No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
A developmental perspective on the minimalist model: The case of respect for ownership
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 October 2023
Abstract
The developmental evidence for children's respect for ownership reveals that children will enforce the property rights of third parties before they themselves respect other's property. This pattern of development suggests the need for clarification or modification of the minimalist model. Here, I consider three explanations for the gap between knowledge and behavior for respect of ownership.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Bakeman, R., & Brownlee, J. R. (1982). Social rules governing object conflicts in toddlers and preschoolers. In Rubin, K. H. & Ross, H. S. (Eds.), Peer relationships and social skills in childhood (pp. 99–111). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-8180-8_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, P. R., McAuliffe, K., & Warneken, F. (2014). The developmental origins of fairness: The knowledge-behavior gap. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 559–561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, D. W., Fein, G. G., Killen, M., & Tam, H. P. (2001). Peer conflicts of preschool children: Issues, resolution, incidence, and age-related patterns. Early Education and Development, 12(4), 523–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davoodi, T., Nelson, L. J., & Blake, P. R. (2020). Children's conceptions of ownership for self and other: Categorical ownership versus strength of claim. Child Development, 91, 163–178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hay, D. F., Paine, A. L., Perra, O., Cook, K. V., Hashmi, S., Robinson, C., … Slade, R. (2021). Prosocial and aggressive behavior: A longitudinal study. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 86(2), 7–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M., Gummerum, M., Canz, T., Gigerenzer, G., & Takezawa, M. (2013). The is and ought of sharing: The equality heuristic across the lifespan. In Simple heuristics in a social world (pp. 171–195). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Licht, B., Simoni, H., & Perrig-Chiello, P. (2008). Conflict between peers in infancy and toddler age: What do they fight about? Early Years, 28(3), 235–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pesowski, M. L., Kanngiesser, P., & Friedman, O. (2019). Give and take: Ownership affects how 2-and 3-year-olds allocate resources. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 185, 214–223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putallaz, M., & Sheppard, B. H. (1990). Social status and children's orientations to limited resources. Child Development, 61(6), 2022–2027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, F., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children's understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition, 121(2), 219–227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shantz, C. U. (1987). Conflicts between children. Child Development, 58, 283–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. E., Blake, P. R., & Harris, P. L. (2013). I should but I won't: Why young children endorse norms of fair sharing but do not follow them. PLoS ONE, 8(3), e59510.Google ScholarPubMed
Steinbeis, N., & Over, H. (2017). Enhancing behavioral control increases sharing in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 310–318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Ownership psychology as a cognitive adaptation: A minimalist model
Related commentaries (31)
A cooperative–competitive perspective of ownership necessitates an understanding of ownership disagreements
A developmental perspective on the minimalist model: The case of respect for ownership
Autonomy, the moral circle, and the limits of ownership
Beyond personal ownership: Examining the complexities of ownership in culture
Boyer's minimal model should also represent multiple ownership without collective agency
Computational theories should be made with natural language instead of meaningless code
Development, history, and a minimalist model of ownership psychology
Hold it! Where do we put the body?
How the minimalist model of ownership psychology can aid in explaining moral behaviors under resource constraints
Invested effort and our open-ended sense of ownership
No single notion of cooperation explains when we respect ownership
Not by intuitions alone: Institutions shape our ownership behaviour
On intuitive versus institutional accounts of ownership
Ownership and willingness to compete for resources
Ownership as a component of the extended self
Ownership as an extension of self: An alternative to a minimalist model
Ownership is (likely to be) a moral foundation
Ownership language informs ownership psychology
Ownership psychology and group size
Ownership psychology as a “cognitive cell” adaptation: A minimalist model of microbial goods theory
Primordial feeling of possession in development
Psychological ownership: Actors' and observers' perspectives
Reciprocal contracts – not competitive acquisition – explain the moral psychology of ownership
Similarity and the coordination of ownership
The curious origins of ownership
The evolutionary psychology of ownership is rooted in the Lockean liberal principle of self-ownership
The missing link? How do non-human primates fit in the minimalist model of ownership?
The origins of property law
The recursive nature of ownership intuitions
What do infants need an ownership concept for? Frugal possession concepts can adequately support early reasoning about distributive dilemmas
When it comes to taxes, ownership intuitions abide by the law
Author response
Ownership psychology, its antecedents and consequences