Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-784d4fb959-pn44h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-16T09:06:45.003Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional Self-Knowledge

How Affective Skills Reveal Our Values, Goals, Cares, and Concerns

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2025

Matt Stichter
Affiliation:
Washington State University
Ellen Fridland
Affiliation:
King's College London

Summary

Knowledge of our emotional and bodily states helps us to further know our goals, values, interests, cares, and concerns. The authors first lay out a puzzle as to why bodily and emotional self-knowledge is strongly associated with good mental health and well-being. They solve this puzzle by mapping out connections between bodily states, emotional states, and our goals with an account of emotions as embodied appraisals. Emotions being embodied implies that self-knowledge of our bodily states aids in acquiring knowledge of our emotional states. Emotions as appraisals means that situations are appraised relative to our goals, such that self-knowledge of emotional states aids in acquiring knowledge of our goals, which are not always transparent to us. While emotional self-knowledge can be difficult to acquire, through skilled practice we can improve awareness and knowledge of our emotional and bodily states.
Get access

Information

Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781009307857
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 03 July 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Element purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Ainley, V., Maister, L., Brokfeld, J., Farmer, H., & Tsakiris, M. (2013). More of myself: Manipulating interoceptive awareness by heightened attention to bodily and narrative aspects of the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 12311238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ainley, V., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Fotopoulou, A., & Tsakiris, M. (2012). Looking into myself: Changes in interoceptive sensitivity during mirror self-observation. Psychophysiology, 49(11), 16721676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aristotle. (1941). Nicomachean ethics (W. D. Ross, trans.). Random House.Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. M. (1993). A materialist theory of the mind. Routledge.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 2858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Pan Macmillan.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion, 15(6), 713724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Aronson, K. R. (2004). Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5), 684697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., & Naqvi, N. (2004). Listening to your heart: Interoceptive awareness as a gateway to feeling. Nature Neuroscience, 7(2), 102–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 159162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betka, S., Pfeifer, G., Garfinkel, S. et al. (2018). How do self-assessment of alexithymia and sensitivity to bodily sensations relate to alcohol consumption?. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 42(1), 8188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R. et al. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133(5), 15151525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonar, A. S., MacCormack, J. K., Feldman, M. J., & Lindquist, K. A. (2023). Examining the role of emotion differentiation on emotion and cardiovascular physiological activity during acute stress. Affective Science, 4(2), 317331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordini, D. (2017). Not in the mood for intentionalism. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 41(1), 6081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornemann, B., & Singer, T. (2017). Taking time to feel our body: Steady increases in heartbeat perception accuracy and decreases in alexithymia over 9 months of contemplative mental training. Psychophysiology, 54(3), 469482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brady, M. S. (2009). The irrationality of recalcitrant emotions. Philosophical Studies, 145, 413430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, M. (2013). Emotional insight. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Britton, W. B. (2019). Can mindfulness be too much of a good thing? The value of a middle way. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 159165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, B. A., Goodman, F. R., Disabato, D. J. et al. (2021). Does negative emotion differentiation influence how people choose to regulate their distress after stressful events? A four-year daily diary study. Emotion (Washington, DC), 21(5), 10001012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, C. D., Payne, B. K., & Doris, J. M. (2013). Morality in high definition: Emotion differentiation calibrates the influence of incidental disgust on moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 719725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(4), 644655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97(1), 1935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassam, Q. (2014). Self-Knowledge for Humans. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., Cheng, C. M., Dalton, A. N., & Tesser, A. (2010). Nonconscious goal pursuit: Isolated incidents or adaptive self-regulatory tool. Social Cognition, 28(5), 569588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chawla, N. & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63(9), 871890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cholbi, M. (2019). Regret, resilience, and the nature of grief. Journal of Moral Philosophy, 16(4), 486508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Gaigg, S. B., & Calvo‐Merino, B. (2018). I can feel my heartbeat: Dancers have increased interoceptive accuracy. Psychophysiology, 55(4), e13008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clore, G. L., Gasper, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect as information. In Forgas, J. P., ed., Handbook of affect and social cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 121144.Google Scholar
Colombetti, G. (2020). Embodied self-referentiality. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 27(1), 5152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombetti, G., & Thompson, E. (2007). The feeling body: Toward an enactive approach to emotion. In Overton, W. F., Müller, U., & Newman, J. L., eds., Developmental Perspectives on Embodiment and Consciousness. Psychology Press, pp. 6184.Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(4), 500505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, H. D., & Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, H. D., & Nagai, Y. (2012). How emotions are shaped by bodily states. Emotion Review, 4(2), 163168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Arms, J., & Jacobson, D. (2003). VIII. The significance of recalcitrant emotion (or, anti-quasijudgmentalism). Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement, 52, 127145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Arms, J., & Jacobson, D. (2010). Demystifying sensibilities. In Goldie, P., ed., Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 585614.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Demiralp, E., Thompson, R. J., Mata, J. et al. (2012). Feeling blue or turquoise? Emotional differentiation in major depressive disorder. Psychological Science, 23(11), 14101416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1993). Consciousness explained. Penguin.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (2002). How could I be wrong? How wrong could I be?. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(5–6), 1316.Google Scholar
Deonna, J. A. (2006). Emotion, perception and perspective. Dialectica, 60(1), 2946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deonna, J., & Teroni, F. (2012). The emotions: A philosophical introduction. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deonna, J., & Teroni, F. (2014). In What Sense Are Emotions Evaluations? In Roeser, S., & Todd, C., eds., Emotion and value. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 15–31.Google Scholar
Deonna, J. A., & Teroni, F. (2022). Why are emotions epistemically indispensable?. Inquiry, 68(1), 123.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1999, first published 1641). Discourse on method and meditations on first philosophy. Hackett.Google Scholar
DeSousa, R. (1987). The rationality of emotion. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., & Tiberius, V. (2023). Value fulfillment from a cybernetic perspective: A new psychological theory of well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 27(1), 327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimeff, L. & Linehan, M. M. (2001). Dialectical behavior therapy in a nutshell. The California Psychologist, 34(3), 1013.Google Scholar
Domschke, K., Stevens, S., Pfleiderer, B., & Gerlach, A. L. (2010). Interoceptive sensitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: An overview and integration of neurobiological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DSMTF & American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (Vol. 5, No. 5). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R. et al. (2010). Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science, 21(12), 18351844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Breuer, P., Dohn, D., & Fiegenbaum, W. (1995). Heartbeat perception and panic disorder: Possible explanations for discrepant findings. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 6976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eickers, G. (forthcoming). Ergo.Google Scholar
Eickers, G., & Prinz, J. (2020). Emotion recognition as a social skill. In Fridland, E. & Pavese, C., eds., The Routledge handbook of philosophy of skill and expertise. Routledge, pp. 347361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. E., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elmas, H. G., Cesur, G., & Oral, E. T. (2017). Aleksitimi ve Patolojik Kumar: Duygu Düzenleme Güçlüğünün Aracı Rolü [Alexithymia and Pathological Gambling: The Mediating Role of Difficulties in Emotion Regulation]. Turk psikiyatri dergisi = Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 28(1), 1724.Google Scholar
Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., Kalokerinos, E. K. et al. (2018). Why I don’t always know what I’m feeling: The role of stress in within-person fluctuations in emotion differentiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(2), 179191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erbas, Y., Kalokerinos, E. K., Kuppens, P., van Halem, S., & Ceulemans, E. (2021). Momentary emotion differentiation: The derivation and validation of an index to study within-person fluctuations in emotion differentiation. Assessment, 29(4), 700716. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191121990089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, A. K. (2008). Deliberate practice and acquisition of expert performance: A general overview. Academic Emergency Medicine, 15(11), 988994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feltovich, P., Prietula, M., & Ericsson, K. A. (2006). Studies of expertise from psychological perspectives. In Ericsson, K. A., ed., The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 4168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, D., Messner, M., & Pollatos, O. (2017). Improvement of interoceptive processes after an 8-week body scan intervention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, J. (2019). Sensorimotor psychotherapy in the treatment of trauma. Practice Innovations, 4(3), 156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frewen, P. A., Dozois, D. J., Neufeld, R. W., & Lanius, R. A. (2008). Meta-analysis of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21(2), 243246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridland, E. (2014). They’ve lost control: Reflections on skill. Synthese, 191(12), 27292750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, E. (2017). Automatically minded. Synthese, 194, 43374363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, E. (2019). Longer, smaller, faster, stronger: On skills and intelligence. Philosophical Psychology, 32(5), 759783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, E. (2020). The nature of skill: functions and control structures. In Fridland, E. & Pavese, C., eds., The Routledge handbook of philosophy of skill and expertise. Routledge, pp. 245257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, E. (2021). Skill and strategic control. Synthese, 199(3–4), 59375964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, E. & Stichter, M. (2021). It Just Feels Right: an account of expert intuition. Synthese, 199, 13271346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, M. (1986). Autonomy and the split-level self. The Southern Journal of Philosophy, 24(1), 1935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Furman, D. J., Waugh, C. E., Bhattacharjee, K., Thompson, R. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Interoceptive awareness, positive affect, and decision making in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151(2), 780785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., & Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 911917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P. (2010). Compassion focused therapy: Distinctive features. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldie, P. (2002). Emotions, feelings and intentionality. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1(3), 235254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldie, P. (2004). Emotion, feeling, and knowledge of the world. In Solomon, R. C., ed., Thinking about feeling: Contemporary philosophers on emotions. Oxford University Press, pp. 91106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, P. E., & Scarantino, A. (2005). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion. In Robbins, P., ed., The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge University Press, pp. 437453.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion Regulation: Conceptual Foundations. In Gross, J. J., ed., Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 324.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2009). Acceptance and commitment therapy. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6), 11521168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, B. M., Herbert, C., & Pollatos, O. (2011). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Is interoceptive awareness related to emotional awareness? Journal of Personality, 79(5), 11491175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, B. M., & Pollatos, O. (2012). The body in the mind: on the relationship between interoception and embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 692704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, B. M., Ulbrich, P., & Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and physical effort: Implications for the self-control of physical load in everyday life. Psychophysiology, 44, 194202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery: From domestic abuse to political terror. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Herwig, U., Kaffenberger, T., Jäncke, L., & Brühl, A. B. (2010). Self-related awareness and emotion regulation. NeuroImage, 50(2), 734741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoemann, K., Feldman Barrett, L., & Quigley, K. S. (2021). Emotional granularity increases with intensive ambulatory assessment: Methodological and individual factors influence how much. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.704125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogeveen, J., & Grafman, J. (2021). Alexithymia. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 183, 4762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jäger, C. (2009). Affective ignorance. Erkenntnis, 71(1), 123139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaggar, A. M. (1989) Love and knowledge: Emotion in feminist epistemology, Inquiry, 32(2), 151176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamieson, J. P., Hangen, E. J., Lee, H. Y., & Yeager, D. S. (2018). Capitalizing on appraisal processes to improve affective responses to social stress. Emotion Review, 10(1), 3039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, M. (2001). The authority of affect. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 63(1), 181214.Google Scholar
Jones, K. (2003). Emotion, weakness of will, and the normative conception of agency. In Hatzimoysis, A., ed., Philosophy and the Emotions. Cambridge University Press. pp. 181200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jongsma, A. E. Jr, Peterson, L. M., & Bruce, T. (2021). The complete adult psychotherapy treatment planner. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F., & McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation: Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 1016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., & Farmer, A. S. (2014). Differentiating emotions across contexts: Comparing adults with and without social anxiety disorder using random, social interaction, and daily experience sampling. Emotion, 14(3), 629638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, T. B., Ferssizidis, P., Collins, R. L., & Muraven, M. (2010). Emotion differentiation as resilience against excessive alcohol use: An ecological momentary assessment in underage social drinkers. Psychological Science, 21(9), 13411347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G. et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: A roadmap. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(6), 501–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Khalsa, S. S., Craske, M. G., Li, W. et al. (2015). Altered interoceptive awareness in anorexia nervosa: Effects of meal anticipation, consumption and bodily arousal. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 48(7), 889897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinnaird, E., Stewart, C., & Tchanturia, K. (2019). Investigating alexithymia in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 55, 8089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, A., & Pollatos, O. (2014). Cardiac sensitivity in children: Sex differences and its relationship to parameters of emotional processing. Psychophysiology, 51(9), 932941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolnai, A. (1978). Deliberation is of ends. In Williams, B. and Wiggins, D. eds., Ethics, Value and Reality, Selected Papers of Aurel Kolnai, Indianapolis: Hackett, pp. 4462.Google Scholar
Krauzlis, R. J., Bollimunta, A., Arcizet, F., & Wang, L. (2014). Attention as an effect not a cause. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9), 457464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauzlis, R. J., Wang, L., Yu, G., & Katz, L. N. (2023). What is attention?. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science, 14(1), e1570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latham, G. P., Brcic, J., & Steinhauer, A. (2017). Toward an integration of goal setting theory and the automaticity model. Applied Psychology, 66(1), 2548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeb, C. (2018a). Rebelling against suffering in capitalism. Contemporary Political Theory, 17(3), 263282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeb, C. (2018b). Rethinking embodied reflective judgment with Adorno and Arendt. Constellations, 25(3), 446458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leweke, F., Leichsenring, F., Kruse, J., & Hermes, S. (2012) Is alexithymia associated with specific mental disorders? Psychopathology, 45, 2228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J. et al. (2007). Putting feelings into words: Affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 18(5), 421428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindahl, J. R., Fisher, N. E., Cooper, D. J., Rosen, R. K., & Britton, W. B. (2017). The varieties of contemplative experience: A mixed-methods study of meditation-related challenges in Western Buddhists. PloS One, 12(5), e0176239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K., & Barrett, L. F. (2008). Emotional complexity. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Barrett, L. F., eds., The handbook of emotion, 3rd ed. New York: Guildford Press, pp. 513530.Google Scholar
Maiese, M. (2014). How can emotions be both cognitive and bodily? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 13, 513531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maister, L. & Tsakiris, M. (2013). My face, my heart: Cultural differences in integrated bodily self-awareness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 1016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majeed, R. (2022). What not to make of recalcitrant emotions. Erkenntnis, 87, 747765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mallon, R. & Stich, S. P. (2000). The odd couple: The compatibility of social construction and evolutionary psychology. Philosophy of Science, 67(1), 133154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendelovici, A. (2013). Intentionalism about moods. Thought, 2, 126136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesquita, B. (2022). Between us: How cultures create emotions. WW Norton.Google Scholar
Miller, J. B. (2012). Toward a new psychology of women. Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (1995). Pushmi-pullyu representations. Philosophical Perspectives, 9, 185200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moss, J. (2011). “Virtue makes the goal right”: Virtue and phronesis in Aristotle’s ethics. Phronesis, 56(3), 204261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, J. M. (2017). How (not) to think of emotions as evaluative attitudes. Dialectica, 71(2), 281308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84(3), 231259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nook, E. C., Sasse, S. F., Lambert, H. K., McLaughlin, K. A., & Somerville, L. H. (2018). The nonlinear development of emotion differentiation: Granular emotional experience is low in adolescence. Psychological Science, 29(8), 13461357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nussbaum, M. C. (2001). Upheavals of thought: The intelligence of emotions. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 46(3), 461488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogden, P., Pain, C., & Fisher, J. (2006). A sensorimotor approach to the treatment of trauma and dissociation. Psychiatric Clinics, 29(1), 263279.Google Scholar
Onur, E., Alkın, T., Sheridan, M. J., & Wise, T. N. (2013). Alexithymia and emotional intelligence in patients with panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder. The Psychiatric Quarterly, 84(3), 303311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1990). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2019). Heart to heart. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phan, K. L., & Sripada, C. S. (2013). Emotion regulation. In Armony, J. & Vuilleumier, P., eds., The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience. Cambridge University Press, pp. 375400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollatos, O., Kurz, A. L., Albrecht, J. et al. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 9(4), 381388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollatos, O., Traut‐Mattausch, E., & Schandry, R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depression and Anxiety, 26(2), 167173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pond, R. S. Jr, Kashdan, T. B., DeWall, C. N. et al. (2012). Emotion differentiation moderates aggressive tendencies in angry people: A daily diary analysis. Emotion, 12(2), 326337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, C. J., & Hooven, C. (2018). Interoceptive awareness skills for emotion regulation: Theory and approach of mindful awareness in body-oriented therapy (MABT). Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, J. J. (2004). Gut reactions: A perceptual theory of emotion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2017). Emotion in action: A predictive processing perspective and theoretical synthesis. Emotion Review, 9(4), 319325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R. C. (1995). Feeling one’s emotions and knowing oneself. Philosophical Studies: An International Journal for Philosophy in the Analytic Tradition, 77(2/3), 319338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R. C. (2003). Emotions: An essay in aid of moral psychology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R. C. (2013). Emotions in the moral life. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1995). A way of being. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Rolla, G. (2018). Radical enactivism and self-knowledge. Kriterion, 141, 732743.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1), 145172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saul, J. (2013). Scepticism and implicit bias. Disputatio, 5(37), 243263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: Event appraisal and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 719745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schirmer-Mokwa, K. L., Fard, P. R., Zamorano, A. M. et al. (2015). Evidence for enhanced interoceptive accuracy in professional musicians. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. Journal of Personality, 67(1), 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultheiss, O. C., & Strasser, A. (2012). Referential processing and competence as determinants of congruence between implicit and explicit motives. In Vazire, S. & Wilson, T. D., eds., Handbook of self-knowledge. The Guilford Press, pp. 3962.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (2011). Feelings-as-information theory. Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, 1, 289308.Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2008). The unreliability of naive introspection. Philosophical Review, 117(2), 245273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2012). Self-ignorance. In Liu, J. & Perry, J., eds., Consciousness and the self: New essays. Cambridge University Press, pp. 184197.Google Scholar
Selby, E., Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., et al. (2014). Nothing Tastes as Good as Thin Feels: Low Positive Emotion Differentiation and Weight-Loss Activities in Anorexia Nervosa. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(4), 514531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shargel, D. & Prinz, J. J. (2018). An enactivist theory of emotional content. In Naar, H. & Teroni, F., eds., The ontology of emotions. Cambridge University Press, pp. 110129.Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22(11), 13911396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., Radu, P., Blechert, J., & Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation choice: A conceptual framework and supporting evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 163181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shipko, S., Alvarez, W. A., & Noviello, N. (1983). Towards a teleological model of alexithymia: Alexithymia and post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 39(2), 122126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva, L. (2021). The epistemic role of outlaw emotions. Ergo an Open Access Journal of Philosophy, 8, 23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, G. E., Shortreed, S. M., Rossom, R. C. et al. (2022). Effect of offering care management or online dialectical behavior therapy skills training vs usual care on self-harm among adult outpatients with suicidal ideation: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA, 327(7), 630638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slaby, J. (2008). Affective intentionality and the feeling body. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 429444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, R. C. (1980). Emotions and choice. In Rorty, A. O., ed., Explaining emotions. University of California Press, pp. 251281.Google Scholar
Sosa, E. (2007). A virtue epistemology: Apt belief and reflective knowledge, Volume 1. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosniak, L. A. (1987). The nature of change in successful learning. Teachers College Record, 88(4), 519535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sreenivasan, G. (2018). Emotions, reasons, and epistemology. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 97(2), 500506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starkey, C. (2008). Emotions and full understanding. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 11(4), 425454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stasiewicz, P. R., Bradizza, C. M., Gudleski, G. D., et al. (2012) The relationship of alexithymia to emotional dysregulation within an alcohol dependent treatment sample. Addictive Behaviors, 37, 469476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephan, A. (2017). Moods in Layers. Philosophia, 45, 14811495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stichter, M. (2018). The skillfulness of virtue: Improving our moral and epistemic lives. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stichter, M. (2020). Learning from failure: Shame and emotion regulation in virtue as skill. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 23, 341354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stichter, M. (2024). Flourishing goals, metacognitive skills, and the virtue of wisdom. Topoi.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suvak, M. K., Litz, B. T., Sloan, D. M. et al. (2011). Emotional granularity and borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(2), 414426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tappolet, C. (2000). Émotions et valeurs. Presses Universitaires France.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tappolet, C. (2016). Emotions, values, and agency. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, J. G., & Fragopanagos, N. F. (2005). The interaction of attention and emotion. Neural Networks: The Official Journal of the International Neural Network Society, 18(4), 353369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terasawa, Y., Moriguchi, Y., Tochizawa, S., & Umeda, S. (2014). Interoceptive sensitivity predicts sensitivity to the emotions of others. Cognition and Emotion, 28(8), 14351448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terhaar, J., Viola, F. C., Bär, K. J., & Debener, S. (2012). Heartbeat evoked potentials mirror altered body perception in depressed patients. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(10), 19501957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teroni, F. (2007). Emotions and formal objects. Dialectica, 61(3), 395415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. J., Springstein, T., & Boden, M. (2021). Gaining clarity about emotion differentiation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 15(3), Article e12584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiberius, V., & DeYoung, C. (2022). Pain, depression, and goal-fulfillment theories of ill-being. Midwest Studies in Philosophy, 46, 165191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomko, R. L., Lane, S. P., Pronove, L. M. et al. (2015). Undifferentiated negative affect and impulsivity in borderline personality and depressive disorders: A momentary perspective. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 740753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Gucht, K., Dejonckheere, E., Erbas, Y. et al. (2019). An experience sampling study examining the potential impact of a mindfulness-based intervention on emotion differentiation. Emotion, 19(1), 123131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Kolk, B. A. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1(5), 253265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vedernikova, E., Kuppens, P., & Erbas, Y. (2021). From knowledge to differentiation: Increasing emotion knowledge through an intervention increases negative emotion differentiation. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 703757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widdershoven, R. L., Wichers, M., Kuppens, P. et al. (2019). Effect of self-monitoring through experience sampling on emotion differentiation in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 244, 7177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, T. (2000). Knowledge and its limits. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2011). Attention as Selection for Action. In Mole, C., Smithies, D. & Wu, W., eds., Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 97116.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2024). We know what attention is!. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 28(4), 304318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaki, J., Davis, J. I., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). Overlapping activity in anterior insula during interoception and emotional experience. Neuroimage, 62(1), 493499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeitlin, S. B., & McNally, R. J. (1993). Alexithymia and anxiety sensitivity in panic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 658660.Google ScholarPubMed

Accessibility standard: WCAG 2.1 AA

The PDF of this Element complies with version 2.1 of the Web Content Accessibility Guidelines (WCAG), covering newer accessibility requirements and improved user experiences and achieves the intermediate (AA) level of WCAG compliance, covering a wider range of accessibility requirements.

Content Navigation

Table of contents navigation
Allows you to navigate directly to chapters, sections, or non‐text items through a linked table of contents, reducing the need for extensive scrolling.

Reading Order & Textual Equivalents

Single logical reading order
You will encounter all content (including footnotes, captions, etc.) in a clear, sequential flow, making it easier to follow with assistive tools like screen readers.

Visual Accessibility

Use of colour is not sole means of conveying information
You will still understand key ideas or prompts without relying solely on colour, which is especially helpful if you have colour vision deficiencies.

Structural and Technical Features

ARIA roles provided
You gain clarity from ARIA (Accessible Rich Internet Applications) roles and attributes, as they help assistive technologies interpret how each part of the content functions.

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Emotional Self-Knowledge
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Emotional Self-Knowledge
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Emotional Self-Knowledge
Available formats
×