Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-qc88w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-10T04:05:31.085Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of smartphones in adolescent-parent discrepancy in reporting adolescents’ internalizing problems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 September 2025

Cory Carvalho*
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, USA
Kalsea Koss
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, USA
Niyantri Ravindran
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Science, University of Georgia, Athens, USA
*
Corresponding author: Cory Carvalho; Email: cory.carvalho@uga.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The current study examined how early smartphone ownership impacts parent-child informant discrepancy of youth internalizing problems during the transition to adolescence. We used four waves of longitudinal data (Years 1–4) from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD; Baseline N = 11,878; White = 52.0%, Hispanic = 20.3%, Black = 15.0%, Asian = 2.1%, Other = 10.5%; Female = 47.8%). Across the full sample, significant parent-child informant discrepancy, such that parents underestimated child reports, appeared at Year 2 (Mage = 12.0) and increased across the remainder of the study (b = −0.21, SE = .042, p < .001, 95%CI [−.29, −.23]). Further, multi-group models indicated that significant parent-child informant discrepancy emerged in the years following initial smartphone acquisition, whereas youth who remained non smartphone owners did not demonstrate such a pattern. Moreover, this discrepancy grew with additional years of smartphone ownership. This study contributes to the ongoing discourse on adolescent smartphone use and mental health by documenting a novel, longitudinally observed risk to timely parental detection of mental health problems by early smartphone ownership.

Information

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The release of the iPhone in 2007 marked a turning point in social interactions. Since then, adolescent smartphone ownership has steadily increased, with 43% of 8 – 12 year olds and 88% of 13 – 18 year olds owning smartphones by 2021 (Rideout & Robb, Reference Rideout and Robb2020). This rapid adoption has prompted research examining its impact on the parent-child relationship (Hawi & Samaha, Reference Hawi and Samaha2017). Smartphone use can reduce face-to-face communication, fragment attention, and isolate one from their immediate physical and social environment (Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, Kushlev and Dunn2018). Empirical evidence suggests that smartphones can degrade communication quality and compromise functioning in close relationships (McDaniel, Reference McDaniel and Bruess2014; Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Briskin and Slatcher2019). This impact on parent-child relationships may lead to fewer conversations about youths’ mental health, potentially impacting parent and youth reporting agreement of youth internalizing problems (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Chan, McCauley and Vander Stoep2016; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Doherty, Cannon, Hickey, Rosenthal, Marino and Skinner2019; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Schlomer, Lippold and Feinberg2021). Notably, early adolescence marks not only when smartphone ownership begins but also when internalizing problems, such as anxiety and depression, become more prevalent (Thapar et al., Reference Thapar, Collishaw, Pine and Thapar2012). Moreover, meta-analyses indicate that parent-adolescent informant discrepancy is more pronounced for internalizing problems than other types psychopathology (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). Parents tend to underestimate youth-reported internalizing symptoms, as they are less observable and often rely on parental solicitation or youth disclosure for detection (Kapetanovic et al., Reference Kapetanovic, Rothenberg, Lansford, Bornstein, Chang, Deater-Deckard, Di Giunta, Dodge, Gurdal, Malone, Oburu, Pastorelli, Skinner, Sorbring, Steinberg, Tapanya, Uribe Tirado, Yotanyamaneewong, Peña Alampay and Bacchini2020; Makol & Polo, Reference Makol and Polo2018). This raises the question: Does owning a smartphone in adolescence increase the likelihood of parent-child discrepancy in reporting internalizing problems? This is a critical concern because parents’ awareness of their children’s mental health is essential for timely intervention. Moreover, parent-child informant discrepancy of psychopathology is a known developmental risk factor, predisposing youth to impaired social functioning and mental health problems (Castagna et al., Reference Castagna, Calamia and Davis2021; Fabris et al., Reference Fabris, Marengo, Longobardi and Settanni2020; Goolsby et al., Reference Goolsby, Rich, Hinnant, Habayeb, Berghorst, De Los Reyes and Alvord2018; Koca & Saatçı, Reference Koca and Saatçı2022). Therefore, the present study aimed to examine the role of adolescent smartphone ownership in parent-child reporting discrepancy of youth internalizing problems.

Smartphone use in adolescence

Youth typically become smartphone owners between the age of 8 (31%) and 14 (72%) according to Rideout et al. (Reference Rideout, Peebles, Mann and Robb2022). Smartphones present adolescents with a unique developmental challenge. Neurobiologically, they experience significant changes that lead to increased novelty-seeking and risk-taking (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2007), more autonomy from parents (Spear & Kulbok, Reference Spear and Kulbok2004), and greater reliance on peers for social support (Farley & Kim-Spoon, Reference Farley and Kim-Spoon2014). At the same time, they experience trailing development in prefrontal brain regions that support executive control and emotion regulation (Romer et al., Reference Romer, Reyna and Satterthwaite2017). Concurrently, various addiction (Poudel & Gautam, Reference Poudel and Gautam2017) and mental health problems (Thapar et al., Reference Thapar, Collishaw, Pine and Thapar2012) tend to first emerge in adolescence as youth renegotiate their place among their peers and the broader social milieu. Amidst this sociobiological backdrop, smartphones present endless opportunities for social and entertainment stimulation. Further increasing their appeal, tech platforms present content through smartphone apps that are algorithmically designed to maximize the capture of users’ attention. Consequently, these smartphone qualities can negatively impact close relationships (Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Briskin and Slatcher2019), potentially interfering with parental detection of fluctuations in their youths’ mental health status.

Parent-child discrepancy in reporting adolescent psychosocial health problems

Using multiple informant methods to measure youth mental health is recognized as a strength in research and clinical practices (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). This is because parents and children sometimes report different yet valid information based on their unique perspectives (Karver, Reference Karver2006). However, empirical studies suggest the concordance between parent’s and child’s reports are typically low to moderate in adolescence across a range of psychosocial outcomes (Rescorla, Reference Rescorla2016), particularly for internalizing problems (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). Low rates of agreement partly reflect excessive reporting discrepancy and may indicate parents’ lack of awareness of their children’s mental health status (Aebi et al., Reference Aebi, Kuhn, Banaschewski, Grimmer, Poustka, Steinhausen and Goodman2017; Lagattuta et al., Reference Lagattuta, Sayfan and Bamford2012). Moreover, parents’ sensitivity to their children’s changing mental health is important for making remedial adjustments within the family and seeking early intervention (Goolsby et al., Reference Goolsby, Rich, Hinnant, Habayeb, Berghorst, De Los Reyes and Alvord2018) and greater informant discrepancy has been identified as a risk factor for youth well-being (Castagna et al., Reference Castagna, Calamia and Davis2021). However, longitudinal studies assessing trajectories of informant discrepancy in reporting internalizing problems across early adolescence are scarce.

Various contributing factors to higher parent-child reporting discrepancy have been identified (Kolko & Kazdin, Reference Kolko and Kazdin1993; Treutler & Epkins, Reference Treutler and Epkins2003), including relationship quality (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Chan, McCauley and Vander Stoep2016; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Doherty, Cannon, Hickey, Rosenthal, Marino and Skinner2019; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Schlomer, Lippold and Feinberg2021). For example, poorer parental engagement (Van Roy et al., Reference Van Roy, Groholt, Heyerdahl and Clench-Aas2010), less communication (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Bornstein, Putnick, Hendricks and Suwalsky2007; Van Roy et al., Reference Van Roy, Groholt, Heyerdahl and Clench-Aas2010), weaker maternal bonding (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Ho, Lee, Wu, Gau and van Luijtelaar2017), and lower parental monitoring (Laird & LaFleur, Reference Laird and LaFleur2016) have been associated with stronger disagreement. Further, qualitative studies suggest that discrepancy can emerge when parents are unaware of adolescents’ symptoms or misread their behavior (Bidaut-Russell et al., Reference Bidaut-Russell, Reich, Cottler, Robins, Compton and Mattison1995).

Smartphones and parent-child discrepancy

Smartphones appeal to parents and youth partly because they allow dyads the ability to communicate despite changes in proximity (Richter et al., Reference Richter, Adkins and Selkie2022). This aspect of smartphone ownership offers clear benefits as adolescents enjoy increasing autonomy and time separated from their parents (Zimmer-Gembeck & Collins, Reference Zimmer-Gembeck and Collins2006). With smartphones, parents can more easily monitor youths’ whereabouts (Weisskirch, Reference Weisskirch2009) and youth can conveniently convey information to their parents about changes in plans or safety concerns (Warren & Aloia, Reference Warren and Aloia2018).

Smartphones also have the potential to negatively impact the quality of interactions in close relationships (Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Briskin and Slatcher2019), including in the parent-child dyad (Stockdale et al., Reference Stockdale, Coyne and Padilla-Walker2018). As adolescents increasingly spend time with smartphones, face-to-face interactions can degrade in frequency and quality within the home (McDaniel, Reference McDaniel and Bruess2014) and feelings of being distant increase between parents and youth (Lanette, Reference Lanette2018). In fact, the mere presence of a smartphone has been shown to decrease feelings of connectedness between individuals in proximity (Misra et al., Reference Misra, Cheng, Genevie and Yuan2016; Przybylski & Weinstein, Reference Przybylski and Weinstein2013). Moreover, Davis et al. (Reference Davis, Dinhopl and Hiniker2019) found evidence supporting the theory that excessive adolescent smartphone use contributes to parent-teen disconnectedness.

Given the impact of smartphones on close relationships, youths’ access to smartphones may contribute to informant discrepancy in reporting internalizing problems between parents and adolescents. Smartphones have been shown to possess addictive qualities that capture youths’ attention (Yildirim et al., Reference Yildirim Demirdöğen, Akinci, Bozkurt, Bayraktutan, Turan, Aydoğdu, Ucuz, Abanoz, Yitik Tonkaz, Çakir and Ferahkaya2024), potentially reducing the frequency and quality of parent-child interactions. Reduced engagement provides fewer opportunities for parental solicitation and observation of affective cues. Moreover, even during interactions, smartphone use may fragment attention and diminish interaction quality (Lanette, Reference Lanette2018), further limiting parents’ ability to perceive internalizing cues. Consequently, adolescents’ preoccupation with smartphones may hinder parents’ awareness of their children’s internalizing problems.

Smartphone use may also decrease adolescents’ disclosure of internalizing problems to their parents. Social media, the most prominent smartphone activity, accounts for more than twice the daily usage time compared to other activities (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Linkersdörfer, Toda-Thorne, Sullivan, Cummins, Tomko, Allen, Bagot, Baker, Fuemmeler, Hoffman, Kiss, Mason, Nguyen-Louie, Tapert, Smith, Squeglia and Wade2024). By expanding the peer social environment beyond schools, extracurricular activities, and social outings to virtually all locations, including the home, social media blurs the boundaries between peer interactions and family time. Previously, adolescents could separate socializing with peers from debriefing with parents at home. Now, they carry their entire social network in their pockets (Kushlev et al., Reference Kushlev, Dwyer and Dunn2019). This constant access enables adolescents to share emotional burdens with peers at any time of day. Although peer support has its benefits, unrestricted access to peer networks may reduce adolescents’ reliance on parental support, leaving parents less informed about their child’s internalizing problems and the contextual factors influencing it.

Youth are acquiring smartphones at increasingly younger ages, and early smartphone ownership has been linked to a range of maladaptive developmental outcomes (Sapien Labs, 2023). Given evidence that smartphones can disrupt close relationships (Lanette, Reference Lanette2018; Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Briskin and Slatcher2019), early smartphone ownership may contribute to parent-child informant discrepancy regarding youth internalizing problems. Specifically, earlier smartphone adoption could influence parent-child relational dynamics during a period of heightened psychological vulnerability due to normative developmental changes. Therefore, it is essential to investigate whether early smartphone ownership is associated with parent-child discrepancy in reporting youth internalizing problems across early adolescence.

The current study

Trajectories of parent-child discrepancy in reporting of youth internalizing problems are understudied in early adolescent youth. Moreover, smartphones can diminish the frequency and quality of interactions in close relationships (Sbarra et al., Reference Sbarra, Briskin and Slatcher2019), potentially limiting opportunities for child disclosure and parental solicitation of internalizing concerns. This could increase parent-child reporting discrepancy of youth internalizing problems. However, the role of early smartphone ownership in parent-child informant discrepancy has not yet been examined. To address this gap, we used a multi-level framework to analyze smartphone ownership age and parent- and child-reported internalizing problems using data from the ABCD Study (Y1: M age = 10.9, Y2: M age = 10.9, Y3: M age = 12.9 Y4: M age = 14.1). This approach is recommended for modeling longitudinal informant discrepancy as an outcome (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Ohannessian and Laird2016) as it addresses the limitations of difference scores analyses (see Edwards, Reference Edwards1994; Laird & LaFleur, Reference Laird and LaFleur2016 for review). First, we examined average changes in discrepancy over time to show the general trajectory of parent-adolescent informant discrepancy across early adolescence in the whole sample. We hypothesized that informant discrepancy would increase across early adolescence. Then, we tested whether overall parent-child reporting discrepancy of youth internalizing problems (i.e., averaged across all time points) varies by age of smartphone ownership across early adolescence after controlling for parental education, youth biological sex, stage of pubertal development, and overall screen time. We hypothesized that discrepancy, such that parents underestimate youths’ internalizing problems, would be higher with earlier smartphones ownership. Lastly, we examined changes in informant discrepancy over time based on the age of onset of youth smartphone ownership. We hypothesized there would be a significant moderating effect of time on informant discrepancy among youth who own smartphones. Further, we hypothesized that the informant effects would be more pronounced in each group for the measurement occasions after youth receive a smartphone. If supported, this would suggest that smartphone ownership contributes to increasing parent-child reporting discrepancy across early adolescence implying smartphone impacts on parent-child relationship dynamics.

Method

We used data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study to address study aims. The ABCD study recruited 11,876 adolescents aged 9 – 10 at baseline and their primary caregivers beginning in 2015 with follow-up data collection planned every 6 months for a total of 10 years. Overall, data collected consists of neuroimaging, cognitive-tasks, and survey data. Data for the current study was from the 5.1 release which includes approximately half of the data from the most recent wave (i.e., Year 4 in the current study) according to the ABCD Study’s planned missingness data release design.

Participants

For the current study, survey data from complete parent-adolescent dyads were used from the baseline (Y0: N = 11,876; M age = 9.9), one-year follow-up (Y1: N = 11,219; M age = 10.9), two-year follow-up (Y2: N = 10,972; M age = 12.0), three-year follow-up (Y3: N = 10,335; M age = 12.9), and four-year follow-up (Y4: N = 4,754; M age = 14.1). The sample was designed to be a representative sample of US adolescents (White = 52.0%, Hispanic = 20.3%, Black = 15.0%, Asian = 2.1%, Other = 10.5%; Female = 47.8%). Parent surveys were completed mostly by biological mothers (85.3%; 10.0% biological fathers; 2.3% adoptive parents; 1.0% custodial parent; 1.4% other) and participants were instructed to include the same parent at each measurement occasion.

Measures

Age of smartphone ownership

Parents were asked several questions regarding their child’s smartphone ownership. First, they were asked whether or not their child owns a smartphone. If parents responded “No” to this item at Y4, then youth were assigned a value of “5” representing no smartphone ownership. In addition, parents were asked annually at what age did their children first own a cell phone. If answers changed throughout the study, then the earlier response was used. Then, smartphone ownership data was recoded according to the following categories based on youths age at each measurement occasion: 8 & under, 9 & 10, 11 & 12, 13 & 14, and No Phone.

Child internalizing problems (parent- and youth-reported)

Internalizing problems were reported by parents using the 112-item Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Edelbrock, Reference Achenbach and Edelbrock1991) and by youth using the 18-item Brief Problem Monitor (BPM; Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, McConaughy, Ivanova and Rescorla2011), which is a companion to the CBCL. The BPM was first administered at the one-year follow-up wave. For each scale, reporters responded to symptoms of internalizing problems according to a 3-item Likert format (0 = Not true, 1 = Somewhat/ Sometimes true, 2 = Very true). To model discrepancies, internalizing problems for the current study are limited to the six items that are present in both the BPM and the CBCL. These shared items are characterized as anxious/depressed (3 items) or withdrawn/depressed (3 items) aspects of internalizing problems (see Supplemental Table 1 for specific items) according the scale’s syndrome scoring protocol (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, McConaughy, Ivanova and Rescorla2011). Internal reliabilities for these six items were acceptable at each timepoint and higher for parents (α1 = .756; α2 = .762; α3 = .776; α4 = .783) than youth (α1 = .619; α2 = .615; α3 = .619; α4 = .616). Internalizing problems distributions were right skewed consistent with prevalence rates for internalizing problems during adolescence; however, observed data span the full range of possible scores (Min = 0, Max = 12) represented at each timepoint for each reporter (see Supplemental Figure 1 for distributions).

Covariates

Parental education and youth biological sex were included as between-level control variables. Parental education level was operationalized as: (1) Did not graduate high school, (2) High school diploma or GED, (3) Some college/Associate’s degree; (4) Bachelor’s degree; and (5) Graduate degree. Further, the effects of pubertal development and overall screentime were modeled at the within-level as time-varying covariates as well as at the between-level by regressing internalizing problems on the respective person-level means. Pubertal development was reported by parents at each time point using the Pubertal Development Scale (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Magis-Weinberg, Guazzelli Williamson, Ladouceur, Whittle, Herting, Uban, Byrne, Barendse, Shirtcliff and Pfeifer2021; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Crockett, Richards and Boxer1988). Parents responded to five items regarding various physical changes related to puberty (e.g., growth spurts and body hair) on a 4-point Likert scale from “has not begun” (1) to “seems complete” (4). Two of the five items were specific to boys or girls, respectively. An average value for each timepoint was used in analyses. Youth reported their screentime via four items from the Screen Time Questionnaire (Bagot et al., Reference Bagot, Tomko, Marshall, Hermann, Cummins, Ksinan, Kakalis, Breslin, Lisdahl, Mason, Redhead, Squeglia, Thompson, Wade, Tapert, Fuemmeler and Baker2022) targeting their total weekday and weekend hours and minutes screen usage for any non-educational purpose. A weighted mean (i.e., 2/7 weekend + 5/7 weekday) was calculated to represent their average total screentime. Values were winsorized to ± 3SD from the mean. Youth-reported screentime has been demonstrated to be more reliable than parents in an ABCD Study subsample (Wade et al., Reference Wade, Ortigara, Sullivan, Tomko, Breslin, Baker, Fuemmeler, Delrahim Howlett, Lisdahl, Marshall, Mason, Neale, Squeglia, Wolff-Hughes, Tapert and Bagot2021) when compared against objective passive sensing measures (N = 67, Youth r = .49, p < .001, Parent r = .10, p = .43).

Analytic plan

All study analyses were conducted using Mplus version 8.3. Attrition remained modest for longitudinal variables through Y3 (∼15% drop from baseline for parent- and youth-reported internalizing problems and 17.9% for smartphone ownership age). Analyses of missing data patterns revealed that boys (F = 10.4, p = .001) and youth from parents with lower education (F = 154.2, p < .001) were more likely to drop out after Y1. These variables were included as covariates and thus missing data were treated as Missing at Random (MAR). Additionally, consistent with ABCD’s data release strategy, roughly half the sample had Y4 data, and all available data were included in analyses with the unreleased data estimated and assumed MAR as this release strategy is consistent with planned missingness designs. Thus, missing data were handled using full information maximum likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) estimation because it produces unbiased parameter estimates with non-normal data (Yuan & Bentler, Reference Yuan and Bentler2000). First, means and correlations among study variables were examined to assess overall sample characteristics and bivariate associations at each time point.

Second, we tested the overall and change in informant discrepancy for the whole sample.

Empirical studies often use XY difference scores (e.g., simple, absolute, standardized) to evaluate reporting discrepancies. However, critiques have identified limitations that undermine the validity of this approach (Edwards, Reference Edwards1994; Laird & De Los Reyes, Reference Laird and De Los Reyes2013). For example, if reporters demonstrate unequal variance across the sample (e.g., parents vs. youth), then the results are driven primarily by the reporter with more variability (Edwards, Reference Edwards1994). Second, difference scores often artificially reduce variance compared to their components which can reduce statistical power. Third, because they are a composite of two values they inherit the accumulation of measurement error from both values, decreasing reliability (Edwards, Reference Edwards1994). Fourth, difference scores introduce ambiguity when interpreting effects because the same difference score can result from various combinations of X and Y (Laird & De Los Reyes, Reference Laird and De Los Reyes2013). As an alternative, polynomial regression (i.e., for cross-sectional models) and multi-level modeling (i.e., for longitudinal models) have been shown to be superior (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Ohannessian and Laird2016) and were used in the current study as described in the following paragraphs.

Thus, we conducted a series of longitudinal models to examine whether significant parent-child informant discrepancy in reporting youths’ internalizing problems was present in the sample and whether this discrepancy changed across time. We used multi-level modeling to distinguish between-dyad variance from within-dyad variance across time as is recommended by De Los Reyes et al. (Reference De Los Reyes, Ohannessian and Laird2016). Internalizing problems were modeled as the outcome variable with informant (i.e., child = 0 and parent = 1), Time, and the Time × Informant product modeled as predictors. In building the model, we first determined in the full sample whether fixed or random effects for Time and Informant provided the better fit using the Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square difference test (Satorra & Bentler, Reference Satorra and Bentler2001). This test is used in place of the standard likelihood ratio test when MLR estimation is used due to the presence of non-normality. Then, in the first model, we tested the within-level direct effects of Time and Informant on internalizing problems. Pubertal development and overall screen time at each wave were included at the within-person level as a time-varying fixed effects while youth sex and parental education were included as time-invariant covariates at the between-level. Then, we added the Time × Informant product term at the within-level to test whether informant discrepancy changed across time. In this final model, β 0i represents the mean of child’s internalizing problems when time is zero (e.g., the one-year follow-up) and the informant is the child, β 1i represents the effect of Time for individual i at time j, β 2i represents the effect of informant k on individual i (i.e., parent-child informant discrepancy), and β 3i represents the moderating effect of time i on the effect of informant k on internalizing problems. In this model, observations are nested within individuals where time j and informant k are level 1 predictors.

Level 1:

Internalizing Problemsijk = β 0i + β 1i(Timeij) + β 2i(Informantk) + β 3i(Timeij × Informantk) + εijk

Level 2:

β 0i = γ0 + u0i

β 1i = γ1 + u1i

β 2i = γ2 + u2i

β 3i = γ3 + u3i

Next, we conducted the above outlined procedure in multi-group models based on age of smartphone ownership to determine whether the informant effects varied across these smartphone age-based groups. We then probed the interactions to further examine how informant discrepancy changed across time for each group. Propensity weights provided by the ABCD Study were also accounted for in all models to calibrate distributions to nationally representative controls based on the American Community survey to increase the generalizability of findings (see Heeringa & Berglund, Reference Heeringa and Berglund2020; Saragosa-Harris et al., Reference Saragosa-Harris, Chaku, MacSweeney, Guazzelli Williamson, Scheuplein, Feola, Cardenas-Iniguez, Demir-Lira, McNeilly, Huffman, Whitmore, Michalska, Damme, Rakesh and Mills2022 for more information).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Means, standard deviations, and correlations among study variables for the whole sample are displayed in Table 1. Parental education was positively correlated with age of cellphone ownership (r = .23, p < .001), indicating that youth with more educated parents tended to acquire smartphones at older ages. Girls generally obtained smartphones earlier than boys (r = .07, p < .001). Internalizing problems increased across the four measurement occasions, with youth consistently reporting higher levels than their parents at each time point.

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of study variables

Note. Y = Youth-report; P = Parent-report; Internal = Internalizing problems. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Table 2 shows the size, proportional demographics, and mean internalizing problems for each cellphone age group. Most youth were in the 9&10 (N = 2,641) and 11&12 (N = 5,037) groups while less membership was found in the 8 & under (N = 677), 13&14 (N = 744), and no phone (N = 376) groups. There was similar representation between boys and girls in the early smartphone ownership groups (8 & under: 53% girls, χ2 = 2.77, df = 1, p = .096; 9&10 : 51% girls, χ2 = 2.47, df = 1, p = .116) but more boys in the remaining groups (11&12: 48% girls, χ2 = 7.55, df = 1, p = .006; 13&14: 39% girls, χ2 = 31.15, df = 1, p < .001; No Phone: 37% girls, χ2 = 23.50, df = 1, p > .001). Youth with parents who had higher education tended to be proportionally larger in groups in which youth began smartphone ownership later or did not yet own a smartphone. ANOVA tests revealed significant differences across groups on all variables in Table 2 (Parent education: F = 146.40, df = 4, p < .001; Youth-reported internalizing problems: Y2 F min = 26.08, p < .001, Y4 F max = 65.43, df = 4, p < .001; Parent-reported internalizing problems: Y1 F min = 14.84, p = .002, Y2 F max = 20.13, df = 4, p < .001), except for parent-reported internalizing problems at Y4 (F = 8.69, df = 4, p = .060).

Table 2. Group means for internalizing problems across time and demographic covariates

Note. F = female; M = male.

For internalizing problems, youth reported higher levels of internalizing problems than parents at all time points, except for the No Phone group. Also, in each of these groups, youth reported higher levels of internalizing problems at each consecutive year. In contrast for youth who did not own a smartphone, youth reported higher levels of internalizing problems at Y1 and Y4, parents and youth reported the same levels at Y2, and parents reported higher levels at Y3. Notably, after Y1, although youth reported higher mean levels of internalizing problems with earlier smartphone ownership, at the same time their parents generally reported lower mean levels, respectively.

A post-hoc multi-group latent growth curve analysis (see Figure 1) revealed that youth-reported internalizing problems showed significant positive slopes over time in all groups with the exception of the No Phone group (8 & under: b = .39, β = .29, SE = .12, p = .001; 9&10: b = .19, β = .29, SE = .02, p < .001; 11&12: b = .20, β = .32, SE = .02, p < .001; 13&14: b = .21, β = .39, SE = .06, p < .001; No Phone: b = −.05, β = −.09, SE = .04, p = .216). However, parent-reported internalizing problems showed significant negative slopes in the 8 & under (b = −.07, β = −.24, SE = .03, p = .013) and No Phone (b = −.07, β = −.19, SE = .03, p = .044) groups, yet non-significant growth in the remaining groups (9&10: b = .00, β = .01, SE = .01, p = .858; 11&12: b = .01, β = .02, SE = .02, p = .439; 13&14: b = −.02, β = −.06, SE = .02, p = .308). Further, within-time correlations between parent and child reports of internalizing problems increased across the three years in the full sample (Y1: r = .25, p < .001; Y2: r = .30, p < .001; Y3: r = .34, p < .001; Y4: r = .38, p < .001).

Figure 1. Multi-group latent growth curve analysis of parent- and child-reported youth internalizing problems by age of cell phone ownership. Note. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Multi-level parent-child reporting discrepancy of internalizing problems models

Full sample random and fixed effects comparison

The Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square difference test was conducted to compare the fixed effects and random effects models of the full sample. Test results showed that modeling random effects significantly improved the model over fixed effects (χSB 2(2) = 8753.51, p < .001). After controlling for parent education, youth sex, and within-level pubertal development, there were significant effects of time (b = 0.04, SE = 0.01, p < .001) and informant (Youth = 0, Parent = 1; b = −0.47, SE = .02, p < .001) on internalizing problems across the full sample. This suggests that overall (i.e., parent- and self-reports collectively) internalizing problems increased across the four-wave period and that youth reported higher levels on average. Further, there was a significant negative effect of the product between time and informant on internalizing problems (b = −0.21, SE = .042, p < .001, 95%CI [−.29, −.23]), suggesting informant discrepancy increased with youth age across the whole sample. Probing the interaction effect (see Figure 2) revealed a significant negative informant effect (Youth = 0, Parent = 1) at baseline (Y1: M age = 10.9, b = −.08, SE = .03, p = .009) that increased across all subsequent timepoints (Y2: M age = 10.9, b = −.34, SE = .03, p < .001; Y3: M age = 12.9, b = −.60, SE = .04, p < .001; Y4: M age = 14.1, b = −.86, SE = .06, p < .001).

Figure 2. Predicted values of informant effect by age of smartphone ownership group. Note. A negative informant effect indicates parent-report underestimated youth-report. Significant informant effects are indicated with circles (p < .05).

Multi-group direct effects of time and informant on youth internalizing problems

Parameter estimates for the direct effects of time and informant on youth internalizing problems are presented in Table 3. For within-level covariate effects, total screen time was related to internalizing problems in the 8 & under, 9&10, and 11&12 groups, but not in the 13&14 and No Phone groups. Moreover, pubertal development was related to internalizing problems in only the 9&10 group. For between-level covariate effects, girls reported significantly more overall internalizing problems in all groups, except for the No Phone group. Moreover, higher parent education was related to more overall internalizing problems in the 9&10 group and 11&12 groups, but not in the 8 & under, 13&14, and No Phone groups. Further, mean person-level total screen time was related to more internalizing problems in all groups, except for 8&under.

Table 3. Parameter estimates for multi-level models examining the impact of time, informant, and time × informant on internalizing problems based on age of smartphone ownership

Note. IP = Internalizing problems, Pedu = Parent’s education, PD = Pubertal development, ST = Screen time.

Direct effects of time (i.e., wave of data collection) and informant (i.e., youth = 0 or parent = 1) are shown in Table 3 prior to adding the interaction (i.e., time × informant) to the model. Time was positively related to internalizing problems for all current cellphone ownership groups, though only significantly for 9&10 and 11&12 (8 & under: b = .03, SE = .03 p = .426; 9 – 10: b = .06, SE = .02, p = .002; 11 – 12: b = .09, SE = .01, p < .001, 13 – 14: b = .03, SE = .03, p = .276). This suggests that the combined average parent- and youth-reported internalizing problems increased across the 3-year period for all groups in which youth owned a smartphone. In contrast, a negative, non-significant association between time and internalizing problems was found in the No Phone group (b = −.09, SE = .05, p = .068).

For overall informant discrepancy, a pattern emerged in which the negative effect of informant (Youth = 0, Parent = 1) on average internalizing problems across all timepoints was increasingly more pronounced the earlier youth began smartphone ownership. This effect was significant for all groups (8 & under: b = −.85, SE = .08, p < .001; 9 – 10: b = -.68, SE = .04, p < .001; 11-12: b = −.44, SE = .17, p < .001; 13 – 14: b = −.27, SE = .03, p < .001), except for the No Phone group (b = −.04, SE = .06, p = .694). This result suggests that parents reported significantly fewer internalizing problems overall in their children than their children self-reported in all groups, except for in the No Phone group, indicating that reporting discrepancy increased as youth smartphone ownership age decreased. Post-hoc Wald Tests revealed that the informant effect was statistically different for the following comparisons: 9&10 vs. 11&12 (χ2(1) = 26.98, p < .001); 11&12 vs. 13&14 (χ2(1) = 5.83, p = .015); 13&14 vs. No Phone (χ2(1) = 3.93, p = .040).

The moderating effect of time on informant discrepancy

Significant interaction (Time × Informant) effects on internalizing problems (see Table 3) were found for all groups except for the No Phone (8 & under: b = −.25, SE = .05, p < .001, 95%CI [−.35, −.15]; 9 – 10: b = −.19, SE = .02, p < .001, 95%CI [−.23, −.14]; 11 – 12: b = −.20, SE = .01, p < .001, 95%CI [−.23, −.16]; 13 – 14: b = −.20, SE = .04, p < .001, 95%CI [−.27, −.14]; No Phone: b = −.02, SE = .05, p = .747, 95%CI [−.12, .09]). These results suggest that parent-child reporting discrepancy increased across time groups where youth owned cell phones. Specifically, parents increasingly reported less youth internalizing problems than their children as participants progressed through the study in these groups. Probing the interaction (see Figure 2 and Table 4) effect in each group revealed there was significant negative informant discrepancy (Youth = 0, Parent = 1) at all timepoints (Y1: M age = 10.9, Y2: M age = 10.9, Y3: M age = 12.9, Y4: M age = 14.1) for the 8 & under and 9&10 groups. Then, informant discrepancy was significant at Y2-Y4 for the 11&12 group and at Y4 for the 13&14 group. Last, informant discrepancy was not significant at any timepoint for the No Phone group. These results suggest that negative informant discrepancy (i.e., parent-report underestimating youth-report) coincided with age of smartphone use and continued to increase in the years after youth received their first smartphone.

Table 4. Probe of the effect of the time × informant product on internalizing problems

Note. M age = Mean age at measurement occasion. b = the effect of informant on internalizing problems. Significant effects indicate the informant effect is statistically nonzero.

Discussion

Parents navigate a historically novel and evolving digital context for raising early adolescent youth. A large portion of social interactions transition online as youth progress from childhood through adolescence, with implications for social development. Smartphones provide entertainment and social connectivity that align with adolescents’ changing interests, making ownership highly desirable, especially as peers adopt them. Yet, time tested norms have yet to establish around youth smartphone access and the long-term impacts of early smartphone adoption are not well understood. Evidence is mixed regarding the impact of smartphone use on youth internalizing problems. Some report null effects (Lapierre et al., Reference Lapierre, Zhao and Custer2019), while others suggest there are mental health costs associated with smartphone use (Coyne et al., Reference Coyne, Stockdale and Summers2019; Wacks & Weinstein, Reference Wacks and Weinstein2021), particularly at higher levels (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Kamper-DeMarco, Zhang, Xiao, Dong and Xue2022). Regardless, early detection of internalizing problems is important for preventing further decline, and parents play a central role in this process. The current study provides early evidence that this early detection may be increasingly compromised in early adolescence as youth become smartphone owners at earlier ages, even after controlling for total screen use. First, we showed that informant discrepancy such that parents underestimated youths’ report of their internalizing problems increased across early adolescence across the whole sample. Second, it was more likely that parents underestimated their child’s overall internalizing problems across early adolescence if youth owned smartphones at earlier ages. Then, we also found that this pattern of reporting discrepancy became more prevalent across time for smartphone owners, but not for those who did not own a smartphone. Moreover, parent-child informant discrepancy was statistically significant only in years after youth owned a smartphone.

Consistent with our first hypothesis, our results showed that parents’ underestimation of youth-reported internalizing problems increased across early adolescence in the whole sample. This finding builds on prior cross-sectional empirical evidence demonstrating that parents and adolescent reports often disagree regarding youths’ internalizing problems (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). Compared to other behavior problems, internalizing problems can be more covert making them challenging for parents to observe as they manifest. This is particularly relevant in early adolescence because these types of problems tend to first emerge as a relevant clinical factor at this stage (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2019). Adding to this challenge, early adolescence marks the initiation of substantial changes in the parent-child relationship as youth experience increasing autonomy (Spear & Kulbok, Reference Spear and Kulbok2004) and reliance on peer support (Brown & Larson, Reference Brown and Larson2009). While these changes are normative and healthy, parents may become less knowledgeable of or slow to recognize changes in their children’s internal state as new relational patterns organize (Granic et al., Reference Granic, Dishion, Hollenstein and Patterson2003). Our findings suggest, on average, parents’ underestimation of youths’ internalizing problems often appears at the start of early adolescence and increases in subsequent years. Notably, this pattern for internalizing problems is distinct from findings by Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Schlomer, Lippold and Feinberg2021) for externalizing problems in which parents reported more symptoms than youth, with informant discrepancy decreasing across early adolescence. To our knowledge, this is among the first longitudinal studies documenting this developmental trend for internalizing problems in this age group.

Results supported our second hypothesis that average parent-child informant discrepancy (i.e., the direct effect of informant on overall internalizing problems) would be more pronounced among youth who acquired smartphones earlier. In all groups except the No Phone group, youth reported significantly higher internalizing problems than their parents. Moreover, informant discrepancy followed a pattern in which the effect was larger in groups characterized by earlier smartphone ownership, respectively. In contrast, no significant informant discrepancy was observed in the No Phone group. This finding suggests that parent-child reporting differences may have a trait-like component that is more pronounced in dyads where youth acquire smartphones earlier.

Smartphone ownership may serve as an indicator of youths’ prior digital media use (i.e., prior to early adolescence), which could contribute to parent-child informant discrepancy. Some supporting evidence suggests digital media use impacts parent-child relationships, which, in turn, influences informant discrepancy (Van Roy et al., Reference Van Roy, Groholt, Heyerdahl and Clench-Aas2010). For example, higher youth digital media use has been linked to poorer parent-child relationship quality (Sampasa-Kanyinga et al., Reference Sampasa-Kanyinga, Goldfield, Kingsbury, Clayborne and Colman2020), lower attachment quality (Richards et al., Reference Richards, McGee, Williams, Welch and Hancox2010), and more reported relationship problems with their parents (Jensen et al., Reference Jensen, George, Russell, Lippold and Odgers2021). However, because this pattern persisted after controlling for overall screen time, digital media use alone is unlikely to explain the full effect. Alternatively, early smartphone ownership may reflect broader family technology habits, wherein parents who are heavy digital media users provide phones to their children at younger ages. Indeed, parental smartphone use has been associated with less responsiveness and sensitivity to their children (Abels et al., Reference Abels, Vanden Abeele, Van Telgen and Van Meijl2018; Kildare & Middlemiss, Reference Kildare and Middlemiss2017). Additionally, family routines may play a role. For example, youth who spend more time away from home may receive smartphones earlier for communication purposes, but reduced parent-child time could limit opportunities for parental monitoring of internalizing problems. In this way, smartphones may be an indicator of broader family dynamics that influence informant discrepancy.

Consistent with our third hypothesis, parent-child informant discrepancy such that parents underestimated youth-reported internalizing problems increased over time in all groups except the No Phone group, even after controlling for total screen time. Also in line with our hypothesis, significant informant discrepancy (i.e., a significant effect of informant on internalizing problems) emerged only after youth acquired a smartphone. Moreover, this discrepancy continued to increase at each subsequent timepoint following smartphone ownership. These findings suggest that group differences in informant discrepancy are linked to smartphone ownership.

Our results indicate that youth smartphone ownership may hinder parents’ ability to detect internalizing problems. Several mechanisms could explain this effect. First, smartphones may facilitate a shift in emotional disclosure from parents to peers. Adolescents primarily use smartphones for social media and texting (Alexander et al., Reference Alexander, Linkersdörfer, Toda-Thorne, Sullivan, Cummins, Tomko, Allen, Bagot, Baker, Fuemmeler, Hoffman, Kiss, Mason, Nguyen-Louie, Tapert, Smith, Squeglia and Wade2024), granting them continuous access to an online social network that other media do not. As youth increasingly seek peer support during adolescence (Brown & Larson, Reference Brown and Larson2009), smartphones enable youth to confide in peers at any time and place, possible reducing their frequency of disclosure to parents. In this way, smartphones may act as a barrier to parental awareness of youth internalizing concerns.

Youth smartphone use may not only shift disclosure patterns but also influence the parent-child relationship more broadly, with implications for closeness and communication. At proximal time scales, smartphone use can disrupt interactions and shared activities through notifications and frequent checking (McDaniel, Reference McDaniel2020). Additionally, parents often cite smartphones as a source of conflict when enforcing technology boundaries (Hattersley et al., Reference Hattersley, Shrewsbury, King, Howlett, Hardy and Baur2009). Over time, these events may accumulate, altering the dynamics of the parent-child dyad. Furthermore, earlier smartphone ownership has been linked to greater aggressiveness and irritability (Thiagarajan & Newson, Reference Thiagarajan and Newson2025), potentially making interactions more challenging and increasing friction in the relationship. As a result, reduced closeness may mediate the relation between early smartphone ownership and informant discrepancy. If so, the effects of early smartphone adoption may extend beyond disclosure patterns to the broader parent-child relationship itself. Future research should examine whether smartphone ownership age contributes to long-term changes in parent-child dynamics.

Consistent with prior research, we found that most youth acquired their first smartphone by age 12, with few remaining without one by age 14. This stresses the normative role of smartphones in adolescent development and the importance of considering their impact during this stage. Additionally, we identified demographic differences in smartphone ownership trends. Girls and youth with less-educated parents were more likely to acquire smartphones earlier. These findings align with prior research showing that girls use smartphones more frequently than boys (Twenge & Martin, Reference Twenge and Martin2020) and that parents with higher education levels tend to impose more digital media restrictions (Livingstone et al., Reference Livingstone, Mascheroni, Dreier, Chaudron and Lagae2015).

Although this study does not establish causality between smartphone ownership and internalizing problems, one notable trend warrants discussion. Across all smartphone-owning groups, youth who acquired smartphones earlier consistently reported higher levels of internalizing problems at each timepoint. This pattern suggests an underlying process linking smartphones and internalizing problems in early adolescence. Several explanations are possible. Smartphone ownership may serve as a marker of an unmeasured factor, such as family or peer influences, that simultaneously promotes early smartphone adoption and increases internalizing problems risk. Alternatively, youth experiencing psychological distress may advocate for smartphone ownership earlier. In support, prior research shows increased screen use among individuals with depression, potentially using screens as a coping mechanism (Elmquist & McLaughlin, Reference Elmquist and McLaughlin2018; Wolfers & Utz, Reference Wolfers and Utz2022). Conversely, smartphone use itself may contribute to internalizing problems (Twenge, Reference Twenge2020). Overall, empirical studies in middle adolescence suggest that problematic (e.g., smartphone dependency) and high levels of smartphone use tends to be associated cross-sectionally and longitudinally with increased internalizing problems (Augner et al., Reference Augner, Vlasak, Aichhorn and Barth2021; Lapierre et al., Reference Lapierre, Zhao and Custer2019; Ng et al., Reference Ng, Wu, Lam, Lam, Nip, Ng, Leung and Leung2020; Wacks & Weinstein, Reference Wacks and Weinstein2021). Yet, null results (Poulain et al., Reference Poulain, Vogel, Kliesener and Kiess2023) and even contrary findings showing mental health benefits from using smartphones (Marciano et al., Reference Marciano, Driver, Schulz and Camerini2022; Minich & Moreno, Reference Minich, Moreno and Limongi2024) are also present. Our results highlight the need for more longitudinal studies on this link, particularly in early adolescence, and provide a more nuanced reason for negative associations in that they may create barriers to identifying symptoms.

Our findings indicate that the No Phone group differed from smartphone users in several key ways. First, prior research consistently shows that parents report lower levels of internalizing problems than youth self-report (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Augenstein, Wang, Thomas, Drabick, Burgers and Rabinowitz2015). This pattern held across all groups except the No Phone group, where parent and child reports were highly aligned. Second, latent growth analyses revealed that while internalizing problems significantly increased in all smartphone groups, they decreased over time in the No Phone group. Third, parent-reported internalizing problems were consistently higher in the No Phone group than in smartphone groups. However, because parents of smartphone users tended to underestimate youth-reported internalizing problems, parent reports in the No Phone group were the most similar to youth self-reports. These findings suggest that the No Phone group may represent a distinct population. Research on this form of smartphone abstention is scarce and given that this group was a small minority in our sample, their divergence from the normative trend of early smartphone adoption warrants further investigation. Future studies should explore the family, peer, and individual factors associated with delaying smartphone ownership.

Limitations

These findings should be considered in light of several limitations. First, while we controlled for overall screen use at each timepoint, we could not account for the frequency of smartphone use throughout the study. Future research should examine how smartphone use, beyond ownership, influences parent-child informant discrepancy. Second, smartphone behaviors vary widely among youth. If primarily used for parental communication, smartphones might enhance rather than hinder parent-child agreement. Future studies should distinguish between different usage patterns to identify risks and benefits. Third, our focus was on informant discrepancy in internalizing problems. It remains unclear whether similar patterns extend to other constructs, such as externalizing behaviors or family dynamics. Fourth, parents retrospectively reported the age of smartphone acquisition, and internalizing problems were assessed at one-year intervals. Future research should measure smartphone adoption more precisely and examine informant discrepancy at shorter timescales before and after acquisition to capture more proximal effects. Moreover, most parents in the current study were mothers. Although mother-father agreement regarding youth internalizing problems tends to be high (Schroeder et al., Reference Schroeder, Hood and Hughes2010), prior study results suggest there are differences between mother- and father-child reporting discrepancy. However, consistent patterns have yet to emerge as some studies have shown better child agreement with fathers (Hughes & Gullone, Reference Hughes and Gullone2010; Treutler & Epkins, Reference Treutler and Epkins2003), and others with mothers (Seiffge-Krenke & Kollmar, Reference Seiffge-Krenke and Kollmar1998). Future studies should test whether findings from the current study differ between father- and mother-reports. Further, although we demonstrated that smartphone ownership is associated with reporting discrepancy, other sources of reporting discrepancy should be explored in future studies (e.g., autonomy development and parental monitoring). Finally, youth-reported internalizing problems were successively higher in groups with earlier smartphone ownership, making it difficult to disentangle informant discrepancy from overall group differences in internalizing problems. However, our multi-level modeling approach accounts for between- and within-person variance and mitigates such confounding issues inherent to difference scores (De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Ohannessian and Laird2016; Laird & De Los Reyes, Reference Laird and De Los Reyes2013). Nonetheless, future research should replicate these findings in clinical samples to further clarify these relations.

Conclusion

Despite these limitations, our study leverages advanced statistical methods and a large, longitudinal, and generalizable dataset to examine parent-child informant discrepancy in youth internalizing problems. First, we found that overall discrepancy was more pronounced among youth who received smartphones earlier, suggesting that studies relying on parent-reported internalizing problems should account for youth smartphone ownership. Second, informant discrepancy increased over time among smartphone owners, highlighting a potential long-term effect of smartphone use on parent-child agreement regarding internalizing problems. Finally, youth without smartphones exhibited distinct developmental patterns in internalizing problems based on both parent- and self-reports, underscoring the need for further research on this subgroup. Our findings provide important insights into the role of smartphone ownership in adolescent psychosocial development and suggest early smartphone ownership may lead to parents’ underestimation of their children’s internalizing problems in early adolescence.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579425100618.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available to qualified researchers with a valid data use contract in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA) at https://nda.nih.gov, NDA Collection #2573, DOI: 10.15154/z563-zd24.

Acknowledgments

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children aged9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from https://doi.org/10.15154/z563-zd24.

Author contributions

Cory Carvalho: Conceptualization (lead); formal analysis (lead); methodology (equal); visualization (lead); and writing – original draft preparation (lead); data curation (lead). Niyantri Ravindran: Conceptualization (supporting); writing – review and editing (equal). Kalsea Koss: Conceptualization (supporting); methodology (supporting); writing – review & editing (equal); writing – original draft preparation (supporting).

Funding statement

KJK was supported by grants R01DA058334 and P50DA051361 in the preparation of this manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Ethical approval

The present study utilized data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, which is a publicly available dataset. The ABCD study was conducted in compliance with the ethical standards set forth by the relevant institutional review boards (IRBs) at each participating site. Informed consent and assent were obtained from all participants and their legal guardians. The current analysis was conducted under data use agreement, and all procedures adhered to the ethical guidelines and principles of the Helsinki Declaration and other relevant ethical standards.

Participant consent statement

Informed consent and assent were obtained from all ABCD Study participants and/or their legal guardians at the time of enrollment. All consenting participants have further given consent for data sharing for secondary data analysis.

References

Abels, M., Vanden Abeele, M., Van Telgen, T., & Van Meijl, H. (2018). Nod, nod, ignore: An exploratory observational study on the relation between parental mobile media use and parental responsiveness towards young children. The Talking Species: Perspectives On the Evolutionary, Neuronal, and Cultural Foundations of Language, 195, 228.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1991). Child behavior bhecklist. Burlington (VT), 7, 371392.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S., Ivanova, M., & Rescorla, L. (2011). Manual for the ASEBA brief problem monitor (BPM) (pp. 33). ASEBA.Google Scholar
Aebi, M., Kuhn, C., Banaschewski, T., Grimmer, Y., Poustka, L., Steinhausen, H.-C., & Goodman, R. (2017). The contribution of parent and youth information to identify mental health disorders or problems in adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 11(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-017-0160-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, J. D., Linkersdörfer, J., Toda-Thorne, K., Sullivan, R. M., Cummins, K. M., Tomko, R. L., Allen, N. B., Bagot, K. S., Baker, F. C., Fuemmeler, B. F., Hoffman, E. A., Kiss, O., Mason, M. J., Nguyen-Louie, T. T., Tapert, S. F., Smith, C. J., Squeglia, L. M., & Wade, N. E. (2024). Passively sensing smartphone use in teens with rates of use by sex and across operating systems. Scientific Reports, 14(1), 17982. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-68467-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Augner, C., Vlasak, T., Aichhorn, W., & Barth, A. (2021). The association between problematic smartphone use and symptoms of anxiety and depression—a meta-analysis. Journal of Public Health, 45(1), 193201. https://doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdab350 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagot, K. S., Tomko, R. L., Marshall, A. T., Hermann, J., Cummins, K., Ksinan, A., Kakalis, M., Breslin, F., Lisdahl, K. M., Mason, M., Redhead, J. N., Squeglia, L. M., Thompson, W. K., Wade, T., Tapert, S. F., Fuemmeler, B. F., & Baker, F. C. (2022). Youth screen use in the ABCD® study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 57, 101150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101150 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, E. T., Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Hendricks, C., & Suwalsky, J. T. D. (2007). Adolescent-mother agreement about adolescent problem behaviors: Direction and predictors of disagreement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(7), 950962. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-006-9164-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bidaut-Russell, M., Reich, W., Cottler, L. B., Robins, L. N., Compton, W. M., & Mattison, R. E. (1995). The diagnostic interview schedule for children (PC-DISC v.3.0): Parents and adolescents suggest reasons for expecting discrepant answers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23(5), 641659. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01447667 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J. (2019). Adolescence and mental health. The Lancet, 393(10185), 20302031. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(19)31013-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, B. B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in adolescence. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, 2(Pt I), 74104.Google Scholar
Castagna, P. J., Calamia, M., & Davis, T. E. (2021). The discrepancy between mother and youth reported internalizing symptoms predicts youth’s negative self-esteem. Current Psychology, 40(11), 53125321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00501-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Y.-Y., Ho, S.-Y., Lee, P.-C., Wu, C.-K., Gau, S. S..-F., & van Luijtelaar, G. (2017). Parent-child discrepancies in the report of adolescent emotional and behavioral problems in Taiwan. PLOS ONE, 12(6), e0178863. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178863 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, T. W., Magis-Weinberg, L., Guazzelli Williamson, V., Ladouceur, C. D., Whittle, S. L., Herting, M. M., Uban, K. A., Byrne, M. L., Barendse, M. E. A., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Pfeifer, J. H. (2021). A researcher’s guide to the measurement and modeling of puberty in the ABCD study® at baseline [Perspective]. Frontiers in Endocrinology, 12, 608575. https://doi.org/10.3389/fendo.2021.608575 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, S. M., Stockdale, L., & Summers, K. (2019). Problematic cell phone use, depression, anxiety, and self-regulation: Evidence from a three year longitudinal study from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Computers in Human Behavior, 96, 7884. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, K., Dinhopl, A., & Hiniker, A. (2019). “Everything’s the phone”: Understanding the phone’s supercharged role in parent-teen relationships. In Proceedings of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’19). Association for Computing Machinery (ACM), New York, NY, USA. https://doi.org/10.1145/3290605.3300457 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Los Reyes, A., Augenstein, T. M., Wang, M., Thomas, S. A., Drabick, D. A., Burgers, D. E., & Rabinowitz, J. (2015). The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 141(4), 858. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038498 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Los Reyes, A., Ohannessian, C. M., & Laird, R. D. (2016). Developmental changes in discrepancies between adolescents’ and their mothers’ views of family communication. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 25(3), 790797. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-015-0275-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dwyer, R. J., Kushlev, K., & Dunn, E. W. (2018). Smartphone use undermines enjoyment of face-to-face social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 78, 233239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.10.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, J. R. (1994). The study of congruence in organizational behavior research: Critique and a proposed alternative. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58(1), 51100. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1994.1029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elmquist, D. L., & McLaughlin, C. L. (2018). Social media use among adolescents coping with mental health. Contemporary School Psychology, 22(4), 503511. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40688-017-0167-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabris, M. A., Marengo, D., Longobardi, C., & Settanni, M. (2020). Investigating the links between fear of missing out, social media addiction, and emotional symptoms in adolescence: The role of stress associated with neglect and negative reactions on social media. Addictive Behaviors, 106, 106364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106364 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farley, J. P., & Kim-Spoon, J. (2014). The development of adolescent self-regulation: Reviewing the role of parent, peer, friend, and romantic relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 37(4), 433440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.03.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goolsby, J., Rich, B. A., Hinnant, B., Habayeb, S., Berghorst, L., De Los Reyes, A., & Alvord, M. K. (2018). Parent–child informant discrepancy is associated with poorer treatment outcome. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27(4), 12281241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0946-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granic, I., Dishion, T. J., Hollenstein, T., & Patterson, G. (2003). The family ecology of adolescence: A dynamic systems perspective on normative development. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 6091). Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Hattersley, L. A., Shrewsbury, V. A., King, L. A., Howlett, S. A., Hardy, L. L., & Baur, L. A. (2009). Adolescent-parent interactions and attitudes around screen time and sugary drink consumption: A qualitative study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 6(1), 61. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-61 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawi, N. S., & Samaha, M. (2017). Relationships among smartphone addiction, anxiety, and family relations. Behaviour & Information Technology, 36(10), 10461052. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929X.2017.1336254 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heeringa, S. G., & Berglund, P. A. (2020). A guide for population-based analysis of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study baseline data [Preprint]. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.02.10.942011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, E. K., & Gullone, E. (2010). Discrepancies between adolescent, mother, and father reports of adolescent internalizing symptom levels and their association with parent symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 66(9), 978995. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20695 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, M., George, M. J., Russell, M. A., Lippold, M. A., & Odgers, C. L. (2021). Does adolescent digital technology use detract from the parent–Adolescent relationship? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 31(2), 469481. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12618 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapetanovic, S., Rothenberg, W. A., Lansford, J. E., Bornstein, M. H., Chang, L., Deater-Deckard, K., Di Giunta, L., Dodge, K. A., Gurdal, S., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., Steinberg, L., Tapanya, S., Uribe Tirado, L. M., Yotanyamaneewong, S., Peña Alampay, L.Bacchini, D. (2020). Cross-cultural examination of links between parent–adolescent communication and adolescent psychological problems in 12 cultural groups. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(6), 12251244. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01212-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karver, M. S. (2006). Determinants of multiple informant agreement on child and adolescent behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(2), 242253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-9015-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kildare, C. A., Middlemiss, W. (2017). Impact of parents mobile device use on parent-child interaction: A literature review. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 579593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, J., Chan, Y.-F., McCauley, E., & Vander Stoep, A. (2016). Parent-child discrepancies in reporting of child depression in ethnic groups. The Journal for Nurse Practitioners, 12(6), 374380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nurpra.2016.01.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koca, F., & Saatçı, F. (2022). The mediator role of fear of missing out in the parent-adolescent relationship quality and problematic internet use. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 20(3), 18971912. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-022-00822-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolko, D. J., & Kazdin, A. E. (1993). Emotional/behavioral problems in clinic and nonclinic children: Correspondence among child, parent and teacher reports. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(6), 9911006. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01103.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushlev, K., Dwyer, R., & Dunn, E. W. (2019). The social price of constant connectivity: Smartphones impose subtle costs on well-being. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 347352. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419847200 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lagattuta, K. H., Sayfan, L., & Bamford, C. (2012). Do you know how I feel? Parents underestimate worry and overestimate optimism compared to child self-report. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(2), 211232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laird, R. D., & De Los Reyes, A. (2013). Testing informant discrepancies as predictors of early adolescent psychopathology: Why difference scores cannot tell you what you want to know and how polynomial regression may. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9659-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, R. D., & LaFleur, L. K. (2016). Disclosure and monitoring as predictors of mother–adolescent agreement in reports of early adolescent rule-breaking behavior. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 45(2), 188200. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2014.963856 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanette, S. (2018). The mere presence of mobile phones during parent-teen interactions [Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Irvine]. eScholarship. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1j709942 Google Scholar
Lapierre, M. A., Zhao, P., & Custer, B. E. (2019). Short-term longitudinal relationships between smartphone use/dependency and psychological well-being among late adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 65(5), 607612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.06.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, M., Kamper-DeMarco, K. E., Zhang, J., Xiao, J., Dong, D., & Xue, P. (2022). Time spent on social media and risk of depression in adolescents: A dose–response meta-analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(9), 5164. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19095164 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, S., Mascheroni, G., Dreier, M., Chaudron, S., & Lagae, K. (2015). How parents of young children manage digital devices at home: The role of income, education and parental style. EU Kids Online, London School of Economics and Political Science. https://eprints.lse.ac.uk/63378/ Google Scholar
Makol, B. A., & Polo, A. J. (2018). Parent-child endorsement discrepancies among youth at chronic-risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(5), 10771088. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0360-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marciano, L., Driver, C. C., Schulz, P. J., & Camerini, A.-L. (2022). Dynamics of adolescents’ smartphone use and well-being are positive but ephemeral. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 1316. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05291-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, B. T. (2014). Technoference”: Everyday intrusions and interruptions of technology in couple and family relationships. In Bruess, C. J. (Ed.), Family communication in the age of digital and social media. Peter Lang Publishing.Google Scholar
McDaniel, B. T. (2020). Technoference: Parent mobile device use and implications for children and parent–child relationships. Zero to Three Journal, 41(2), 3036. https://www.zerotothree.org/resource/journal/technoference-parent-mobile-device-use-and-implications-for-children-and-parent-child-relationships/ Google Scholar
Minich, M., Moreno, M., & Limongi, R. (2024). Real-world adolescent smartphone use is associated with improvements in mood: An ecological momentary assessment study. PLOS ONE, 19(5), e0298422. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0298422 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misra, S., Cheng, L., Genevie, J., & Yuan, M. (2016). The iPhone effect: The quality of in-person social interactions in the presence of mobile devices. Environment and Behavior, 48(2), 275298. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916514539755 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, K. C., Wu, L. H., Lam, H. Y., Lam, L. K., Nip, P. Y., Ng, C. M., Leung, K. C., & Leung, S. F. (2020). The relationships between mobile phone use and depressive symptoms, bodily pain, and daytime sleepiness in Hong Kong secondary school students. Addictive Behaviors, 101, 105975. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.04.033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117133. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poudel, A., & Gautam, S. (2017). Age of onset of substance use and psychosocial problems among individuals with substance use disorders. BMC Psychiatry, 17(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-1191-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulain, T., Vogel, M., Kliesener, T., & Kiess, W. (2023). Associations between changes in behavioral difficulties and levels of problematic smartphone use in adolescents over a 1-year period. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 533536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01874-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2013). Can you connect with me now? How the presence of mobile communication technology influences face-to-face conversation quality. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(3), 237246. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407512453827 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. A. (2016). Cross-cultural perspectives on parent–adolescent discrepancies: Existing findings and future directions. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(10), 21852196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016-0554-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, R., McGee, R., Williams, S. M., Welch, D., & Hancox, R. J. (2010). Adolescent screen time and attachment to parents and peers. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 164(3), 258262. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.280 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, A., Adkins, V., & Selkie, E. (2022). Youth perspectives on the recommended age of mobile phone adoption: Survey study. JMIR Pediatr Parent, 5(4), e40704. https://doi.org/10.2196/40704 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rideout, V., Peebles, A., Mann, S., & Robb, M. B. (2022). The Common sense census: Media use by tweens and teens, 2021. Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-tweens-and-teens-2021 Google Scholar
Rideout, V., & Robb, M. B. (2020). The Common Sense Census: Media use by Kids Age Zero to Eight, 2020. Common Sense Media. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-kids-age-zero-to-eight-2020 Google Scholar
Robinson, M., Doherty, D. A., Cannon, J., Hickey, M., Rosenthal, S. L., Marino, J. L., & Skinner, S. R. (2019). Comparing adolescent and parent reports of externalizing problems: A longitudinal population-based study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 247268. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12270 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romer, D., Reyna, V. F., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sampasa-Kanyinga, H., Goldfield, G. S., Kingsbury, M., Clayborne, Z., & Colman, I. (2020). Social media use and parent–child relationship: A cross-sectional study of adolescents. Journal of Community Psychology, 48(3), 793803. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.22293 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapien Labs. (2023, May 15). Age of First Smartphone/Tablet and Mental Wellbeing Outcomes. https://sapienlabs.org/age-of-first-smartphone-tablet-and-mental-wellbeing-outcomes-report/ Google Scholar
Saragosa-Harris, N. M., Chaku, N., MacSweeney, N., Guazzelli Williamson, V., Scheuplein, M., Feola, B., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Demir-Lira, E., McNeilly, E. A., Huffman, L. G., Whitmore, L., Michalska, K. J., Damme, K. S. F., Rakesh, D., & Mills, K. L. (2022). A practical guide for researchers and reviewers using the ABCD study and other large longitudinal datasets. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 55, 101115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101115 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference Chi-Square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4), 507514. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02296192 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sbarra, D. A., Briskin, J. L., & Slatcher, R. B. (2019). Smartphones and close relationships: The case for an evolutionary mismatch. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(4), 596618. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619826535 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schroeder, J. F., Hood, M. M., & Hughes, H. M. (2010). Inter-parent agreement on the syndrome scales of the child behavior checklist (CBCL): Correspondence and discrepancies. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19(5), 646653. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-010-9352-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Kollmar, F. (1998). Discrepancies between mothers’ and fathers’ perceptions of sons’ and daughters’ problem behaviour: A longitudinal analysis of parent–adolescent agreement on internalising and externalising problem behaviour. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 687697. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021963098002492 Google ScholarPubMed
Spear, H. J., & Kulbok, P. (2004). Autonomy and adolescence: A concept analysis. Public Health Nursing, 21(2), 144152. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0737-1209.2004.021208.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 5559. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockdale, L. A., Coyne, S. M., & Padilla-Walker, L. M. (2018). Parent and child technoference and socioemotional behavioral outcomes: A nationally representative study of 10- to 20-year-old adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 88, 219226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.034 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thapar, A., Collishaw, S., Pine, D. S., & Thapar, A. K. (2012). Depression in adolescence. The Lancet, 379(9820), 10561067. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60871-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thiagarajan, T., & Newson, J. (2025, January 23). The youth mind: Rising aggression and anger [Rapid Report]. Sapien Labs. https://sapienlabs.org/the-youth-mind/.Google Scholar
Treutler, C. M., & Epkins, C. C. (2003). Are discrepancies among child, mother, and father reports on children’s behavior related to parents’ psychological symptoms and aspects of parent–child relationships? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(1), 1327. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021765114434 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M. (2020). Why increases in adolescent depression may be linked to the technological environment. Current Opinion in Psychology, 32, 8994. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.06.036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twenge, J. M., & Martin, G. N. (2020). Gender differences in associations between digital media use and psychological well-being: Evidence from three large datasets. Journal of Adolescence, 79, 91102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.12.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Roy, B., Groholt, B., Heyerdahl, S., & Clench-Aas, J. (2010). Understanding discrepancies in parent-child reporting of emotional and behavioural problems: Effects of relational and socio-demographic factors. BMC Psychiatry, 10(1), 56. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-10-56 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wacks, Y., & Weinstein, A. M. (2021). Excessive smartphone use is associated with health problems in adolescents and young adults [Mini review]. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 669042. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.669042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, N. E., Ortigara, J. M., Sullivan, R. M., Tomko, R. L., Breslin, F. J., Baker, F. C., Fuemmeler, B. F., Delrahim Howlett, K., Lisdahl, K. M., Marshall, A. T., Mason, M. J., Neale, M. C., Squeglia, L. M., Wolff-Hughes, D. L., Tapert, S. F., Bagot, K. S., & ABCD Novel Technologies Workgroup (2021). Passive sensing of preteens’ smartphone use: An adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) cohort substudy. JMIR Ment Health, 8(10), e29426. https://doi.org/10.2196/29426 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, R., & Aloia, L. (2018). Parent–adolescent communication via mobile devices: Influences on relational closeness. Journal of Family Issues, 39(15), 37783803. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513x18793924 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisskirch, R. S. (2009). Parenting by cell phone: Parental monitoring of adolescents and family relations. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(8), 11231139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9374-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfers, L. N., & Utz, S. (2022). Social media use, stress, and coping. Current Opinion in Psychology, 45, 101305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, P., Schlomer, G. L., Lippold, M. A., & Feinberg, M. E. (2021). Longitudinal discrepancy in adolescent aggressive behavior problems: Differences by reporter and contextual factors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50(8), 15641581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-021-01431-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yildirim Demirdöğen, E., Akinci, M. A., Bozkurt, A., Bayraktutan, B., Turan, B., Aydoğdu, S., Ucuz, İ., Abanoz, E., Yitik Tonkaz, G., Çakir, A., & Ferahkaya, H. (2024). Social media addiction, escapism and coping strategies are associated with the problematic internet use of adolescents in Türkiye: A multi-center study [Original research]. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 15, 1355759. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1355759 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, K.-H., & Bentler, P. M. (2000). Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociological Methodology, 30(1), 165200. https://doi.org/10.1111/0081-1750.00078 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Collins, W. A. (2006). Autonomy development during adolescence. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 174204). Blackwell Publishing.10.1002/9780470756607.ch9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means, standard deviations, and correlations of study variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Group means for internalizing problems across time and demographic covariates

Figure 2

Figure 1. Multi-group latent growth curve analysis of parent- and child-reported youth internalizing problems by age of cell phone ownership. Note. ***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Predicted values of informant effect by age of smartphone ownership group. Note. A negative informant effect indicates parent-report underestimated youth-report. Significant informant effects are indicated with circles (p < .05).

Figure 4

Table 3. Parameter estimates for multi-level models examining the impact of time, informant, and time × informant on internalizing problems based on age of smartphone ownership

Figure 5

Table 4. Probe of the effect of the time × informant product on internalizing problems

Supplementary material: File

Carvalho et al. supplementary material

Carvalho et al. supplementary material
Download Carvalho et al. supplementary material(File)
File 138.2 KB