No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions as an important source of symbolic material culture with low preservation probability
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 14 January 2025
Abstract
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions appear to be a human universal and may have been prevalent throughout the hominin lineage. These activities are cognitively complex and can be associated with a distinctive and symbolic material culture. Yet, many of the artifacts used by foraging groups for sports, team games, and athletic competitions often have a low preservation probability.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Bale, J., & Sang, J. (2013). Kenyan running: Movement culture, geography and global change. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deaner, R. O., Balish, S. M., & Lombardo, M. P. (2016). Sex differences in sports interest and motivation: An evolutionary perspective. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 10(2), 73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deaner, R. O., & Smith, B. A. (2013). Sex differences in sports across 50 societies. Cross-Cultural Research, 47(3), 268–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furley, P. (2019). What modern sports competitions can tell us about human nature. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(2), 138–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallup, A. C., & Deaner, R. O. (2021). A Darwinian approach to the study of sports, games, and athletics. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? A meta-ethnographic review. Human Nature, 28, 367–394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindblom, K. G. (1916). The Akamba in British East Africa: An ethnological monograph. Appelberg.Google Scholar
Lombardo, M. P. (2012). On the evolution of sport. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(1), 1–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, J. M., Arth, M. J., & Bush, R. R. (1959). Games in culture. American Anthropologist, 61(4), 597–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, M. S., Mendoza, M., & Sugiyama, L. (2021). War games: Intergroup coalitional play fighting as a means of comparative coalition formidability assessment. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 91–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, M. S., Mendoza, M., White, F., & Sugiyama, L. (2018). Coalitional play fighting and the evolution of coalitional intergroup aggression. Human Nature, 29, 219–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton-Smith, B., & Roberts, J. M. (1971). The cross-cultural and psychological study of games. International Review of Sport Sociology, 6(1), 79–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turnbull, C. M. (1982). The ritualization of potential conflict between the sexes among the Mbuti. In Leacock, E. & Lee, R. (Eds.), Politics and history in band societies (pp. 133–156). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Target article
Reconsidering the link between past material culture and cognition in light of contemporary hunter–gatherer material use
Related commentaries (21)
Advancing paleoanthropology beyond default nulls
All that glitters is not gold: The false-symbol problem in archaeology
Animal artefacts challenge archaeological standards for tracing human symbolic cognition
Archaeology retains a central role for studying the behavioral and cognitive evolution of our species and genus
Are we jingling modern hunter-gatherers and early Homo sapiens?
Behavioural modernity is dead: Long live behavioural modernity
Beyond the binary: Inferential challenges and solutions in cognitive archaeology
Cultural innovation is not only a product of cognition but also of cultural context
Don't ignore cognitive evolution during the three million years that preceded the archaeological record of material culture!
Inferences from absences
Material culture both reflects and causes human cognitive evolution
Negative priors and inferences from absence of evidence in cognitive and linguistic archaeology: Epistemically sound and scientifically strategic
Not just symbolism: Technologies may also have a less than direct connection with cognition
Perishable material choice indicates symbolic and representational capacities
Proposing the DN(C)-model of material evidence for well-calibrated claims about past cultures
Revising the null model in language evolution research
Shared intentionality may have been favored by persistence hunting in Homo erectus
Sports, team games, and physical skill competitions as an important source of symbolic material culture with low preservation probability
The cognitive and evolutionary science of behavioural modernity goes beyond material chronology
The Mbuti people still reproduce a 75,000 years old recursive pattern
What would be pre-modern human cognition?
Author response
Hominin cognition: The null hypothesis