correlated (ρ = 0.79, p < 0.01; Fig 7), suggesting that this co-abundance factor was associated with greater microbial diversity. We also observed that Shannon diversity and Factor 1 at Day 30 were positively associated (ρ = 0.87, p < 0.001; Fig 8). There were no statistically significant correlations between any co-abundance factor nor Shannon diversity with any neurodevelopmental or cognitive developmental outcome (Figures 7–9).

Figure 7. Day 14 (MPR: n = 7; NR: n = 11; total: n = 18).

Figure 8. Day 30 (MPR: n = 9; NR: n = 10; total: n=19).

Figure 9. Day 180 (MPR: n = 8; NR: n = 12; total: n = 20).

Multiple linear regression models and AIC model selection

Our regression models and AIC model selection procedure, in general, demonstrated that rearing environment was more often significantly associated with neuro and cognitive developmental outcomes than the composition and diversity of the infant fecal microbiome (Tables 2–5). Being NR was associated with lower emotional responsivity, higher visual orientation, and lower motor maturity in early infancy; however, there were no rearing-based differences in reward association, cognitive flexibility, or impulsivity. While most microbiome co-abundance factors and Shannon diversity across different ages were not significantly associated with neuro-/cognitive developmental outcomes, we found that fecal microbiome composition and diversity at 30 days of age were associated with emotional responsivity (Tables 25).

Table 2. Multiple linear regression models with microbial co-abundance factors predicting neurodevelopment (Experiment 1) 1

1 Models for each Factor were run separately. Coefficients, confidence intervals and p-values of covariates (infant sex, growth rate and rearing environment) reflect the mean of their respective values across all models with Factor 1, 2 and 3 as predictors.

2 Day 14: MPR: n = 7; NR: n = 11; total: n = 18.

3 Day 30: MPR: n = 9, NR: n = 10; total: n = 19.

Table 3. Multiple linear regression models with microbial co-abundance factors predicting cognitive outcomes (Experiment 2) 1

1 Models for each Factor were run separately. Coefficients, confidence intervals and p-values of covariates (infant sex, growth rate and rearing environment) reflect the mean of their respective values across all models with Factor 1, 2 and 3 as predictors.

2 Reward Association: MPR: n = 6; NR: n = 12; total: n = 18.

3 Cognitive flexibility: MPR: n = 3; NR: n = 12; total: n = 15.

4 Impulsivity: MPR: n = 8; NR: n = 12; total: n = 20.

Table 4. Multiple linear regression models with Shannon diversity predicting neurodevelopment (Experiment 1)

1 Day 14: MPR: n = 7; NR: n = 11; total: n = 18.

2 Day 30: MPR: n = 9, NR: n = 10; total: n = 19.

Table 5. Multiple linear regression model with Shannon diversity predicting cognitive outcomes (Experiment 2)

1 Reward Association: MPR: n = 6; NR: n = 12; total: n = 18.

2 Cognitive flexibility: MPR: n = 3; NR: n = 12; total: n = 15.

3 Impulsivity: MPR: n = 8; NR: n = 12; total: n = 20.

Experiment 1: early infancy gut microbiome and neurodevelopment

In Experiment 1, infant fecal microbiome composition was only associated with infant emotional responsivity at 30 days, while rearing environment was linked to differences in almost every neurodevelopmental outcome. Specifically, we found that at Day 14, there were no significant associations between microbiome characteristics and neurodevelopmental outcomes. However, at Day 14, being NR as opposed to MPR was associated with lower emotional responsivity (β = −2.73, p = 0.01; Table 2) and lower motor maturity (β = −0.54, p < 0.01; Table 2) while accounting for other covariates and microbiome co-abundance factors. At Day 14, in most of the models we ran, being NR was associated with higher visual orientation (Factor 1 model: β = 4.47 (95% CI: 0.23- 8.12), p = 0.04; Factor 2 model: β = 3.76 (95% CI: 0.05-7.5), p = 0.05); however, in one model, where Factor 2 was the independent variable, being NR was not significantly associated with visual orientation and the confidence interval included zero (Factor 2 model: β = 3.29 (95% CI: −0.98-7.6), p = 0.11). Therefore, the averaged 95% confidence interval (Rearing environment (NR): 95% CI: 0.98-8.12) shown in Table 2 passes through zero, even though the p-value indicates statistical significance. We emphasize that the effect size, the beta coefficient, is relatively consistent across models and that this points to a relationship between the rearing environment and visual orientation in the Day 14 age group.

At Day 30, Factor 1 (a pattern with high Prevotella and Lactobacillus, low Bifidobacterium and Faecalibacterium; β = −0.88, p = 0.04) and being NR (β = −3.13, p < 0.01) were negatively associated with emotional responsivity (Table 2). The model with Factor 1 and rearing environment as predictor variables was the “best-fit” model and accounted for 68% of the total predictive power in the model set (AIC Weight = 68%). Shannon diversity at Day 30 was also negatively associated with emotional responsivity, such that a 1-unit increase in Shannon diversity at Day 30 corresponded to a ∼ 1.33 unit decrease in emotional responsivity score (β = −1.33, p = 0.02, AIC Weight = 80%; Table 4).

When we compared different models by using AIC model selection, “rearing” models in which only rearing environment was included as a predictor were consistently the best-fit models for predicting infants’ early neurodevelopment (Supplemental Table S3 a-b). However, “rearing and microbiome” models were best-fit among models predicting emotional responsivity at Day 30 (Factor 1: AIC Weight = 68%; Shannon diversity: AIC Weight = 80%; neurodevelopment; Supplemental Table S3a–b), suggesting that the gut microbiome and rearing environment together provide the greatest amount of predictive power among all models in explaining infant emotional responsivity at this age.

Experiment 2: late infancy gut microbiome and cognitive development

We did not observe statistically significant relationships between the microbiome, rearing environment, and cognitive outcomes in Experiment 2. Neither microbial co-abundance factors nor Shannon diversity were significantly associated with either reward association, cognitive flexibility, and impulsivity; rearing environment was also not associated with these variables (Tables 3,5). Being NR was significantly associated with higher average % correct responses on the Object Detour Reach task (Supplementary Table S4; β = 5.97, p = 0.02; β = 5.02, p = 0.05). However, this “average % correct” measure simply represents the number of correct trials in a 60 s interval and is not an accurate reflection of impulsivity like the composite impulsivity measure (which incorporates the total number of all possible impulsive responses before getting the trial correct). Therefore, we conclude that while NR infants persisted more to get the Object Detour Reach trial correct, rearing environment was not necessarily associated with infant impulsivity in this group of subjects.

Differential abundance analysis via MaAsLin2: associations between the abundance of microbial taxa and neurodevelopmental/cognitive outcomes

Emotional responsivity and cognitive flexibility were associated with several specific microbial genera. For Experiment 1, infants with greater abundance of fecal microbial taxa such as Enterococcus (coefficient = -0.0012, q = 0.144; significance at q < 0.25) and Campylobacter (coefficient = −0.0004, q = 0.156) had lower scores of emotional responsivity regardless of rearing condition at Day 30 (Table 6). For Experiment 2, higher abundance of Butyrococcus (coefficient = −0.00025, q = 0.204), Streptococcus (coefficient = −0.00084, q = 0.115), and Lactobacillus (coefficient = −0.0057, q = 0.164) at Day 180 were negatively associated with cognitive flexibility. We did not observe any statistically significant associations between specific taxa at Day 14, nor with any other neurodevelopmental or cognitive measure besides emotional responsivity and cognitive flexibility.

Table 6. MaAsLin2 analysis results: associations between gut microbiome taxa at different ages postpartum and cognitive and neurodevelopmental measures across both rearing environments1 (Total: n = 33)

1 All models adjusted for growth rate (g/Day), infant sex (Male, Female), and rearing environment (MPR, NR).

2 Number of subjects with detectable feature; for brevity, we only show results for taxa present in greater than 10 subjects.

3 significance at <0.05.

4 FDR Adjusted P-Value; significance at q < 0.25.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated how patterns of gut microbiome composition and diversity predicted infant neurodevelopment in early infancy (Experiment 1) and cognitive development in mid to late infancy (Experiment 2) following controlled exposure to differing early social environments. Overall, we found that rearing environment was more often significantly associated with most of the repertoire of neurodevelopmental and cognitive outcomes in the multiple regression models; through an information theory approach, we also demonstrate that rearing-environment-driven models were most often the best-fit models. However, a gut microbiome pattern high in Prevotella and Lactobacillus and alpha diversity at 30 days of age was linked to emotional responsivity in early infancy; additionally, several models including both rearing environment and gut microbiome features (co-abundance factors, alpha diversity) were “best-fit” to the data. In all, this study is, to the best of our knowledge, the first study to investigate rearing environment, the infant gut microbiome, and neurodevelopment and cognition in infant rhesus macaques. This study provides novel findings showing that the gut microbiome’s composition and diversity may partially explain infant emotional responsivity in addition to rearing condition; these results may have implications for the development of psychobiotic interventions. Because this study is not designed to test causality, we suggest that future research with larger sample sizes could include formal mediation analyses with interaction terms to determine whether the gut microbiome acts as a physiological link between early-rearing environments and neurodevelopmental and cognitive outcomes.

Through an exploration of taxonomic composition (Figs 1– 3) by age and rearing environment, we found several age- and rearing-specific taxonomic patterns that were similar to previous work. The family Bifidobacteriaceae and the genus Bifidobacterium were highly abundant in infants who were NR and at 14 and 30 days of age (Figs 1– 3). This was noted in previous research with this cohort, which found that NR infants at 14 and 30 days had greater Bifidobacterium abundance than MPR infants. Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 These differences may be attributed to the fact that the commercial formula given to the NR infants has plant-derived galacto-oligosaccharides that favor the growth of bifidobacteria, Reference Gopal, Sullivan and Smart34Reference Kukkonen, Savilahti and Haahtela36 a feature that may have sustained a relatively high abundance of Bifidobacterium in NR infants at both 14 and 30 days. Prevotella was the most abundant genus in MPR infants in all age groups (Figs 13). Prevotella is thought to play roles in fiber digestion in humans Reference Chen, Long, Zhang, Liu, Zhao and Hamaker37 and rhesus macaques alike. Reference Chen, Li, Liang, Li and Huang38 MPR infants in this study had continuous access to nuts, seeds, and commercial monkey chow that their mothers ate; this nutritional environment may contribute to the relatively high Prevotella exhibited by MPR infants across age groups.

At 30 days of age (roughly equivalent to 4 months in humans), being NR and harboring a gut microbiome pattern with high Prevotella and Lactobacillus and low Faecalibacterium was associated with lower emotional responsivity. In addition, when we examined taxa-specific associations, several bacterial taxa (e.g., Campylobacter, Enterococcus, and genera from Lacnospiraceae and Bifidobacteriaceae) in the infant gut at 30 days of age were associated with emotional responsivity, but were not significantly associated other neurodevelopmental measures (Table 6). Our finding that features of the infant primate gut microbiome predict infant emotional responsivity, specifically, is in line with the literature and may reflect a relationship between the early infant microbiome and infant temperament. In humans, infant temperament forms the basis of social and emotional health. Temperament in infancy is associated with emotional and behavioral characteristics in childhood, including hyperactivity/inattention scores and emotional difficulties. Reference Brown39 The PNNA was developed as a laboratory assessment of newborn macaque temperament, and its structure mirrors that of the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale. Reference Schneider, Moore, Suomi and Champoux22 Emotional responsivity is a composite of irritability, consolability, struggle during the test, and predominant state. Emotional responsivity reflects an individual’s ability to regulate arousal in response to external stimuli and to move from high arousal to lower arousal state Reference Zeanah40 ; emotional responsivity may reflect characteristics of individual temperament. Reference Rothbart, Posner, Hartlage and Telzrow41

The specific pattern of high Prevotella and Lactobacillus and high alpha diversity being linked to emotional responsivity warrants explanation, yet current literature shows that these relationships are still being uncovered. For instance, Prevotella and Lactobacillus species are reported to be a common dominant feature of rhesus macaque and other nonhuman primate microbiomes, Reference Gao, Salzwedel and Carlson42,Reference Fan, Zang and Liu43 but also vary substantially between individuals. Reference Clayton, Gomez and Amato44 Prevotella appears to play an important role in maintaining a healthy overall structure of the gut microbiome, at least in human populations. 45,Reference Yatsunenko, Rey and Manary46 Lactobacillus species, on the other hand, have probiotic properties and are common to macaque and human milk microbiomes alike, Reference Gao, Salzwedel and Carlson42Reference Clayton, Gomez and Amato44 possibly playing an integral role in the microbial exchange network in breastfeeding mother-infant dyads. Reference Qi, Zhou and Tu50 In this sample and in previous research with this cohort, Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 Lactobacillus and Prevotella are higher in the gut microbiome of MPR infants than in NR infants (Supplemental Figure S2d), a finding that may be related to aggregate differences in nutritional and social environments across the experimental rearing conditions. Sociability is also positively associated with the relative abundance of Prevotella among adult macaques, Reference Johnson, Watson, Dunbar and Burnet51 suggesting that Prevotella species may be transmitted through early-life environmental factors, such as feeding mode and social contact. High Prevotella and Lactobacillus abundances seem to be linked to reduced emotional problems in early human development. For instance, human toddlers with a low abundance of Prevotella show higher levels of sadness, a temperamental trait that contributes to the broader domain of negative affectivity. Reference Fan, Zang and Liu43 Human infants with a low abundance of Lactobacillus have greater negative affectivity Reference Fox, Lee and Wiley52 ; supplemented as a probiotic, Lactobacillus reduced symptoms of anxiety and depression in human adults. Reference Messaoudi, Violle, Bisson, Desor, Javelot and Rougeot53 Reduced abundances of Prevotella have also been associated with other emotional problems in humans, including increased odds of internalizing disorder symptoms and autism in childhood. Reference Loughman, Ponsonby and O’Hely54,Reference Kang, Park, Ilhan and Gilbert55 The mechanisms by which taxa act remain unclear, but researchers have speculated that they may include interaction with host immune systems, production of short-chain fatty acids, and regulation of host metabolism. Reference Borre, Moloney, Clarke, Dinan, Cryan, Lyte and Cryan56,Reference Dinan and Cryan57

Greater taxonomic diversity of the infant gut microbiome is associated with lower emotional responsivity in this sample. Alpha diversity tends to increase with age in infant macaques and humans alike and is thought to reflect a state of gut microbial maturation. Reference Bokulich, Chung and Battaglia58 Similar to our findings, Aatsinki and colleagues Reference Aatsinki, Lahti and Uusitupa59 found that alpha diversity measured at two and half months was inversely associated with negative emotionality and fear reactivity in six-month-old infants. Gut microbiome alpha diversity has also been found to be positively associated with concurrent fear behavior in twelve-month-old human infants. Reference Carlson, Xia and Azcarate-Peril60 Gut microbial diversity may influence temperament indirectly, as several studies have shown that it may predict aspects of brain structure and functional connectivity in infancy. Reference Carlson, Xia and Azcarate-Peril13,Reference Gao, Salzwedel and Carlson42,Reference Kelsey, Prescott and McCulloch61 Caution is warranted, as is further research, because other studies have reported no associations between alpha diversity and temperament across infancy. Reference Fox, Lee and Wiley52,Reference Loughman, Ponsonby and O’Hely54,Reference Kelsey, Prescott and McCulloch61

That both the microbiome and rearing environment are together associated with emotional responsivity suggests that even when accounting for the complexities of nutritional, social, immunological, and environmental factors, the infant gut microbiome may still be partially associated with variation in infant temperament. Differences between these rearing conditions are likely to contribute to variation in the development of the gut microbiome, which is supported by our results here and in previous work. Reference Dettmer, Allen, Jaggers and Bailey5 There are several potential avenues by which early-life environments may contribute to variation in the gut microbiome that are pertinent to our study. For both humans and macaques, breastmilk and formula feeding have been shown to have distinct influences on the development of the gut microbiome. Reference Timmerman, Rutten and Boekhorst62,Reference Ardeshir, Narayan and Méndez-Lagares63 Furthermore, bottle feeding of pumped breastmilk may lead to reduced co-occurrence of microbiota between breastmilk and infant stool as pumping can affect the composition of the breastmilk microbiome and potentially prevent the transfer of maternal (breast) skin microbes to the infant gut. Research in both humans and primates also points to the influence of the social environment, non-maternal allocaregivers, and social partners on the developing gut microbiome via vertical and horizontal transmission pathways, Reference Manus, Sardaro and Dada64Reference Degnan, Pusey and Lonsdorf69 which may potentially explain some of the microbial differences we observed between rearing conditions and the finding that both gut microbiome composition and rearing environment were significantly associated with one aspect of neurodevelopment (i.e., emotional responsivity).

Together, these results point to a model where early-life microbiomes converge with early social environments to influence infant neurodevelopment and cognition. This study contributes to the growing translational science literature on the link between the gut microbiome and neuro-/cognitive development in early life by presenting these results in a tractable nonhuman primate model. We note, however, that the specific physiological mechanisms underlying our reported associations are unclear and require further investigation. The gut microbiome is connected to the brain through immune, endocrine (e.g., glucocorticoid hormones, such as cortisol), and neural (e.g., the vagus nerve) pathways Reference Borre, Moloney, Clarke, Dinan, Cryan, Lyte and Cryan56,Reference Cryan and Dinan70,Reference Dinan, Stilling, Stanton and Cryan71 Therefore, future research in nonhuman primate models and humans alike could additionally assess hormonal and immune biomarkers in addition to gut microbiome and cognitive data to further elucidate these physiological mechanisms and probable causal pathways.

Our study also has implications for translational science that aims to identify interventions for improving outcomes after early-life adversity. Our results suggest that the composition of the gut microbiome may impact infant outcomes jointly and independently from the contribution of early-life environments (indicated by rearing condition) on infant temperament and cognitive development. Work from experimental manipulation of rearing environment in other animal species points to potential mechanisms and opportunities for interventions. Several studies of piglets support the hypothesis that psychobiotic interventions that supplement the diet with fiber and pre/probiotics may accelerate the maturation of the gut microbiome in ways that support infant growth, increased expression of intestinal neurotransmitters, and reduced incidence of post-weaning problems, such as diarrhea. Reference Choudhury, Middelkoop and Boekhorst72,Reference Berding and Donovan73 Such findings provide support for additional research aimed at evaluating the use of psychobiotic interventions to support the development of the gut microbiome of infants affected by early-life adversity. This body of work also supports the development of new milk substitutes that may more closely mirror the components of milk that contribute to the normative development of the gut microbiome.

This study has several limitations that warrant discussion. First, the sample sizes for our analysis were relatively small (< 50 individuals); not all infants could be given cognitive assessments, so many infants with microbiome samples did not yield cognitive data. The small sample sizes in our analysis likely reduced statistical power and thus may limit the generalizability of our results. Additionally, while the experimental rearing environments in our study can be considered a proxy for infant diet, we do not have available data on specific dietary intakes of individual infants and thus cannot control for dietary variation that may have influenced the development of the gut microbiome. To address this limitation, future research could include detailed measures of infant nutrient intake in addition to neurodevelopmental/cognitive measures and fecal samples or a formula-supplemented group of MPR infants to control for dietary confounders.

Conclusion

Through a multi-faceted methodological approach, we examined the relationship between the infant gut microbiome and neurodevelopment and cognitive development in captive rhesus macaques subject to two differing early social/rearing environments. We found that along with being exposed to a nursery-rearing environment as opposed to mother-peer rearing, an infant gut microbiome pattern with a high abundance of Prevotella and Lactobacillus, as well as higher alpha diversity, are both associated with lower emotional responsivity in infant macaques. Our results suggest that infant gut microbiome composition and diversity uniquely explain a part of infant neurodevelopmental outcomes, even when accounting for the aggregate differences in diet, environment, and social contact across experimental rearing conditions. This further points to a potential role of the microbiome in shaping infant temperament in conjunction with early-rearing environment that could be teased apart in future research designed to test potentially mediatory and causal relationships.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2040174424000400.

Acknowledgements

We thank Ashley Murphy, Kathryn Jones, Ryan McNeill, Kristen Byers, and animal care staff for their assistance with data collection. We would also like to thank Katherine Amato, Melissa Manus, Elijah Watson, the Yale University Center for Clinical Investigation, and the Yale Cushing/Medical Library Bioinformatics Help Desk team for advice on statistical methods.

Financial support

This study was funded by the Division of Intramural Research at the NICHD and by the American Society of Primatologists Legacy Award (AMD).

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national guides on the care and use of laboratory animals (American Society of Primatologists Principles for the Ethical Treatment of Nonhuman Primates) and have been approved by the institutional committee (NICHD Animal Care and Use Committee).

References

Kuzawa, CW, Quinn, EA. Developmental origins of adult function and health: evolutionary hypotheses. Annu Rev Anthropol. 2009; 38, 131147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen-Blevins, CR, Sela, DA, Hinde, K. Milk bioactives may manipulate microbes to mediate parent-offspring conflict. Evol Med Public Health. 2015; 2015, 106121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stinson, LF. Establishment of the early-life microbiome: a DOHaD perspective. J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2020; 11, 201210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, AL. Developmental origins of obesity: early feeding environments, infant growth, and the intestinal microbiome. Am J Hum Biol. 2012; 24, 350360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dettmer, AM, Allen, JM, Jaggers, RM, Bailey, MT. A descriptive analysis of gut microbiota composition in differentially reared infant rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) across the first 6 months of life. Am J Primatol. 2019; 81,19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyman, M, van Houten, MA, Watson, RL, et al. Effects of early-life antibiotics on the developing infant gut microbiome and resistome: a randomized trial. Nat Commun. 2022; 13, 893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd-Price, J, Abu-Ali, G, Huttenhower, C. The healthy human microbiome. Genome Med. 2016; 81, 51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arrieta, MC, Stiemsma, LT, Amenyogbe, N, Brown, E, Finlay, B. The intestinal microbiome in early life: health and disease. Front Immunol. 2014 ; 5, 427.Google ScholarPubMed
Kelsey, C, Dreisbach, C, Alhusen, J, Grossmann, T. A primer on investigating the role of the microbiome in brain and cognitive development. Dev Psychobiol. 2019; 61,341349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heijtz, RD, Wang, S, Anuar, F, et al. Normal gut microbiota modulates brain development and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011; 108, 30473052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neufeld, KM, Kang, N, Bienenstock, J, Foster, JA. Reduced anxiety-like behavior and central neurochemical change in germ-free mice. Neurogastroenterol Motil. 2011; 23, 255e119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sordillo, JE, Korrick, S, Laranjo, N, et al. Association of the infant gut microbiome with early childhood neurodevelopmental outcomes: an ancillary study to the VDAART randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2019; 2, e190905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, AL, Xia, K, Azcarate-Peril, MA, et al. Infant gut microbiome associated with cognitive development. Biol Psychiatry. 2018; 83, 148159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothenberg, SE, Chen, Q, Shen, J, et al. Neurodevelopment correlates with gut microbiota in a cross-sectional analysis of children at 3 years of age in rural China. Sci Rep. 2021; 11, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, KA, Bales, KL, Capitanio, JP, et al. Why primate models matter. Am J Primatol. 2014; 76, 801827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dettmer, AM, Suomi, SJ, Hinde, K. Nonhuman primate models of mental health. In Ancestral Landscapes in Human Evolution: Culture, Childrearing and Social Wellbeing, 2014;  pp. 4258. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, SA. A field study of the sociology of rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1962; 102, 338435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, HF, Lauersdorf, HE. Sex differences in passion and play. Perspect Biol Med. 1974; 17, 348360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, SJ. Attachment in rhesus monkeys. In Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (eds. J. Cassidy and P. Shaver), 1999; pp. 181197. New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Murphy, AM, Dettmer, AM. Impacts of early social experience on cognitive development in infant rhesus macaques. Dev Psychobiol. 2020; 62, 895908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laue, HE, Korrick, SA, Baker, ER, Karagas, MR, Madan, JC. Prospective associations of the infant gut microbiome and microbial function with social behaviors related to autism at age 3 years. Sci Rep. 2020; 10, 15515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, ML, Moore, CF, Suomi, SJ, Champoux, M. Laboratory assessment of temperament and environmental enrichment in rhesus monkey infants (Macaca mulatta). Am J Primatol. 1991; 25, 137155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dettmer, AM, Murphy, AM, Suomi, SJ. Development of a cognitive testing apparatus for socially housed mother-peer-reared infant rhesus monkeys: cognitive testing of mother-reared infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2015; 57, 349355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schloss Patrick, D, Westcott Sarah, L, Ryabin, T, et al. Introducing mothur: open-source, platform-independent, community-supported software for describing and comparing microbial communities. Appl Environ Microbiol. 2009; 75, 75377541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sackett, G, Ruppenthal, G, Hewitson, L, Simerly, C, Schatten, G. Neonatal behavior and infant cognitive development in rhesus macaques produced by assisted reproductive technologies. Dev Psychobiol. 2006; 48, 243265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dettmer, AM, Murphy, AM, Guitarra, D, et al. Cortisol in neonatal mother’s milk predicts later infant social and cognitive functioning in rhesus monkeys. Child Dev. 2018; 89, 525538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallick, H, Rahnavard, A, Mclever, LJ, et al. Multivariable association discovery in population-scale meta-omics studies. PLOS Comput Biol. 2021; 17, e1009442. Coelho LP, editor.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMurdie, PJ, Holmes, S. phyloseq: An R package for reproducible interactive analysis and graphics of microbiome census data. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8, e61217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W, . psych: Procedures for Personality and Pscyhological Research [Internet] . 2023. Evanston, Illinois, Northwestern University. R Version 2.4.1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.Google Scholar
Schwager, E, Weingart, G, Bielski, C, Huttenhower, C. CCREPE: compositionality corrected by permutation and Renormalization. R/Bioconductor https://doi.org/10.18129B.2014;9 Google Scholar
Oksanen, J, Blanchet, FG, Friendly, M, Kindt, R, Legendre, P, McGlinn, D. Vegan: community ecology package (2.5-7) [Computer software]. 2019.Google Scholar
Bullard, JH, Purdom, E, Hansen, KD, Dudoit, S. Evaluation of statistical methods for normalization and differential expression in mRNA-Seq experiments. BMC Bioinformatics. 2010; 11, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casals-Pascual, C, González, A, Vázquez-Baeza, Y, Song, SJ, Jiang, L, Knight, R. Microbial diversity in clinical microbiome studies: sample size and statistical power considerations. Gastroenterology. 2020; 158, 15241528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopal, PK, Sullivan, PA, Smart, JB. Utilisation of galacto-oligosaccharides as selective substrates for growth by lactic acid bacteria including, Bifidobacterium lactis, DR10 and, Lactobacillus rhamnosus, DR20. Int Dairy J. 2001; 11, 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijman, RM, Liu, Y, Bunyatratchata, A, Smilowitz, JT, Stahl, B, Barile, D. Characterization and quantification of oligosaccharides in human milk and infant formula. J Agric Food Chem. 2018; 66, 68516859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kukkonen, K, Savilahti, E, Haahtela, T, et al. Probiotics and prebiotic galacto-oligosaccharides in the prevention of allergic diseases: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2007; 119, 192198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, T, Long, W, Zhang, C, Liu, S, Zhao, L, Hamaker, BR. Fiber-utilizing capacity varies in Prevotella- versus Bacteroides-dominated gut microbiota. Sci Rep. 2017; 7, 2594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, T, Li, Y, Liang, J, Li, Y, Huang, Z. Gut microbiota of provisioned and wild rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) living in a limestone forest in southwest Guangxi, China. MicrobiologyOpen. 2020; 9, e981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abulizi X, Pryor L, Michel G, Melchior M, Van Der Waerden J, on behalf of The EDEN Mother-Child Cohort Study Group. Temperament in infancy and behavioral and emotional problems at age 5.5: the EDEN mother-child cohort. In PLOS ONE. vol. 12, (eds Brown, S), 2017; pp. e0171971.Google Scholar
Zeanah, CH. Handbook of infant mental health, 2018. Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Rothbart, MK, Posner, MI. Temperament and the development of self-regulation. In The Neuropsychology of Individual Differences: A Developmental Perspective [Internet] (eds. Hartlage, LC, Telzrow, CF), 1985; pp. 93123. Springer US, Boston, MA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gao, W, Salzwedel, AP, Carlson, AL, et al. Gut microbiome and brain functional connectivity in infants-a preliminary study focusing on the amygdala. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2019; 236, 16411651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, X, Zang, T, Liu, J, et al. Changes in the gut microbiome in the first two years of life predicted the temperament in toddlers. J Affect Disord. 2023; 333, 342352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, JB, Gomez, A, Amato, K, et al. The gut microbiome of nonhuman primates: lessons in ecology and evolution. Am J Primatol. 2018; 80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MetaHIT Consortium (additional members), Arumugam M, Raes J, Pelletier E, et al. Enterotypes of the human gut microbiome. Nature. 2011; 473, 174180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yatsunenko, T, Rey, FE, Manary, MJ, et al. Human gut microbiome viewed across age and geography. Nature. 2012; 486, 222227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, L, Hinde, K, Tao, L. Species diversity and relative abundance of lactic acid bacteria in the milk of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Med Primatol. 2011; 40, 5258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, P, Curtis, N. Breast milk microbiota: a review of the factors that influence composition. J Infect. 2020; 81, 1747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Togo, A, Dufour, JC, Lagier, JC, Dubourg, G, Raoult, D, Million, M. Repertoire of human breast and milk microbiota: a systematic review. Future Microbiol. 2019; 14, 623641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qi, C, Zhou, J, Tu, H, et al. Lactation-dependent vertical transmission of natural probiotics from the mother to the infant gut through breast milk. Food Funct. 2022; 13, 304315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, KVA, Watson, KK, Dunbar, RIM, Burnet, PWJ. Sociability in a non-captive macaque population is associated with beneficial gut bacteria. Front Microbiol. 2022; 13, 1032495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M, Lee, SM, Wiley, KS, et al.. Development of the infant gut microbiome predicts temperament across the first year of life. Dev Psychopathol. 2022; 34, 19141925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messaoudi, M, Violle, N, Bisson, JF, Desor, D, Javelot, H, Rougeot, C. Beneficial psychological effects of a probiotic formulation ( Lactobacillus helveticus, R0052 and, Bifidobacterium longum, R0175) in healthy human volunteers. Gut Microbes. 2011; 2, 256261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughman, A, Ponsonby, AL, O’Hely, M, et al. Gut microbiota composition during infancy and subsequent behavioural outcomes. EBioMedicine. 2020; 52, 102640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, DW, Park, JG, Ilhan, ZE, et al. Reduced incidence of Prevotella and other fermenters in intestinal microflora of autistic children. In PLoS ONE. vol. 8, (eds. Gilbert, JA), 2013; pp. e68322.Google Scholar
Borre, YE, Moloney, RD, Clarke, G, Dinan, TG, Cryan, JF. The impact of microbiota on brain and behavior: mechanisms & therapeutic potential. In Microbial Endocrinology: The Microbiota-Gut-Brain Axis in Health and Disease [Internet]. vol. 817, (eds. Lyte, M, Cryan, JF), 2014; pp. 373403. Springer New York, New York, NY. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. Available from: https://link.springer.com/10.1007/978-1-4939-0897-4_17 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinan, TG, Cryan, JF. Microbes, immunity, and behavior: psychoneuroimmunology meets the microbiome. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017; 42, 178192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bokulich, NA, Chung, J, Battaglia, T, et al. Antibiotics, birth mode, and diet shape microbiome maturation during early life. Sci Transl Med. 2016; 8, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aatsinki, AK, Lahti, L, Uusitupa, HM, et al. Gut microbiota composition is associated with temperament traits in infants. Brain Behav Immun. 2019; 80, 849858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, AL, Xia, K, Azcarate-Peril, MA, et al.Infant gut microbiome composition is associated with non-social fear behavior in a pilot study. Nat Commun. 2021; 12, 3294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelsey, CM, Prescott, S, McCulloch, JA, et al. Gut microbiota composition is associated with newborn functional brain connectivity and behavioral temperament. Brain Behav Immun. 2021; 91, 472486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmerman, HM, Rutten, NBMM, Boekhorst, J, et al. Intestinal colonisation patterns in breastfed and formula-fed infants during the first 12 weeks of life reveal sequential microbiota signatures. Sci Rep. 2017; 7, 8327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardeshir, A, Narayan, NR, Méndez-Lagares, G, et al. Breast-fed and bottle-fed infant rhesus macaques develop distinct gut microbiotas and immune systems. Sci Transl Med [Internet]. 2014; 6.Google ScholarPubMed
Manus, MB, Sardaro, MLS, Dada, O, et al. Interactions with alloparents are associated with the diversity of infant skin and fecal bacterial communities in Chicago, United States. Am J Hum Biol. 2023; e23972.Google ScholarPubMed
Wiley, KS, Gregg, AM, Fox, MM, et al. Contact with caregivers is associated with composition of the infant gastrointestinal microbiome in the first 6 months of life. Am J Biol Anthropol. 2024; 183, e24858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amato, KR, Van Belle, S, Di Fiore, A, et al. Patterns in gut microbiota similarity associated with degree of sociality among sex classes of a neotropical primate. Microb Ecol. 2017; 74, 250258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tung, J, Barreiro, LB, Burns, MB, et al. Social networks predict gut microbiome composition in wild baboons. eLife. 2015; 16, e05224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perofsky, AC, Lewis, RJ, Abondano, LA, Di Fiore, A, Meyers, LA. Hierarchical social networks shape gut microbial composition in wild verreaux’s sifaka. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2017; 284, 20172274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Degnan, PH, Pusey, AE, Lonsdorf, EV, et al. Factors associated with the diversification of the gut microbial communities within chimpanzees from Gombe National Park. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012; 109, 1303413039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cryan, JF, Dinan, TG. Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012; 13, 701712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinan, TG, Stilling, RM, Stanton, C, Cryan, JF. Collective unconscious: how gut microbes shape human behavior. J Psychiatr Res. 2015; 63, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choudhury, R, Middelkoop, A, Boekhorst, J, et al. Early life feeding accelerates gut microbiome maturation and suppresses acute post-weaning stress in piglets. Environ Microbiol. 2021; 23, 72017213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berding, K, Donovan, SM. Microbiome and nutrition in autism spectrum disorder: current knowledge and research needs. Nutr Rev. 2016; 74, 723736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics by infant’s age at microbiome sampling (mean [standard deviation] unless noted;* indicates % [proportion of sample])

Figure 1

Figure 1. Gut microbiome composition at phylum level (top 4 phyla)1.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Gut microbiome composition at family level (top 10 families)1.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Gut microbiome composition at genus level (top 10 genera)1.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Day 14 (MPR: n = 7; NR: n = 11; total: n = 18)1.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Day 30 (MPR: n = 9; NR: n = 10; total: n=19)1.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Day 180 (MPR: n = 8; NR: n = 12; total: n = 20)1.

Figure 7

Figure 7. Day 14 (MPR: n = 7; NR: n = 11; total: n = 18).

Figure 8

Figure 8. Day 30 (MPR: n = 9; NR: n = 10; total: n=19).

Figure 9

Figure 9. Day 180 (MPR: n = 8; NR: n = 12; total: n = 20).

Figure 10

Table 2. Multiple linear regression models with microbial co-abundance factors predicting neurodevelopment (Experiment 1)1

Figure 11

Table 3. Multiple linear regression models with microbial co-abundance factors predicting cognitive outcomes (Experiment 2)1

Figure 12

Table 4. Multiple linear regression models with Shannon diversity predicting neurodevelopment (Experiment 1)

Figure 13

Table 5. Multiple linear regression model with Shannon diversity predicting cognitive outcomes (Experiment 2)

Figure 14

Table 6. MaAsLin2 analysis results: associations between gut microbiome taxa at different ages postpartum and cognitive and neurodevelopmental measures across both rearing environments1 (Total: n = 33)

Supplementary material: File

Daiy et al. supplementary material

Daiy et al. supplementary material
Download Daiy et al. supplementary material(File)
File 320.3 KB