Hostname: page-component-669899f699-8p65j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-25T13:39:28.042Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of age on susceptibility to the attraction effect in sequential risky decision-making

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2024

Maciej Koscielniak
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland Interdisciplinary Center for Applied Cognitive Studies, Warsaw, Poland
Klara Rydzewska*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland Interdisciplinary Center for Applied Cognitive Studies, Warsaw, Poland
Agata Gasiorowska
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland Center for Research in Economic Behavior, Wrocław, Poland
Grzegorz Sedek
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland Interdisciplinary Center for Applied Cognitive Studies, Warsaw, Poland
*
Corresponding author: Klara Rydzewska; Email: krydzewska1@swps.edu.pl

Abstract

Our study aims to contribute to the existing body of research on age-related changes in decision-making by investigating susceptibility to the attraction effect across adulthood. Prior studies have produced inconsistent conclusions regarding the decision-making abilities of older individuals, with some portraying them as easily manipulated and risk-averse, while others suggest the opposite. To address this issue, we conducted two experiments using a novel paradigm of the roulette task: (1) in an online environment with 357 participants and (2) in a laboratory setting with 173 participants. The results were consistent and demonstrated the robustness of the attraction effect. However, no age differences in susceptibility to the attraction effect as a common decision bias were found. As predicted, older adults were more likely to commit simple decision-making mistakes, especially in the preliminary trials, which could have serious financial or societal consequences. Additionally, older adults exhibited more risk-seeking behaviours. Furthermore, we observed that the dynamics of decision competence (as indicated by a decrease in the selection of erroneous decoy options and an increase in decision fluency) were similar for both younger and older adults, suggesting preservation of the ability to optimise decision-making while becoming familiar with new tasks. These findings provide insight into the cognitive functioning of older adults and indicate that decision-making abilities in late adulthood may be more complex than commonly assumed.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Ariely, D (2010) Predictably Irrational: The Hidden Forces That Shape Our Decisions. New York: Harper Perennial.Google Scholar
Baltes, PB and Baltes, MM (1990) Successful Aging: Perspectives from the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, AS, Timpe, J, Edmonds, EC, Bechara, A, Tranel, D and Denburg, NL (2013) Myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Age-related changes in decision making on the Iowa gambling task. Emotion 13, 1924. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellucci, D, Fuochi, G and Conzo, P (2020) Childhood exposure to the Second World War and financial risk taking in adult life. Journal of Economic Psychology 79, . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2019.102196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, R and Charness, N (2015) Age differences in the effect of framing on risky choice: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging 30, 688698. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruine de Bruin, W, Parker, AM and Fischhoff, B (2012) Explaining adult age differences in decision-making competence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25, 352360. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruine de Bruin, W, Parker, AM and Fischhoff, B (2020) Decision-making competence: More than intelligence? Current Directions in Psychological Science 29, 186192. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420901592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruine de Bruin, W, Strough, J and Parker, AM (2014) Getting older isn’t all that bad: Better decisions and coping when facing ‘sunk costs’. Psychology and Aging 29, 642647. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carstensen, LL, Isaacowitz, DM and Charles, ST (1999) Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist 54, 165181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cataldo, AM and Cohen, AL (2019) The comparison process as an account of variation in the attraction, compromise, and similarity effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 26, 934942. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1531-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M and Wilson, M (1997) Crime and conflict: Homicide in evolutionary psychological perspective. Crime and Justice 22, 51100. https://doi.org/10.1086/449260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denburg, NL, Weller, JA, Yamada, TH, Shivapour, DM, Kaup, AR, LaLoggia, A, Cole, CA, Tranel, D and Bechara, A (2009) Poor decision making among older adults is related to elevated levels of neuroticism. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 37, 164172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9094-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dhar, R and Simonson, I (2003) The effect of forced choice on choice. Journal of Marketing Research 40, 146160. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.40.2.146.19229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duverne, S and Lemaire, P (2005) Aging and mental arithmetic. In Campbell, JID (ed), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press, 397411.Google Scholar
Eppinger, B, Hämmerer, D and Li, S-C (2011) Neuromodulation of reward-based learning and decision making in human aging. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1235, 117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06230.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartshorne, JK and Germine, LT (2015) When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychological Science 26, 433443. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedgcock, W, Rao, AR and Chen, H (2009) Could Ralph Nader’s entrance and exit have helped Al Gore? The impact of decoy dynamics on consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research 46, 330343. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, TM (2014) Selective engagement of cognitive resources: Motivational influences on older adults’ cognitive functioning. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 9, 388407. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614527465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, TM (2022) Subjective perceptions of cognitive costs: Determinants and impact on motivation and engagement. In Sedek, G, Hess, TM and Touron, DR (eds), Multiple Pathways of Cognitive Aging: Motivational and Contextual Influences. New York: Oxford University Press, 1939.Google Scholar
Hess, TM, Strough, J and Löckenhoff, CE (eds) (2015) Aging and Decision Making: Empirical and Applied Perspectives. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hitchcott, PK, Fastame, MC, Langiu, D and Penna, MP (2017) Cognitive failures in late adulthood: The role of age, social context and depressive symptoms. PLoS (Public Library of Science) ONE 12, . https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0189683.Google ScholarPubMed
Hu, J and Yu, R (2014) The neural correlates of the decoy effect in decisions. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8, . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, J, Payne, JW and Puto, C (1982) Adding asymmetrically dominated alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of Consumer Research 9, 9098. https://doi.org/10.1086/208899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huber, J, Payne, JW and Puto, CP (2014) Let’s be honest about the attraction effect. Journal of Marketing Research 51, 520525. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S and Hasher, L (2005) The attraction effect in decision making: Superior performance by older adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 58, 120133. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kite, ME, Stockdale, GD, Whitley, J and Johnson, BT (2005) Attitudes toward younger and older adults: An updated meta-analytic review. Journal of Social Issues 61, 241266. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00404.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koscielniak, M, Rydzewska, K and Sedek, G (2016) Effects of age and initial risk perception on balloon analog risk task: The mediating role of processing speed and need for cognitive closure. Frontiers in Psychology 7, . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koscielniak, M, Rydzewska, K and Sedek, G (2018) Commentary – The attraction effect in decision making: Superior performance by older adults. Frontiers in Psychology 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurnianingsih, YA, Sim, SKY, Chee, MWL and Mullette-Gillman, OA (2015) Aging and loss decision making: Increased risk aversion and decreased use of maximizing information, with correlated rationality and value maximization. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, . https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J and Soberon-Ferrer, H (1997) Consumer vulnerability to fraud: Influencing factors. Journal of Consumer Affairs 31, 7089. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6606.1997.tb00827.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lichters, M, Sarstedt, M and Vogt, B (2015) On the practical relevance of the attraction effect: A cautionary note and guidelines for context effect experiments. Academy of Marketing Science Review 5, 119. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13162-015-0066-8.Google Scholar
Luce, RD (1977) The choice axiom after twenty years. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 15, 215233. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-2496(77)90032-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mata, R, Josef, AK, Samanez-Larkin, GR and Hertwig, R (2011) Age differences in risky choice: A meta-analysis. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1235, 1829. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06200.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mell, T, Heekeren, HR, Marschner, A, Wartenburger, I, Villringer, A and Reischies, FM (2005) Effect of aging on stimulus-reward association learning. Neuropsychologia 43, 554563. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikels, JA, Löckenhoff, CE, Maglio, SJ, Goldstein, MK, Garber, A and Carstensen, LL (2010) Following your heart or your head: Focusing on emotions versus information differentially influences the decisions of younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 16, 8795. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018500.Google ScholarPubMed
Moran, S and Meyer, J (2006) Using context effects to increase a leader’s advantage: What set of alternatives should be included in the comparison set? International Journal of Research in Marketing 23, 141154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijresmar.2005.09.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, EL and Hess, TM (2020) Differential focus on probability and losses between young and older adults in risky decision-making. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition 27, 532552. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2019.1642442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ory, MG, Hoffman, M, Hawkins, M, Sanner, B and Mockenhaupt, R (2003) Challenging aging stereotypes: Designing and evaluating physical activity programs. American Journal of Preventive Medicine 25, . https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-3797(03)00181-8.Google Scholar
Pachur, T, Mata, R and Hertwig, R (2017) Who dares, who errs? Disentangling cognitive and motivational roots of age differences in decisions under risk. Psychological Science 28, 504518. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616687729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, JW (2007) PsychoPy: Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 162, 813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, E, Finucane, ML, MacGregor, DG and Slovic, P (2000) The bearable lightness of aging: Judgment and decision processes in older adults. In Stern, PC and Carstensen, LL (eds), The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 144165.Google Scholar
Rabi, R and Minda, JP (2017) Familiarization may minimize age-related declines in rule-based category learning. Psychology and Aging 32, 654674. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roe, RM, Busemeyer, JR and Townsend, JT (2001) Multialternative decision field theory: A dynamic connectionst model of decision making. Psychological Review 108, 370392. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rydzewska, K, Koscielniak, M and Sedek, G (2022) Age differences in complex decision making and judgment: The role of motivational factors and individual differences. In Sedek, G, Hess, TM and Touron, DR (eds), Multiple Pathways of Cognitive Aging: Motivational and Contextual Influences. New York: Oxford University Press, 203233.Google Scholar
Salthouse, TA (1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review 103, 403428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, TA (2012) Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annual Review of Psychology 63, 201226. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samanez-Larkin, GR, Kuhnen, CM, Yoo, DJ and Knutson, B (2010) Variability in nucleus accumbens activity mediates age-related suboptimal financial risk taking. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 14261434. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4902-09.2010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H (1957) Models of Man: Social and Rational. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Simonson, I (1989) Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise effects. Journal of Consumer Research 16, 158174. https://doi.org/10.1086/209205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sivakumar, K and Cherian, J (1995) Role of product entry and exit on the attraction effect. Marketing Letters 6, 4551. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00994039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strough, J, Bruine de Bruin, W and Peters, E (2015) New perspectives for motivating better decisions in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology 6, . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strough, J, Bruine de Bruin, W and Wilson, J (2020) Age differences in decision making. In Thomas, K and Gutchess, A (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging: A Life Course Perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press, 302325.Google Scholar
Tentori, K, Osherson, D, Hasher, L and May, C (2001) Wisdom and aging: Irrational preferences in college students but not older adults. Cognition 81, B87B96. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00137-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tombaugh, TN and McIntyre, NJ (1992) The mini-mental state examination: A comprehensive review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 40, 922935. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01992.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trueblood, JS, Brown, SD and Heathcote, A (2014) The multiattribute linear ballistic accumulator model of context effects in multialternative choice. Psychological Review 121, 179205. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trueblood, JS, Brown, SD, Heathcote, A and Busemeyer, JR (2013) Not just for consumers: Context effects are fundamental to decision making. Psychological Science 24, 901908. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612464241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsetsos, K, Usher, M and Chater, N (2010) Preference reversal in multiattribute choice. Psychological Review 117, 12751291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Weert, JCM, Alblas, MC, van Dijk, L and Jansen, J (2021) Preference for and understanding of graphs presenting health risk information: The role of age, health literacy, numeracy and graph literacy. Patient Education and Counseling 104, 109117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2020.06.031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed