Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-fg9bn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-28T19:20:45.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - From Pattern to Constructions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 August 2025

Susan Hunston
Affiliation:
University of Birmingham

Summary

The chapter describes the process by which 50 verb complementation patterns have been reinterpreted as verb argument constructions, leading to the identification of 800 constructions. The chapter gives examples from seven patterns: V into n; V n, V n adj; V n to n; V after n; V n that and V n with n. The constructions derived from each pattern are arranged in networks inspired by those used in Systemic Functional Grammar. The networks show the similarities and differences between constructions. They can also be used to show constructions at broader and narrower levels of generality.

Information

Type
Chapter
Information
Pattern, Construction, System
A Unified Approach to Grammar and Lexis
, pp. 25 - 56
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NC
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

3 From Pattern to Constructions

Chapter 2 argued that grammar (or valency) patterns, as identified through a lexicography project, could be a useful resource for identifying verb argument constructions, noting that other researchers such as Ellis et al. (Reference Ellis, Römer and O’Donnell2016), Herbst and Uhrig (Reference Herbst and Uhrig2019), and Perek and Pattern (Reference Perek and Patten2019) have reached the same conclusion. This chapter describes the process used to derive candidate constructions from the Pattern Grammar resource and proposes that taxonomy networks can be used to depict the relationship between those constructions. It further argues that the ‘size’ of a construction is not fixed, but that more general and more specific constructions, as indicated by the taxonomy networks, can be proposed.

3.1 Background

A precursor to the argument in this book can be found in Hunston and Su (Reference Hunston and Su2019), though that paper dealt with the patterning of adjectives while this book takes verbs as its starting point. Working specifically with the semantic area of evaluation (Martin and White Reference Martin and White2005; Hunston Reference Hunston2011), Hunston and Su hypothesise an alignment between pattern and construction, arguing that ‘each of the meaning-pattern combinations identified in Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996, Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1998) can be regarded as a construction’ (Hunston and Su Reference Hunston and Su2019: 567). They use the adjective meaning groups in Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1998) as a basis for identifying constructions, though in practice they depart from the groups themselves quite substantially.

For example, there are a large number of adjectives that are complemented by a that-clause (the pattern ADJ that) and that indicate an emotive reaction to a target. In Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1998) these are divided into several meaning groups (‘surprised’, ‘angry’, ‘horrified’, ‘glad’, and ‘anxious’), whereas Hunston and Su (Reference Hunston and Su2019: 575) suggest there is one construction: ‘person is reaction that-situation’. In contrast, the pattern ADJ for n (grateful for, fearful for, good for, convenient for, etc.) is interpreted as six constructions, based on the function of the preposition; the adjectives recorded for this pattern are listed under 13 meaning groups that do not align with the proposed constructions. In some cases, the mapping of construction to meaning groups is straightforward: the ‘proxy for’ construction, for instance, unites adjectives meaning ‘afraid’ (afraid, concerned, fearful, and worried) and those meaning either ‘happy’ or ‘sad’ (e.g. delighted, glad, and happy; sad and sorry), which Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1998) split into two groups. In other cases, the picture is more complex: the ‘specifying for’ construction (e.g. suitable for children, ready for action, necessary for success, and convenient for the airport) brings together adjectives from four different meaning groups. This is because the proposed constructions focus on the function of the preposition whereas the original meaning groups prioritised the meaning of the adjectives.

Hunston and Su (Reference Hunston and Su2019) proceed to group the constructions under seven ‘analyses’, each labelled with functional roles, such as ‘Target’ and ‘Evaluator’. For example, the second analysis comprises constructions with the elements Evaluator-Hinge-Evaluation-Target. Eight constructions are listed, each based on a different pattern: Person is happy about a situation; Person feels guilty at a situation; Person is interested in something; Person is dismissive of something; Person is keen on something; Person is happy with a situation; Person is happy to do something; Person is annoyed that a situation existed. As in this book, the constructions are named after one of the adjectives used with it. Hunston and Su (Reference Hunston and Su2019: 581) go on to argue that a taxonomy of evaluation constructions could be proposed, based on the notion of delicacy (Halliday Reference Halliday1985; Wible and Tsao Reference Wible and Tsao2020). The eight constructions above, for example, are all more specific versions of either a ‘Person + BE + Affect + Preposition + Entity’ construction or a ‘Person + BE + Affect + Clause’ construction.

The research reported in this chapter applies the same principles to verb complementation patterns, but here there is no focus on a single area of meaning, so there is a much wider area of expression to consider. A total of 54 verb patterns have been reinterpreted. These exclude the intransitive pattern V, patterns with it and there, and a few patterns that are used with very few verbs, such as V n towards n. Table 3.1 lists the patterns and shows the number of constructions identified for each. Three columns of patterns are shown in Table 3.1. The first column lists patterns composed of phrases and clauses but not prepositional phrases. The second column lists patterns consisting of the verb and a prepositional phrase. The third column lists patterns with a verb, a noun phrase, and a prepositional phrase. The full set of constructions can be found on the Transitivity-Net website: transitivity-net.bham.ac.uk. The process of identifying constructions, and the issues that arise in that process, are the subject of this chapter. The chapter also develops the notion of taxonomy and system. The question of element annotation is reserved for Chapter 4.

Table 3.1List of patterns and the number of constructions identified in each
A table listing patterns and showing the number of constructions in each. See long description.
Table 3.1:Long description

The patterns and number of constructions are as follows. V n (Cognition): 9. V n (Communication): 10. V n (Material): 41. V n (Relational): 19. V adj: 12. V -ing: 16. V to-inf: 25. V that: 21. V wh: 20. V n n: 20. V n adj: 17. V n -ing: 11. V n to-inf: 27. V n that: 8. V n wh: 8. V about n: 16. V after n: 4. V against n: 12. V around n: 7. V as adj: 2. V as n: 8. V at n: 17. V between pl-n: 4. V by amount: 3. V by -ing: 3. V for n: 21. V from n: 16. V in n: 19. V into n: 18. V of n: 12. V off n: 2. V on n: 30. V onto n: 4. V out of n: 7. V over n: 15. V through n: 7. V to n: 42. V towards n: 5. V with n: 36. V way prep/adv: 23. V n about n: 8. V n against n: 8. V n as adj: 6. V n as n: 10. V n at n: 8. V n by n: 6. V n for n: 21. V n from n: 26. V n in n: 24. V n into n: 18. V n into -ing: 6. V n of n: 8. V n on n: 20. V n out of n: 13. V n to n: 37. V n with n: 29.

3.2 Meaning Groups and Constructions: The Example of ‘V into n’

To recap: Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996) group all the verbs recorded as occurring with a given complementation pattern into ‘meaning groups’. The general purpose of this is to demonstrate that form is linked to meaning, and that the verbs that share complementation patterns tend also to share aspects of meaning (Hunston and Francis Reference Hunston and Francis2000: 83). The more specific purpose is to identify the range of meanings made by each pattern and to organise the long list of verbs found to occur with that pattern into something that makes sense and is usable in language learning and teaching. As noted in Chapter 2 and Section 3.1, the resource has been used by constructions researchers as a starting point for identifying constructions (Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, Römer and O’Donnell2016; Hunston and Su Reference Hunston and Su2019; Perek and Patten Reference Perek and Patten2019), though there is clearly no one-to-one correspondence between construction and meaning group. Rather, the pattern-based meaning groups are a useful resource, a preliminary organisation of a mass of data, available for further use.

In this section, the question of meaning group and construction will be discussed in relation to the pattern V into n (verb + into + noun phrase). It is obvious that this pattern participates in at least two constructions with radically different meanings, exemplified by ‘He turned into a frog’ [= became a frog] and ‘She bumped into a tree’ [= made physical contact with a tree]. The difference in form between the two is the verb: TURN and BUMP. There is, therefore, a ‘turn into’ construction that is markedly different from, on the one hand, a ‘turn left/right’ construction with the same verb and on the other hand, from a ‘physical contact’ construction with the same preposition. If, however, the form is ‘grow into’, as in ‘He grew into a handsome man’, to what extent is this the same construction as ‘turn into’ and to what extent is it different? Both mean a process of becoming, at the end of which the Subject ‘he’ is identifiable as the object of the preposition: ‘a frog’ or ‘a handsome man’. In one case the process implies supernatural involvement, while in the other case it implies gaining maturity.

Arguably, then, it is the case that each verb+ into form is an individual construction, but that some level of generality has to be imposed if the constructions are to do more than replicate a dictionary. The position taken in this book is that there are two superordinate constructions – the ‘change/turn/grow into something’ construction and the ‘bump/barge/crash into something’ construction. There are also more specific constructions, including ‘turn into’, ‘grow into’, and ‘bump into’.

To demonstrate more comprehensively how issues such as this play into deriving constructions from the pattern, 3 of the 13 meaning groups from Francis et al.’s (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996) account of the pattern V into n are discussed further here. (This example was suggested by Peter Uhrig, personal communication). The groups are: the ‘crash’ group; the ‘infiltrate’ group; the ‘turn’ group. Table 3.2 gives the relevant information from each meaning group: its mnemonic name, the rubric for the group, and the verbs listed in the group. The three groups have been selected as examples because they illustrate different decisions made in reinterpreting meaning groups as constructions. It should be recalled that in this book, non-technical names for constructions are used, based on a selected verb from the construction, such as the ‘turn into’ construction or the ‘crash into’ construction. This does not imply that the construction is used with the one verb only. It is proposed that the ‘turn into’ construction, for instance, is used with 34 verbs, or is the superordinate for 34 more specific constructions, while the ‘crash into’ construction is used with 9 verbs, or is the superordinate for 9 more specific constructions. It should also be recalled that a verb may appear in more than one construction, used in slightly different senses or with different collocates. For example, ‘He barged into the railings’ means physical contact has taken place whereas ‘He barged into the room’ does not.

Table 3.2Three meaning groups from the pattern V into n
A table listing three groups, crash, infiltrate, and turn, with the description of each group and the verbs in it, taken from Francis et al. 1996. See long description.
Table 3.2:Long description

The Crash group. Francis et al. 1996: 205. Description: These verbs are concerned with collisions. The prepositional phrase indicates the thing or person that someone or something hits. Verbs: bang, barge, bump, cannon, crash, plough, run, slam, smash. The Infiltrate group. Francis et al. 1996: 207-208. Description: These verbs indicate that someone or something enters a place, group, or thing, physically or metaphorically. Verbs: ascend, assimilate, barge, bleed, book, break, check, cram, creep, cross, crowd, crumble, dive, empty, fall, filter, fit, flash, get, go, hack, infiltrate, integrate, jam, marry, move, pack, pile, plug, push, put, roll, slot, splash, throng, tumble, withdraw. The Turn group. Francis et al. 1996: 207-208. Description: These verbs are concerned with becoming. The prepositional phrase indicates what something becomes. We include here segue and shade, which indicate either that something becomes something else or that it is next to or followed by something else. Verbs: amalgamate, ball, blook, blossom, broaden, build, change, coalesce, condense, convert, curl, decompose, degenerate, develop, erupt, escalate, evolve, fizzle, form, gel, germinate, grow, merge, metamorphose, mushroom, mutate, ossify, segue, shade, transmute, turn, blow up, curl up, shape up.

We return now to the meaning groups shown in Table 3.2.

the ‘crash’ group is treated in this book, and on the Transitivity-Net website, as a single construction, named the ‘crash into’ construction, with the description: ‘An entity collides with another entity’ (‘entity’ here meaning a person or a thing). On the Transitivity-Net website it is construction six in the pattern V into n. The verbs listed for the construction are identical with the list of verbs in the meaning group shown in Table 3.2. The verbs BANG, BARGE, CRASH, SLAM, and SMASH are close synonyms. The verbs CANNON, PLOUGH, and RUN are more problematic: in other contexts they are not associated with this meaning, but ‘cannon into something’, ‘plough into something’, and ‘run into something’ do mean that a collision has taken place. It could be argued that ‘bump into something’ implies a less catastrophic collision, but physical contact still takes place, and the difference is one of degree.

The description of the ‘infiltrate’ group itself includes a list of subgroups (entering a place, group, or thing, which might be a physical location or figurative), suggesting a more eclectic mix of verbs and the possibility of more than one construction mapped on to this meaning group. In response, it is interpreted as seven distinct constructions, as follows (the numbers such as Cx1 refer to number given to this construction on the Transitivity-Net website):

  • Cx1 the ‘crowd into’ construction. Many people move into a space so that the space is full. Verbs: cram, crowd, jam, pack, pile, and throng. Note that the Subject of the verb is plural.

  • Cx2 the ‘barge into’ construction. A person moves into a space. Verbs: barge, break, cross, dive, push, and withdraw.

  • Cx3 the ‘filter into’ construction. A substance moves into a space. Verbs: bleed, empty, and filter.

  • Cx15 the ‘integrate into’ construction. A person joins a community. Verbs: assimilate, infiltrate, integrate, and intrude.

  • Cx16 the ‘get into’ construction. A person joins a social or physical entity. Verbs: book, check, get, hack, marry, and tumble.

  • Cx17 the ‘fit into’ construction. An entity is consistent with a category. Verbs: fall and fit.

  • Cx18 the ‘creep into’ construction. An entity starts to exist somewhere. Verbs: creep and flash.

Even with these seven proposed constructions, the question of how general or specific a construction should be remains. Of the above, construction 16 is probably the most diverse and least satisfactory. It takes a broad, abstract view of the meaning (joining an entity), but risks overstating the similarities between the more specific constructions: ‘book/check into a hotel’; ‘hack into a computer’; ‘get/tumble into a situation’; ‘marry into a family’.

the ‘turn’ group is, arguably, equally diverse, but in this book and the Transitivity-Net website it is treated as a single construction (Cx7). The construction is described as ‘An entity changes to become something else’ and the example given is Though the tenants didn’t like living there, they grew into a community in the course of their campaigns. A total of 34 verbs are listed in the construction. In each case, there is a meaning of ‘become something different’, although, as previously illustrated, more precisely focused constructions could be proposed. These could be:

  • General change, with no specifics added: change into, convert into, mutate into, and turn into.

  • Become a larger entity: broaden into, build into, escalate into, grow into, mushroom into, and blow up into.

  • Change into a more advanced or mature entity: bloom into, blossom into, develop into, evolve into, germinate into, grow into, and shape up into.

  • Conversely, change into something less advanced or mature: degenerate into, fizzle into, and ossify into.

  • Change to a different structure: condense into, decompose into, metamorphose into, mutate into, and transmute into.

  • Several things combine to form one thing: amalgamate into, coalesce into, form into, gel into, and merge into.

  • Change shape: ball into a fist, curl into a ball, curl up into a ball.

  • Gradually change: segue into and shade into.

  • Suddenly change: erupt into.

The main difficulty here is that each verb+preposition instance has a quite specific meaning, and the list as a whole could be divided up in different ways. For example, the ‘become a larger entity’ construction (second bullet point above), could be divided into sequences implying a negative affect (build into, escalate into, mushroom into, and blow up into), and those suggesting a positive change (broaden into and grow into). An alternative would be to pick out mushroom into and match it with balloon into in a construction that in abstract terms is something like ‘[entity] + [verb from noun indicating something that increases in size quickly] + into + [ensuing larger entity]’. The solution adopted here is to take a compromise position and to propose a fairly general superordinate construction. This issue of taxonomy will be returned to in Section 3.4.

3.3 The Most Difficult Pattern: ‘V n’

Before moving on to the representation of construction networks, it is as well to say something about the most difficult pattern dealt with in this study: the V n pattern (verb + noun phrase). This pattern encompasses all transitive verbs (Verb + Object), and it includes also the Verb + Complement sentence pattern. The difficulty of this pattern arises simply because of the sheer volume of verbs that occur in it, the vast range of meanings expressed, and the variety of relations between the verb and the noun phrase that follows it. Halliday (Reference Halliday1994), for example, identifies several kinds of relation between Subject and Object, as shown in examples (1)–(7):

  1. (1) The lion caught the tourist. Actor-Process-Goal (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 109);

  2. (2) I believe you. Senser-Process-Phenomenon (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 118);

  3. (3) John made a statement. Sayer-Process-Verbiage (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 141);

  4. (4) You haven’t signed your name. Actor-Process-Scope (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 149);

  5. (5) Your story sounds complete nonsense. Carrier-Process-Attribute (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 120);

  6. (6) The deadliest spiders are the funnelwebs. Identified-Process-Identifier (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 122);

  7. (7) Peter has a piano. Possessor-Process-Possessed (Halliday Reference Halliday1994: 133).

Halliday and Matthiessen (Reference Halliday and Matthiessen2014) propose distinctions between process types. The primary distinctions are between ‘material’, ‘relational’, ‘mental’, and ‘verbal’ processes (see Chapter 5), but there are also subcategories. Their examples mainly come from verbs that are used intransitively (the pattern V), and those that are used transitively (the pattern V n). For example, they distinguish ‘types of doing-&-happening’ (Halliday and Matthiessen Reference Halliday and Matthiessen2014: 228ff.), noting that these can be ‘creative’ or ‘transformative’, with cross-cutting distinctions between physical and abstract. Creative processes are divided into ‘general’ and ‘specific’. Transformative processes are divided into ‘elaborating’, ‘extending’, and ‘enhancing’. The elaborating processes are further divided into 15 types, of which these are examples: change a state (e.g. burn/freeze/harden something); change the make-up of something (e.g. blow up/prune/smash something); change a surface (e.g. polish/wipe/sweep something); change the size of something (e.g. compress/grow/stretch something); change a shape (e.g. coil/deform/flatten something). For each category, example verbs are given, divided into those that are used intransitively, transitively, or both. This potentially provides the basis for identifying constructions, though a relatively small number of verbs are considered.

There is, then, a vast amount of material to be considered in this pattern. To help with the issue of volume, in this book, the pattern V n has been subdivided into four, based on Halliday’s primary process types, i.e.: V n (Relational); V n (Material); V n (Cognition); V n (Communication). This reduces the number of constructions in each category and, as we shall see, the complexity of the pattern networks.

Another difficulty is that Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996) break with their practice of listing all verbs identified as occurring with a pattern, and in the case of V n (and the intransitive V), list only the most frequent verbs. This was necessary because of space constraints. In this book, the coverage is even less comprehensive, and some verbs have been discarded because they could not be accounted for by a construction. The result of both these decisions is that some of the constructions give the impression of being quite incomplete in terms of the verbs that could be said to belong to them. For example, construction 1 in the V n (material) pattern is the ‘build something’ construction, described as ‘A person or entity brings a physical entity into being’. The construction lists only seven verbs: blow bubbles; build a building; lay a trap; leave a mark; make a meal; rule a line; start a fire. This admittedly feels quite random, with build and make having a very general use but the others having a very restricted collocation. There are, surely, many more verbs in English that mean ‘bring into being’. Many of these, however, did not make the cut in Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996), while others that are frequent enough to have been included, such as start, may be so frequent because they have a range of other uses (start running, start to think, the day started, etc.) rather than because their ‘create’ use is frequent.

The commentary on the ‘build something’ construction above illustrates another noticeable aspect of the V n pattern: it is often necessary to specify collocates in order to make sense of the verbs in a construction. Here are a few examples:

  • V n (relational) Cx8: the ‘develop a fault’ construction. A person or entity gains a quality. Verbs: adopt an attitude; affect an interest; assume a manner; develop a fault; develop an illness; gain weight; gain speed; gather speed; lose weight; lose speed; recover consciousness.

  • V n (relational) Cx11: the ‘follow a route’ construction. A person or entity moves relative to a place or entity. Verbs: clear an object; cover a distance; cross a line; describe a circle; follow a route; jump a fence; run a distance; travel a distance; travel a place; walk a distance; walk a route.

  • V n (communication) Cx9: the ‘make a suggestion’ construction. A person produces an utterance. The verb and the noun phrase following indicate the type of utterance. Verbs: add a remark; ask a question; call a name; draw a comparison; make a suggestion; offer advice; pass a comment; put a question; withdraw a remark.

The pattern V n (material) is particularly problematic in terms of the range of things it covers, and it could well be argued that constructions have been ‘merged’ inappropriately, just to reduce the complexity. Here are two examples:

  • V n (material) Cx4: the ‘repair something’ construction. The description is: ‘A person or entity changes the state of a physical thing’. The verbs listed in this construction on the Transitivity-Net website are arranged in five groups, each of which refers to a different kind of physical change. The first group consists of verbs indicating destruction: break, destroy, fight, hurt, shoot, and strike. The second group consists of verbs with the opposite meaning: fix, mend, and repair. The third group consists of verbs indicating making a change in appearance: mark, paint, and decorate. The fourth group consists of: change, close and open. The fifth group is a random collection: cook food, prepare food, sign a document, and work metal.

  • V n (material) Cx26: the ‘start work’ construction. The description is: ‘A person starts, stops, or continues an activity’. Again, the listed verbs are divided into groups. The first set of verbs indicates ‘stop’: abandon a process; drop an activity; finish an activity; stop an activity. The second set indicates ‘start’: begin talks; attempt a task; open a meeting; start work. The third set indicates ‘continue’: continue an activity; maintain a process; repeat an action. The final two verbs indicate that an activity does not take place are: avoid an activity and prevent an event.

It is probably then the case that specifying constructions from the V n pattern brings into the sharpest focus the question of ‘what a construction is’. It may indeed be the case that each construction should consist of a small number of verbs and quite specific argument collocates. There is certainly scope for splitting the constructions shown on the Transitivity-Net website.

3.4 From Constructions to Construction Network

The first task in this project, then, was to derive a set of Verb Argument Constructions from each verb complementation pattern. The decisions about demarcating one construction from another (what constitutes ‘the same’ form and meaning) make it clear that a simple list is not adequate for recording constructions. Some constructions are more like each other than others are. There is, for example, a more significant difference between ‘turn into a frog’ and ‘bump into a tree’ than between ‘crowd into a space’ and ‘cross into a space’. This is hardly a novel observation. As noted in Chapter 2, researchers who build constructicons aim not only to identify sets of constructions but also to show how they relate to one another (Lyngfelt et al. Reference Lyngfelt, Borin, Ohara and Torrent2018; Herbst and Uhrig Reference Herbst and Uhrig2019). This chapter has the more modest aim of showing how the constructions derived from a single pattern relate to one another. To do this, the concept of the network is adopted from Systemic Functional Grammar (Matthiessen Reference Matthiessen2023). The aim is to represent all the constructions proposed from a given verb complementation pattern as a network. Identifying the nodes of the network gives information about how the constructions are the same as or different from one another. That is, the nodes express the parameters of difference found within a single pattern. The process of moving from pattern to construction to network will now be illustrated in three examples: V n adj; V n to n; V after n.

A Simple Example: V n adj

The first example is the pattern V n adj (verb + noun phrase + adjective). It has been chosen for its relative simplicity. In Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996: 280–285), the pattern is described, as all the verb patterns are, in terms of traditional structural categories, with meaning groups then identified within each structure. For the pattern V n adj, the structures and meaning groups are shown in Table 3.3. It will be noted that one group (group 2.2) is divided into several subgroups, and that where the verb-adjective collocation is restricted, the relevant adjective is shown in the list of verbs. Meaning groups 2.2 (vii) and 2.5 list ‘other’ verbs, that is, verbs that occur with the pattern but which do not fit into any other group. Additional information given by Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996), and not repeated here, is the sense number of each verb as given in the Collins Cobuild English Dictionary (Sinclair. Reference Sinclair1995). Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996) also give examples for several of the verbs in each meaning group, which are not repeated here.

Table 3.3Structures, meaning groups, and verbs in the pattern V n adj (adapted from Francis et al. 1996: 280–285)
A table with three columns showing three structures, the meaning groups in each and the verbs in each meaning group. See long description.
Table 3.3:Long description

Structure 1 is Subject-Verb-Object, for example, He wished both of them dead. There are two meaning groups. The 1.1 like meaning group includes 5 verbs: like, need, prefer, want, wish. The 1.2 imagine meaning group includes 2 verbs: imagine, picture. Structure 2 is Subject-Verb-Object-Object Complement, for example, The doctor caught her asleep. There are five meaning groups, the second divided into seven sub-groups. The 2.1 consider and call group includes 24 verbs: account, believe, brand, call, certify, confess, consider, count, declare, deem, diagnose, find, hold, judge, label, pass, presume, profess, pronounce, prove, rate, report, rule, think. 2.2 make sub-group. The 2.2 1. pull open sub-group includes 26 verbs: blast (open), blow (open), clamp (shut), close, ease, force, jam, kick, lever, nail (shut), open (wide), prize (open), pry, pull, push, shove, shut (tight), slam (shut), slide, spread (wide), tape (shut), tear (open), tug, wedge, wrench (open), yank. The 2.2. 2. squash flat sub-group includes 20 verbs: batter (flat), blot (dry), cram (full), draw (close), jerk (loose), leave (dead), pat (dry), plane (flat), pull (free), scrub (clean), set (free), shake (loose), shoot (dead), squash (flat), squeeze (dry), stuff (full), sweep (clean), towel (dry), wipe (clean), wrench (loose). The 2.2 3. hold steady sub-group includes 4 verbs: have, hold, keep, leave. The 2.2 4. drive mad sub-group includes 8 verbs: batter (unconscious), beat (unconscious), drive (mad), jolt (awake), knock (unconscious), scare (stiff), send (mad), strike (blind). The 2.2 5. turn down low sub-group includes 4 verbs: pitch, crank up, turn down, turn up. The 2.2 6. paint yellow sub-group includes 4 verbs: colour, paint, spray, turn. The 2.2 7. other verbs sub-group includes 4 verbs: get, make, render, slice. The 2.3 find group includes 3 verbs: capture, catch, find. The 2.4 bury alive group includes 3 verbs: burn, bury, skin. The 2.5 other verbs group includes 3 verbs: be born, picture, serve. Structure 3 is Subject-Verb-Object-Complement, for example, Shares ended the day slightly higher. There is one meaning group: 3.1 begin with 4 verbs: begin, end, finish, start.

Altogether, Table 3.3 shows 110 verbs divided into 14 meaning groups. In some cases, reinterpreting these as constructions is relatively straightforward. For example, meaning group 1.2, with the two verbs IMAGINE and PICTURE, can easily be considered a construction. In other cases, decisions have to be made, as they were when the original meaning groups were described. For example, meaning group 2.2 (i) contains:

  • A set of verbs indicating ‘a person [cause] something open’: BLAST, BLOW, EASE, FORCE, KICK, LEVER, PRIZE, PRY, PULL, PUSH, SHOVE, SLIDE, TEAR, TUG, WRENCH, and YANK. (In each case the adjective is open.)

  • A partially overlapping set of verbs indicating ‘a person [cause] something closed’: CLAMP, EASE, FORCE, KICK, NAIL, PULL, PUSH, SHOVE, SLAM, SLIDE, and TAPE. (In each case the adjective is closed or shut.)

  • A set of verbs indicating ‘a person [close/open] something [to a degree]’: OPEN, SHUT, and SPREAD. (The adjective is wide or tight.)

  • A set of verbs indicating ‘a person [forcibly maintain] something [open/closed]’: JAM, NAIL, TAPE, and WEDGE. (The adjective is open, closed or shut.)

It is a matter of judgement as to whether these are treated as one construction or as four ‘smaller’ constructions. In this instance, the constructions follow the meaning group, and one ‘larger’ construction is proposed, though of course these could be divided up.

For the pattern V n adj, a total of 17 constructions are proposed, that is, slightly more than the number of meaning groups. Table 3.4 shows these constructions, giving the construction name, its description, a sample list of verbs and an example taken from the British National Corpus. The construction name and description allow restrictions on form and/or meaning to be recorded. For example, construction 6 has a reflexive pronoun in the place of the noun phrase. Construction 13 specifies that the affected entity is food. In the case of all the constructions, the description specifies whether the Subject in the construction is a person, an entity, or possibly either. Construction 14, for example, specifies ‘a financial entity’. (See Chapter 4 for further discussion of this point.)

Table 3.4Proposed constructions from the pattern V n adj
A table showing 17 constructions. For each construction the table shows the name, description, sample verbs and an example. See long description.
Table 3.4:Long description

Construction 1. The like something quality construction. Description: A person likes something to have a particular quality. Sample verbs: like, prefer. Example: He likes his coffee strong. Construction 2. The want something quality construction. Description: a person wants something to have a particular quality. Sample verbs: need, want, wish. Example: She wants her garden wild. Construction 3. The believe something quality construction. Description: A person thinks that something has a particular quality. Sample verbs: believe, consider, find, presume, think. Example: …many considered them too weak… Construction 4. The imagine something quality construction. Description: A person imagines a possible entity to have a particular quality. Sample verbs: imagine, picture. Example: She imagined him haggard and afraid. Construction 5. The call something quality construction. Description: A person says that something has a particular quality. Sample verbs: brand, call, declare, label, pronounce, report. Example: Other people pronounced them wanton… Construction 6. The profess oneself quality construction. Description: A person says of themselves that they have a particular quality. Sample verbs: confess, find, profess, pronounce, prove, Example: She professed herself reluctant to remain indoors. Construction 7. The make something quality construction. Description: A person or entity changes something so that it has a particular quality. Sample verbs: get, make, render, Example: The pain of the spasms had made him impervious to… Construction 8. The push something open construction. Description: A person or entity moves something so that it is open or closed. Sample verbs: blast, close, force, kick, jam, open, pull, push, tug, yank. Example: The wind had … slammed the door shut. Construction 9. The wipe something clean construction. Description: A person or entity changes something so that it has a particular quality. Sample verbs: squash (flat), towel (dry), cram (full), pull (loose), wipe (clean), Example: She wiped the surface clean. Construction 10. The keep something safe construction. Description: A person or entity maintains something in a particular state. Sample verbs: have, hold, keep, leave, Example: You have to keep the powder dry. Construction 11. The crank the volume up higher construction. Description: A person changes the volume of something. Sample verbs: crank up, pitch, turn down, turn up. Example: She cranked the volume up higher. Construction 12. The colour something red construction. Description: A person or entity changes the colour of something. Sample verbs: colour, paint, spray, turn. Example: Children learn to colour the sky blue. Construction 13. The serve food cold construction. Description: A person serves a meal either hot or cold. Sample verbs: serve. Example: It can be served hot or cold. Construction 14. The begin the day higher construction. Description: A financial entity begins or ends a period of time at a particular level. Sample verbs: begin, end, finish, start. Example: Shares ended the day lower. Construction 15. The leave someone alive construction. Description: A person does or does not kill someone. Sample verbs: leave (alive), shoot (dead). Example: He shot him dead. Construction 16. The capture someone alive construction. Description: A person finds a person in a particular state. Sample verbs: capture, catch, find. Example: …most of the soldiers had been captured alive. Construction 17. The bury someone alive construction. Description: A person causes harm to someone while they are alive. Sample verbs: burn, bury, skin. Example: They were buried alive.

It is immediately apparent from Table 3.4 that while the 17 proposed constructions can be represented as a simple list, they could also be sensibly divided into groups. One clear group is the set of constructions that indicate causation. Constructions 7–13 all mean that one person or entity causes another entity to exist in a particular state or to have a particular quality. Constructions 15–17 could be added to these, though in those cases the caused state is represented by the verb rather than by the adjective, and the acted upon participant is a person. Another group is the set of constructions that indicate construal, that is they mean that a person ‘brings a situation into being’ through thought or speech. These are constructions 1–6. This act of construal could be said to be a kind of figurative causation – a situation is brought about figuratively, by saying or thinking something, rather than literally, by doing something. This leaves construction 14, which remains something of an outlier. These observations are summarised in Table 3.5. In this table, the constructions are identified simply by a number and a verb, rather than by the full construction name. The full name and description can be found in Table 3.4.

Table 3.5Groups of constructions derived from the pattern V n adj
A table listing constructions in groups. See long description.
Table 3.5:Long description

There are three groups: construal, causation and time. Construal is divided into two sub-groups: cognition and communication. Causation is divided into two sub-groups: acted upon participant is entity and acted upon participant is person. The cognition sub-group consists of constructions 1 to 4, that is like, want, believe and imagine. Note: A person brings a situation into being, metaphorically, through thought. The communication sub-group consists of constructions 5 and 6, that is, call and profess oneself. Note: A person brings a situation into being, metaphorically, by saying something. The participant is entity sub-group consists of constructions 7 to 13, that is, make, push open, wipe clean, keep, crank up, colour and serve. Note: The action brings about a situation represented by the adjective. The participant is person sub-group consists of constructions 15 to 17, that is, leave, capture and bury. Note: The action brings about a situation, with the adjective indicating a related circumstance. The time group consists of construction 14, that is begin.

As previously explained, the connections between these constructions can be shown as a network, see Figure 3.1. In this figure, construction 14 is subsumed under ‘causation’, just to neaten the figure. The layout is in a sense arbitrary, moving as it does from left to right rather than from top to bottom, but it follows the convention used in Systemic Functional Grammar. Importantly, although the most specific, rightmost points of the network consist of the named constructions, each node in the network can also be described as a construction – a ‘larger’, or more general, construction. These constructions might be described as:

  • V n adj = the ‘make/construe entity quality’ construction

    • The ‘construe entity quality’ construction

      • The ‘cognise entity quality’ construction

      • The ‘communication entity quality’ construction

    • The ‘cause entity quality/state’ construction

      • The ‘cause entity quality/state’ construction

      • The ‘act_on person quality/state’ construction

A horizontal tree diagram representing the information in Table 3.5. See long description.

Figure 3.1 A network for the constructions derived from the pattern V n adj

Figure 3.1Long description

The diagram depicts the following: V n adj branches into a. construal and causation. Construal is divided into cognition and communication. Causation is divided into change to entity and change to person. Cognition splits into like, want, believe, and imagine. Communication splits into call and profess self. Change to entity splits into make, push open, wipe clean, keep, crank up, colour, serve, and begin. Change to person splits into leave, capture, and bury.

It is thus the case that from the pattern a construction network is proposed comprising levels of generality. From left to right in Figure 3.1, we move from more general constructions to more specific ones.

Figure 3.1 replicates the network that appears on the Transitivity-Net website. It is worth noting, though, that a further stage could identify still more specific constructions that lie between some of the rightmost constructions proposed here and the individual verb constructions. As an example, consider the ‘call something quality’ construction (Cx5). A total of 11 verbs are listed as occurring in this construction: BRAND, CALL, CERTIFY, DECLARE, DIAGNOSE, LABEL, PASS, PRONOUNCE, PROVE, REPORT, and RULE. There are, then, 11 distinct constructions that are here grouped together. It would be possible to take the network further to the right to show how they relate to one another. A ‘prose’ version of the network might look like this:

  • The entry point to the network is the V n adj pattern, where V represents an act of communication. They can be divided into: illocutionary (where the words bring a change in the world) and non-illocutionary (where the words reflect a situation in the world).

    • Illocutionary: CERTIFY, DECLARE, PASS, PRONOUNCE, and RULE. These can be divided into actions that are conventionally written and those that are conventionally spoken.

      • Conventionally written: CERTIFY and PASS

      • Conventionally spoken: DECLARE, PRONOUNCE, and RULE

    • Non-illocutionary: BRAND, CALL, DIAGNOSE, LABEL, PROVE, and REPORT. These can be divided into actions calling on expertise and those not calling on expertise.

      • With expertise: DIAGNOSE and PROVE

      • Without expertise: BRAND, CALL, LABEL, and REPORT. These can be divided into communications that indicate attitude and those that do not.

        • Attitudinal: BRAND, LABEL, and CALL. These form a cline from negative to more neutral

        • Non-attitudinal: REPORT

Figure 3.2 shows this diagrammatically.

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V n adj Communication. See long description.

Figure 3.2 Further degrees of delicacy in the network for the constructions derived from V n adj

Figure 3.2Long description

The first choice is between illocutionary and non-illocutionary. Within illocutionary there is a choice between written and spoken. Within written there are two constructions: certify something adj and pass something adj. Within spoken there are three constructions: declare something adj, pronounce something adj and rule something adj. Within non-illocutionary there is a choice between with expertise and without expertise. Within with expertise, there are two constructions: diagnose something adj and prove something adj. Within without expertise, there is a choice between attitudinal and non-attitudinal. Within attitudinal there are three constructions: brand something adj, label something adj and call something adj. Within non-attitudinal there is one construction: report something adj.

A More Complex Example: V n to n

The second example to be given here is the pattern V n to n (verb + noun phrase + to + noun phrase). This is an example of a pattern comprising an Object noun phrase and a prepositional phrase; patterns of this kind were an important innovation in Francis’s original work (Francis Reference Francis, Baker, Francis and Tognini-Bonelli1993). This pattern is described in Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996) as having three structures:

  • Structure I: Subject-Verb-(Direct) Object-(Indirect) Object. Example: We explained the situation to him.

  • Structure II: Subject-Verb-Object-Object Complement. Example: She changed her name to Caroline.

  • Structure III: Subject-Verb-Object-Adjunct. Example: She banished him to the upstairs room.

There are a few points that need to be made about these structures. Firstly, the notation in Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996) is slightly different from that given above. In that book the distinction between ‘Direct Object’ and ‘Indirect Object’ is not made, but novel terms ‘Prepositional Object’ and ‘Prepositional Complement’ are used. In the examples given above, ‘to him’ (Structure I) is labelled as a Prepositional Object and ‘to Caroline’ (Structure II) is labelled as a ‘Prepositional Object Complement’. These terms might be glossed as ‘like an ordinary Object (etc.) but a prepositional phrase’. Secondly, integral to Francis’s work is that the optional Adjunct element (see Structure III) is not excluded from the description of verb complementation if the form of the Adjunct (in this case, a prepositional phrase beginning with to) is dependent on the verb.

Francis et al. (Reference Francis, Hunston and Manning1996: 418–433) identify 10 meaning groups with Structure I, one group with Structure II, and 19 groups with Structure III, giving a total of 30 meaning groups. For this book, these groups have been reinterpreted as 37 constructions and these are organised, as in the case of V n adj, as a network. At the most general, or leftwards extreme of the network is a distinction based on the type of process indicated by the construction. Inspired by the process types proposed by Halliday (Halliday and Matthiessen Reference Halliday and Matthiessen2014), but changing the terminology somewhat, these processes are: Action, Communication, Cognition, and Relation. Because the networks can become too dense to see clearly, the practice adopted here is to show detail in separate figures. Figure 3.3 shows the constructions subsumed by Communication, Cognition, and Relation only. In the case of Communication, a further distinction is made between communicating about information (e.g. ‘break the news to someone’) and communicating about action: telling someone to do something (e.g. ‘challenge someone to a duel’) or promising to do something (e.g. ‘promise help to someone’). In the case of Cognition, a distinction is made between constructions where the Cognizer is the Subject (e.g. ‘person prefers tea to coffee’ Cx30) and those where some other entity is the Subject (e.g. ‘person drew my attention to the problem’ Cx33).

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V n to n and with detail shown for the choices action, communication, cognition, and relation. See long description.

Figure 3.3 Communication, Cognition, and Relation process-based constructions from the pattern V n to n

Figure 3.3 long description.

Figure 3.3Long description

Action doesn’t have any further choices. Within Communication, there is a choice between about information and about action. Within about information, there are five constructions: 20 break the news to someone; 21 announce information to someone; 22 commend something to someone; 23 denounce something to someone; 24 demonstrate something to someone. Within about action, there are three constructions: 25 challenge someone to an action; 26 suggest an action to someone; 27 promise action to someone. Within Cognition, there is a choice between Cognizer as Subject and Non-Cognizer as Subject. Within Cognizer as Subject, there are two constructions: 30 someone prefers one thing to another; 31 someone accustoms themselves to a situation. Within Non-Cognizer as Subject, there are four constructions: 28 a quality attracts someone to something; 29 a quality endears something to someone; 32 something awakens someone to something; 33 someone draws attention to something. Within Relation, there are four constructions: 34 someone compares one thing to another; 35 someone attributes a quality to something; 36 something imparts a quality to something; 37 one topic leads a discourse to another topic.

For clarity, the constructions shown in Figure 3.3 are expressed more fully than those in Figure 3.1. It should be remembered that in each case several verbs can be assigned to a construction. For example, construction 34 (a person compares one thing to another) occurs with verbs such as anchor, connect, correlate, index, liken, link, match (up), relate, and tie, as well as with compare. As previously noted, more specific constructions could be proposed from this list.

Construction 34 illustrates a further point: although networks are a convenient way to express relationships between constructions, they do tend to stress, or even exaggerate, the differences between alternatives. It could well be argued that construction 34 indicates an act of Cognition (someone thinks that two things are similar) as well as a process of Relation (one thing is similar to another). The same could be said of construction 35. The network representation does not allow for a construction to be ‘partly this and partly that’. However, overlaps of this kind are considered further in Chapter 4.

The ‘Action’ processes that occur with the pattern V n to n are rather more complex and interesting. They are shown in Figure 3.4. First, a distinction can be drawn between the actions of ‘transfer’, ‘change’, and ‘cause’ (Figure 3.4a). An example of transfer is ‘give something to someone’ (Cx1), an example of change is ‘change one thing to another’ (Cx9), and an example of cause is ‘incite someone to an action’ (Cx17). A second relevant distinction appears to be between literal and figurative actions. In the case of ‘transfer’ (Figure 3.4b) and ‘cause’ (Figure 3.4d), the constructions reflect this distinction. Literal transfer is exemplified by ‘give something to someone’ (Cx1), while figurative transfer is exemplified by ‘grant respect to someone’ (Cx5). There is one literal cause construction – ‘deport someone to a place’ (Cx12) – while the other constructions express figurative rather than literal movement, such as ‘promote someone to a role’ (Cx15). Again, the network configuration requires that fuzzy boundaries be treated as clear. For example, constructions such as Cx17 (‘incite someone to action’) and Cx18 (‘condemn someone to an ordeal’) are treated as figurative, though the results may be physical enough, because the acts of inciting and condemning are generalised accounts of complex situations and not as literal as the act of deportation. In the case of ‘change’ constructions (Figure 3.4c), the figure may raise questions, because the same numbered constructions are shown as both literal and figurative. This is because this distinction is not made in respect of these constructions on the Transitivity-Net website. However, it makes sense to distinguish between, for example, ‘choke someone to death’ (Cx11), where the victim actually dies, and ‘bore someone to death’ (also Cx11), where the most usual interpretation is that the victim remains alive. Within figurative cause actions another distinction is made: between acting on an abstract entity and acting on a person. Constructions 17 and 18 exemplify acting on a person, while constructions 13 and 14 (e.g. ‘attach a condition to a ruling’ and ‘broaden the scope of the enquiry to the whole organisation’) exemplify acting on an entity.

Within Action, there are three choices: Transfer (as shown in Figure 3.4b), Change (as shown in Figure 3.4c), and Cause (as shown in Figure 3.4d). All of them have two choices each: literal and figurative.

Figure 3.4a Action process-based constructions from the pattern V n to n

Tree diagram showing constructions in the transfer network. See long description.

Figure 3.4b Action process-based constructions, the transfer network

Figure 3.4bLong description

Within Transfer, there are two choices: literal and figurative. Within literal, there are two constructions: 1 give something to someone; 2 mail something to someone. Within figurative, there are five constructions: 3 allocate resources to something; 4 bequeath assets to someone; 5 grant respect to someone; 6 concede argument to someone; 7 introduce someone to someone.

Tree diagram showing constructions in the change network. See long description.

Figure 3.4c Action process-based constructions, the change network

Figure 3.4c:Long description

Within Change, there are two choices: literal and figurative. Within literal, there are four constructions: 8 increase sound to a level; 9 change something to something; 10 attach one thing to another; 11 batter someone to death. Within figurative, there are four constructions: 8 increase tension to a level; 9 change abstraction to abstraction; 10 add a comment to an argument; 11 bore someone to death.

Tree diagram showing constructions in the cause network. See long description.

Figure 3.4d Action process-based constructions, the cause network

Figure 3.4d:Long description

Within Cause, there are two choices: literal and figurative. Within literal, there is one construction: 12 deport someone to a place. Within figurative, there are two choices: on entity and on person. Within on entity, there are two constructions: 13 connect one abstraction to another; 14 broaden a scope to a wider one. Within on person, there are five constructions: 15 promote someone to a role; 16 delegate activity to a person; 17 incite someone to an action; 18 condemn someone to a situation; 19 apply oneself to an action.

As with the pattern V n adj, the constructions that are identified can be expressed as more general than the 37 listed, and indeed as more specific. It has already been noted that constructions 8–11 can be (and probably should be) divided into two more specific constructions each. Going up the hierarchy, or leftwards in the network, the more general constructions might be expressed as:

  • The ‘person transfers_possession to person’ construction.

  • The ‘person/entity changes entity to a state’ construction.

  • The ‘person/entity causes person/entity to new situation’ construction.

  • The ‘person communicates something to someone’ construction.

  • The ‘person cognizes one thing to another thing’ construction.

  • The ‘entity causes person to mental state’ construction.

  • The ‘person forms_relation one thing to another thing’ construction.

The Indeterminacy of Networks: V after n

The discussions of the previous two patterns have made the point that the proposed networks are to an extent arbitrary, in the sense that in many cases alternative versions might be proposed. This point is made more strongly in respect to the final example, the pattern V after n. Four constructions only are proposed for this pattern:

  • Cx1 the ‘yearn after something’ construction. Description: A person wants something very much. Verbs: HANKER, HUNGER, LUST, THIRST, and YEARN. Example: He’s never hankered after the travelling life. (BNC)

  • Cx2 the ‘follow after someone’ construction. Description: A person follows another person, probably with negative intentions. Verbs: CHASE and FOLLOW. Example: I’ve been threatened, spat on, chased after … (BNC)

  • Cx3 the ‘chase after something’ construction. Description: A person tries to get a physical or abstract entity that is difficult to get. Verbs: CHASE, GO, and RUN. Example: All these years you’ve spent chasing after something you can’t have. (BNC)

  • Cx4 the ‘clean up after someone’ construction. Description: A person helps someone, especially by correcting their mistakes and/or doing something they should have done. Verbs: CLEAN UP, CLEAR UP, RUN AROUND, and TIDY UP. Example: He always expected other people to clean up after him. (BNC)

The network shown on the Transitivity-Net website prioritises the distinction between processes: cognition (‘yearn after’) as opposed to action (the other constructions). Then a distinction is made between literal actions (‘follow after someone’) and figurative ones (‘chase after’ and ‘clean up after’). This is shown in Figure 3.5. An alternate network would begin with what might be called the two meanings or functions of after: indicating an aim or desire (‘yearn after’ and ‘chase after a desired thing’); indicating space or time (‘follow after someone’ and ‘clean up after someone’). Figure 3.5b shows this alternative. The point is that neither of these is transparently ‘correct’ but that both model one perspective on the relationship between the constructions.

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V after n and an initial choice between cognition and action. See long description.

Figure 3.5a Alternative networks for constructions derived from V after n

Figure 3.5a:Long description

The diagram depicts the entry as V after n. There is an initial choice between cognition and action. Within cognition, there is one construction: 1 yearn after something. Within action, there is a choice between literal and figurative. Within literal, there is one construction: 2. follow after someone. Within figurative there are two constructions: 3. chase after something and 4. clean up after someone.

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V after n and an initial choice between aim after and space/time after. See long description.

Figure 3.5b Alternative networks for constructions derived from V after n

Figure 3.5b long description.

Figure 3.5b:Long description

The diagram depicts the entry as V after n. There is an initial choice between aim after and space/time after. Within aim after, there are two constructions: 1. yearn after something and chase after something and 3. Chase after something. Within space/time after, there are two constructions: 2. follow after someone and 4. clean up after someone.

3.5 The Construction Networks as Heuristic: Identifying Distinctions

In this study, then, the constructions drawing on each pattern are modelled in a network. As well as the ‘end-point’ constructions, the nodes of the network can be interpreted as ‘more general’ constructions. The networks are a way of organising the constructions neatly, but they also act as a heuristic to discover what language resources are relevant to each set of constructions. The distinctions that are made in deriving the networks give an insight into how constructions with the same complementation form might differ because of the configuration of the other elements in the construction.

To give a sense of the range of distinctions that have been proposed, two verb complementation patterns will be given as examples.

Example: V n that

The first example is the pattern V n that (verb + noun phrase + that-clause). Eight constructions are identified from this pattern. They are:

  • Cx1: speaker informs/notifies/teaches/tells someone that …

  • Cx2: speaker reassures/guarantees/promises someone that …

  • Cx3: speaker warns/cautions someone that …

  • Cx4: speaker persuades/convinces someone that …

  • Cx5: speaker bets/wagers someone that …

  • Cx6: evidence shows/decides/tells someone that …

  • Cx7: cognizer deludes/flatters/kids themselves that …

  • Cx8: thought hits/strikes someone that …

The network accounting for these is shown in Figure 3.6. In forming the network, the following distinctions are made:

  • A distinction in the process: communication (Cx1–Cx6) or cognition (Cx7 and Cx8) (Figure 3.6). Note that construction 6 is ambiguous in this respect. On the one hand, the evidence acts like a speaker, ‘telling’ or ‘showing’ someone that something is the case. This is, however, a metaphoric representation of what is ‘really’ happening: a person is forming a thought based on the available evidence. Thus the evidence communicates to a cognizer that something is the case. Because of the network format, the construction has to be classed as communication or cognition: for the purposes here, the surface form of ‘telling’ takes precedence. Chapter 4 discusses instances such as this further and shows examples of dual coding.

  • A distinction in the source of the communication/cognition (Figures 3.6b and 3.6c). This may be a person (Cx1–Cx5, where the source is a speaker, also Cx7, where the source is the cognizer), or an entity (Cx6), or a thought/idea (Cx8).

  • A distinction in whether the communication leads to a change or not (Figure 3.6b). In most cases, the giving of information does not necessarily lead to change: someone can be told something or warned of something without changing thoughts or actions. In the case of the verbs PERSUADE and CONVINCE (Cx4), however, there is a perlocutionary effect of the hearer changing their perception, and in the case of BET and WAGER (Cx5) there is a change in the situation of speaker and hearer – they have formed a contract.

  • Finally, Cx1–Cx3 are distinguished by the presence or absence of affect (Figure 3.6b). Cx1 (INFORM) does not necessarily imply affect. Cx2 (REASSURE) implies positive affect and Cx3 (WARN) implies negative affect.

Tree diagram with the entry V n that. See long description.

Figure 3.6a The network of constructions from the pattern V n that

Figure 3.6a:Long description

V n that has an initial choice between communication (as shown in 3.6b) and cognition (as shown in 3.6c). Within communication, there is a choice between person as source and entity as source. Within cognition, there is a choice between self as source and thought as source.

Tree diagram with the entry V n that Communication. See long description.

Figure 3.6b V n that: Communication constructions

Figure 3.6b:Long description

Within communication, there is a choice between person as source and entity as source. Within person as source, there is a choice between no change in hearer and change in hearer. Within no change in hearer, there is a choice between no affect, the construction 1. speaker informs hearer that, positive affect, the construction 2. speaker reassures hearer that, and negative affect, the construction 3. speaker warns hearer that. Within change in hearer, there are two constructions: 4. speaker persuades hearer that and 5. speaker bets hearer that. Within entity as source, there is one construction: 6. evidence shows cognizer that.

Within cognition there is a choice between self as source, the construction: 7. cognizer deludes self that, and thought as source, the construction: 8. thought hits cognizer that.

Figure 3.6c V n that: Cognition constructions

Figure 3.6a shows the network of constructions derived from the pattern V n that.

If this network were continued to the right, that is, making distinctions of greater delicacy, more parameters of difference could be identified. In construction 1, for example, there are seven verbs: INFORM, INSTRUCT, NOTIFY, REMIND, SHOW, TEACH, and TELL. These could be divided into groups:

  • The ‘give information’ constructions: the ‘inform someone that’ construction; the ‘notify someone that’ construction; the ‘tell someone that’ construction.

  • The ‘give instruction’ constructions: the ‘instruct someone that’ construction; the ‘teach someone that’ construction.

  • The ‘renew information’ construction: the ‘remind someone that’ construction.

  • The ‘non-specific modality’ construction: the ‘show someone that’ construction.

These distinctions use concepts such as ‘general vs specific modality’ (talking versus showing), the nature of the speech act involved (telling versus teaching), and the new/old status of the content (telling versus reminding).

Example: V n with n

The second example discussed here is the pattern V n with n (verb + noun phrase + with + noun phrase). A total of 29 constructions are proposed for this pattern. The network is shown in Figure 3.7. This network draws on the following distinctions.

  • The first distinction made is based on the apparent meaning of the preposition with that arises out of the meaning of the construction. Three meanings are proposed: ‘conjunction’, ‘transfer’, and ‘manner’ (Figure 3.7a). ‘Conjunction’ means that with indicates two entities in relation to each other; for example, ‘contrast one thing with another’ (Cx1), or ‘link one thing with another’ (Cx6), or ‘exchange blows with someone’ (Cx5) (Figure 3.7b). ‘Transfer’ means that with indicates that something gains possession of something or that one thing is placed in proximity to another. Examples include ‘furnish someone with something’ (Cx11), ‘sweeten a dish with something’ (Cx16), or ‘imbue a situation with a quality’ (Cx19) (Figure 3.7c). ‘Manner’ means that with indicates how something is done; for example, ‘answer a question with a comment’ (Cx24), or ‘oblige someone with a service’ (Cx27) (Figure 3.7d). All the constructions with this pattern are assigned to one or other meaning of with.

  • The second distinction is made based on the nature of the process expressed by the construction. The process types are based on but not identical to those used by Halliday (Halliday and Matthiessen Reference Halliday and Matthiessen2014). They are: ‘relation’; ‘action’; ‘communication’; ‘cognition’. An example of ‘relation’ is ‘preface one thing with another’ (Cx2). An example of ‘action’ is ‘exchange blows with someone’ (Cx5). An example of ‘communication’ is ‘bandy words with someone’ (Cx9). An example of ‘cognition’ is ‘amaze someone with something’ (Cx22). Each of the preposition meaning types has constructions with more than one process type. All the constructions with this pattern are assigned to one or other process type.

  • The third distinction is between ‘literal’ and ‘figurative’ meaning constructions. Although all the constructions are either literal or figurative, the distinction is made only when there are constructions that differ along this dimension. In practice, this means that it is used only with a subset of the ‘action’ constructions. Literal actions include examples such as ‘blend one thing with another’ (Cx3), ‘furnish someone with something’ (Cx11), and ‘stock an area with something’ (Cx18). Figurative actions include examples such as ‘fuse one thing with another’, glossed as ‘A person establishes a connection between two abstract entities’ (Cx6), ‘imbue a situation with a quality’ (Cx19), and ‘beset someone with problems’ (Cx20).

  • The eight constructions that share the features of ‘transfer’, ‘action’ and ‘literal’ are further distinguished by whether the construction relates to ‘possession’ or to ‘location’. The ‘possession’ constructions are: ‘furnish someone with something’ (Cx11) and ‘saddle someone with something’ (Cx12). The ‘location’ constructions are: ‘decorate something with something’ (Cx13); ‘coat something with something’ (Cx14); ‘flood something with something’ (Cx15); ‘sweeten something with something’ (Cx16); ‘pepper something with something’ (Cx17); ‘stock an area with something’ (Cx18).

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V n with n. See long description.

Figure 3.7a The network of constructions from the pattern V n with n

Figure 3.7a:Long description

From the root V n with n, there is an initial choice between conjunction with (shown in 3.7b), transfer with (shown in 3.7c), and manner with (shown in 3.7d). Within conjunction with, there is a choice between Relation, Action and Communication. Within transfer with, there is a choice between Action and Cognition. Within manner with, there is a choice between communication and action.

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V n with n Conjunction. See long description.

Figure 3.7b V n with n: Conjunction with constructions

Figure 3.7b:Long description

Within conjunction with, there is a choice between Relation, Action, and Communication. Within Relation, there are two constructions: 1 contrast one thing with another and 2 preface one thing with another. Within Action, there is a choice between Literal and Figurative. Within literal, there are three constructions: 3 blend one thing with another; 4 match one thing with another; 5 exchange blows with someone. Within figurative, there are three constructions: 6 fuse one thing with another; 7 intersperse one thing with another; 8 replace one thing with another. Within Communication, there are two constructions: 9 bandy words with someone; 10 forge agreement with someone.

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V n with n Transfer. See long description.

Figure 3.7c V n with n: Transfer with constructions

Figure 3.7c:Long description

Within transfer with, there is a choice between Action and Cognition. Within Action, there is a choice between literal and figurative. Within literal, there is a choice between possessions and location. Within possession, there are two constructions: 11 furnish someone with something; 12 saddle someone with something. Within a location there are 6 constructions: 13 decorate something with something; 14 coat something with something; 15 flood something with something; 16 sweeten something with something; 17 pepper something with something; 18 stock an area with something. Within figurative, there are three constructions: 19 imbue a situation with a quality; 20 beset someone with problems; 21 temper something with something. Within Cognition, there are two constructions: 22 amaze someone with something; 23 confront someone with something.

A horizontal tree diagram with the entry V n with n Manner. See long description.

Figure 3.7d V n with n: Manner with constructions

Figure 3.7d:Long description

Within manner with, there is a choice between communication and action. Within communication, there are two constructions: 24 answer a question with words; 25 charge someone with a crime. Within action, there are four constructions: 26 assist someone with a task; 27 oblige someone with a service; 28 occupy self with an activity; 29 begin a time period with an activity

3.6 Conclusion

This chapter has illustrated two processes: reinterpreting each of 54 verb complementation patterns as constructions and representing those constructions as a taxonomy or network. Both processes are, admittedly, subjective and open to challenge or revision, but they make use of concepts found elsewhere in the field, such as the distinction between process types, or between literal and figurative meaning. Examples of verb argument constructions and networks are given in this chapter. Constructions and networks for each of the 52 grammar patterns considered in this study can be found on the Transitivity-Net website.

As a conclusion to the chapter, it is worth considering again what is meant by ‘construction’ in this book, and to illustrate what kinds of form and meaning slots occur in those constructions, bearing in mind Haspelmath’s (Reference Haspelmath2023) account of a construction having ‘at least one open slot’. Consider example (8):

  1. (8) She struck me as a very bossy, short-tempered teacher. (BNC)

This is an instance of the grammar pattern V n as n, and it is an instance of the ‘strike someone as’ construction. There is one other verb in the same construction – impress – and this is one of 10 constructions proposed with the same pattern.

Example (8) consists of five elements, shown in Table 3.6. Row 1 shows the example. Row 2 shows the form of the construction: noun + verb + noun + as + noun. Row 5 explains the meaning of the construction as a whole. Row 3 describes the range of form and meaning available in each slot. NP1 construes any person or thing. NP2 construes only a person (something capable of an opinion). NP3 construes only a characteristic or identity, such as ‘a very bossy, short-tempered teacher’. The verb may be only strike or impress. The preposition may only be as. Row 4 classifies the slots into four types: the preposition is a fixed item; the verb is a slot that is variable within a small range; NP1 is an open slot; NP2 and NP3 are ‘restricted’ slots – they do not contain items that are listable like the verbs, but their meaning is more restricted than NP1. With these constructions, then, there is not a simple distinction between ‘closed’ and ‘open’ slots, but degrees of variation and fixedness. This would seem to be a characteristic, maybe even a distinguishing characteristic, of verb argument constructions.

Table 3.6 illustrates the presentation of constructions as a sequence of elements. In many traditions, those elements are labelled with their semantic roles. Chapter 4 discusses this issue and explains the process of semantic role labelling in this study.

Table 3.6Slots in verb argument constructions
A table annotating the elements of an example. See long description.

Example (1) consists of five elements, shown in Table 3.6. Row 1 shows the example. The header row shows the form of the construction: noun (NP1) + verb + noun (NP2) + as + noun (NP3). Row 2 describes the range of form and meaning available in each slot. NP1 construes any person or thing. NP2 construes only a person (something capable of an opinion). NP3 construes only a characteristic or identity, such as ‘a very bossy, short-tempered teacher’. The verb may be only STRIKE or IMPRESS. The preposition may only be as. Row 3 classifies the slots into four types: the preposition is a fixed item; the verb is a slot that is variable within a small range; NP1 is an open slot; NP2 and NP3 are ‘restricted’ slots – they do not contain items that are listable like the verbs, but their meaning is more restricted than NP1.With these constructions, then, there is not a simple distinction between ‘closed’ and ‘open’ slots, but degrees of variation and fixedness. This would seem to be a characteristic, maybe even a distinguishing characteristic, of verb argument constructions. In summary, the meaning of the construction as a whole is: NP2 has an opinion about NP1, ascribing to NP1 the characteristic of NP3, based on NP1’s appearance, speech or actions.

Table 3.6:Long description

The example she struck me as a very bossy short-tempered teacher is divided into four elements: 1 she; 2 struck; 3 me; 4 as; 5 s very bossy short-tempered teacher. The first line of annotation is: 1 noun N P 1; 2 verb; 3 noun N P 2; 4 as; 5 noun N P 3. The second line of annotation is: 1 Noun phrase construing any person or thing; 2 One of a limited set of verbs-strike, impress; 3 Noun phrase construing a person only; 4 as; 5 Noun phrase construing a characteristic or identity only. The third line of annotation is: 1 Open slot; 2 Variable slot; 3 Restricted slot; 4 Fixed item; 5 Restricted slot.

Figure 0

Table 3.1 List of patterns and the number of constructions identified in eachTable 3.1 long description.

Figure 1

Table 3.2 Three meaning groups from the pattern V into nTable 3.2 long description.

Figure 2

Table 3.3 Structures, meaning groups, and verbs in the pattern V n adj (adapted from Francis et al. 1996: 280–285)Table 3.3 long description.

Figure 3

Table 3.4 Proposed constructions from the pattern V n adjTable 3.4 long description.

Figure 4

Table 3.5 Groups of constructions derived from the pattern V n adjTable 3.5 long description.

Figure 5

Figure 3.1 A network for the constructions derived from the pattern V n adjFigure 3.1 long description.

Figure 6

Figure 3.2 Further degrees of delicacy in the network for the constructions derived from V n adjFigure 3.2 long description.

Figure 7

Figure 3.3 Communication, Cognition, and Relation process-based constructions from the pattern V n to nFigure 3.3 long description.

Figure 8

Figure 3.4a Action process-based constructions from the pattern V n to n

Figure 9

Figure 3.4b Action process-based constructions, the transfer networkFigure 3.4b long description.

Figure 10

Figure 3.4c Action process-based constructions, the change networkFigure 3.4c long description.

Figure 11

Figure 3.4d Action process-based constructions, the cause networkFigure 3.4d long description.

Figure 12

Figure 3.5a Alternative networks for constructions derived from V after nFigure 3.5a long description.

Figure 13

Figure 3.5b Alternative networks for constructions derived from V after nFigure 3.5b long description.

Figure 14

Figure 3.6a The network of constructions from the pattern V n thatFigure 3.6a long description.

Figure 15

Figure 3.6b V n that: Communication constructionsFigure 3.6b long description.

Figure 16

Figure 3.6c V n that: Cognition constructions

Figure 17

Figure 3.7a The network of constructions from the pattern V n with nFigure 3.7a long description.

Figure 18

Figure 3.7b V n with n: Conjunction with constructionsFigure 3.7b long description.

Figure 19

Figure 3.7c V n with n: Transfer with constructionsFigure 3.7c long description.

Figure 20

Figure 3.7d V n with n: Manner with constructionsFigure 3.7d long description.

Figure 21

Table 3.6 Slots in verb argument constructionsTable 3.6 long description.

Accessibility standard: WCAG 2.1 AA

The HTML of this book complies with version 2.1 of the Web Content Accessibility Guidelines (WCAG), covering newer accessibility requirements and improved user experiences and achieves the intermediate (AA) level of WCAG compliance, covering a wider range of accessibility requirements.

Content Navigation

Table of contents navigation
Allows you to navigate directly to chapters, sections, or non‐text items through a linked table of contents, reducing the need for extensive scrolling.
Index navigation
Provides an interactive index, letting you go straight to where a term or subject appears in the text without manual searching.

Reading Order & Textual Equivalents

Single logical reading order
You will encounter all content (including footnotes, captions, etc.) in a clear, sequential flow, making it easier to follow with assistive tools like screen readers.
Short alternative textual descriptions
You get concise descriptions (for images, charts, or media clips), ensuring you do not miss crucial information when visual or audio elements are not accessible.
Full alternative textual descriptions
You get more than just short alt text: you have comprehensive text equivalents, transcripts, captions, or audio descriptions for substantial non‐text content, which is especially helpful for complex visuals or multimedia.

Visual Accessibility

Use of colour is not sole means of conveying information
You will still understand key ideas or prompts without relying solely on colour, which is especially helpful if you have colour vision deficiencies.

Structural and Technical Features

ARIA roles provided
You gain clarity from ARIA (Accessible Rich Internet Applications) roles and attributes, as they help assistive technologies interpret how each part of the content functions.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×