Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-fg9bn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-25T09:54:11.462Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The irrelevant-menu affect on valuation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Doron Sonsino*
Affiliation:
School of Business Administration, The College of Management, 7 Rabin Blvd., P.O.B. 9017, Rishon LeZion 75190, Israel

Abstract

Three experiments are designed to test if the level of irrelevant prizes in the menu has a positive (assimilation) or negative (contrast) effect on the perceived valuation of target objects. Familiar field prizes and binary lotteries over such prizes are placed within “more-expensive” and “less-expensive” menus. Subjects fill-in a sequence of binary choice problems to reveal their preference between given cash and a designated prize from the menu. Between-subject comparisons reveal that the prize-level in the menu positively affects perceived valuations in spite of procedural attempts to rule out menu-dependent preferences and prohibit experimenter bias. The effect also shows within-subject in auction experiments: the price that subjects are willing to pay for given monetary lotteries significantly increases with the average payoff in the irrelevant-menu. The bias finally manifests even when subjects are led to choose the target lottery, independently, from the underlying menu.

Information

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Economic Science Association 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi: 10.1007/s10683-010-9243-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Andersen, S., Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., & Rutström, E. E. (2006). Elicitation using multiple price list formats. Experimental Economics, 9(4), 383405.10.1007/s10683-006-7055-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2003). ‘Coherent arbitrariness’: stable demand curves without stable preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(1), 73105.10.1162/00335530360535153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bettman, J.R., Johnson, E.J., & Payne, J. W. (1990). A componential analysis of cognitive effort in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 111139.10.1016/0749-5978(90)90007-VCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birnbaum, M. H. (1999). How to show that 9 > 221: collect judgments in a between-subject design. Psychological Methods, 4(3), 243249.10.1037/1082-989X.4.3.243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunegon, K. J., Duchon, D., & Barton, S. L. (1992). Affect, risk and decision criticality: replication and extension in business settings. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53(3), 335351.Google Scholar
Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Kuschel, S. (2008). The effect of global versus local processing styles on assimilation versus contrast in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 579599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, W. H. (2003). Econometric analysis. New York: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. (1976). Within-subjects designs: to use or not to use. Psychological Bulletin, 83(2), 314320.10.1037/0033-2909.83.2.314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herne, K. (1999). The effects of decoy gambles on individual choice. Experimental Economics, 2(1), 3140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, L., Payne, J. W., & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 9098.10.1086/208899CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M. (2008). Positive affect and decision making. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L.F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Patrick, R. (1983). The effect of positive feelings on risk-taking: when the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 194202.10.1016/0030-5073(83)90120-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M., & Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: a cognitive loop. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(1), 112.10.1037/0022-3514.36.1.1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2007). Frames and brains: elicitation and control of response tendencies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 4546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeBoeuf, R. A., & Shafir, E. (2003). Deep thoughts and shallow frames: on the susceptibility to framing effects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 16(2), 7792.10.1002/bdm.433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D., & Raiffa, H. (1957). Games and decisions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mano, H. (1994). Risk-taking, framing effects and affect. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 216245.10.1016/0749-5978(92)90036-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzotta, M. J., & Opaluch, J. J. (1995). Decision-making when choices are complex: a test of Heiner's hypothesis. Land Economics, 71(4), 500515.10.2307/3146714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElroy, T., & Seta, J. J. (2003). Framing effects: an analytic-holistic perspective. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(6), 610617.10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00036-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKelvey, R. D., & Zavoina, W. (1975). A statistical model for the analysis of ordinal level dependent variables. Journal of Mathematical Sociology, 4, 103120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2000). The “relative self”: informational and judgmental consequences of comparative self-evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(1), 2338.10.1037/0022-3514.79.1.23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mussweiler, T., Ruter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The ups and downs of social comparison: mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(6), 832844.10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noor, J., & Takeoka, N. (2007). Menu dependent self control. Boston: Boston University.Google Scholar
Nygren, T. E., Isen, A.M., Taylor, P. J., & Dulin, J. (1996). The influence of positive affect on the decision rule in risk situations: focus on outcome (and especially avoidance of loss) rather than probability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(1), 5972.10.1006/obhd.1996.0038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its alternatives: an inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation and contrast effects in social judgment. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 217245). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stapel, D. A. (2007). In the mind of the beholder: the interpretation comparison model of accessibility effects. In Stapel, D. A. & Suls, J. (Eds.), Assimilation and contrast in social psychology (pp. 143164). New York: Psychological Press.Google Scholar
Stapel, D. A., & Koomen, W. (2001). I, we, and the effects of others on me: how self-construal moderates social comparison effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(5), 766781.10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.766CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wedell, D. H., & Pettibone, J. C. (1996). Using judgments to understand decoy effects in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67(3), 326344.10.1006/obhd.1996.0083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Houston, C. E., Etling, K. E., & Brekke, N. (1996). A new look at anchoring effects: basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(4), 387402.10.1037/0096-3445.125.4.387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zizzo, J. D. (2009). Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 13(1), 7598.10.1007/s10683-009-9230-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Sonsino supplementary material

Supplementary Appendix
Download Sonsino supplementary material(File)
File 228.9 KB