To save content items to your account,
please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies.
If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account.
Find out more about saving content to .
To save content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org
is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings
on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part
of your Kindle email address below.
Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations.
‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi.
‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
To investigate whether anxiety reductions attributed to healing crystals reflect placebo responses driven by conditioning and belief-related biases rather than specific therapeutic effects.
Methods
In a randomized, controlled study, 138 adults were classified as believers or nonbelievers in crystal efficacy and assigned to rose quartz (experimental) or a visually matched placebo. Participants followed a standardized 14-day protocol. Anxiety was assessed pre- and post-intervention with the Beck Anxiety Inventory and the Spanish Kuwait University Anxiety Scale. Multilevel analyses of variance (ANOVA) and Bayesian models were used to evaluate main effects, interactions, and evidence for treatment specificity.
Results
Anxiety reductions occurred only among believers, regardless of crystal assignment. No differences were detected between groups in primary outcomes, and improvements did not exceed the magnitudes typically associated with placebo responses. Bayesian estimates favored the null hypothesis for specific treatment effects. Preexisting belief strongly predicted perceived efficacy and symptom change, consistent with causal illusions plausibly shaped by conditioning mechanisms. Nonbelievers showed no reliable improvement.
Conclusion
Healing crystals did not demonstrate anxiolytic effects beyond those of the placebo. Symptom change was mediated by expectancy and conditioning, particularly in individuals inclined toward intuitive or magical thinking. Although nonspecific, context-dependent factors—such as elements of the therapeutic alliance—may amplify placebo responsiveness in clinical settings, these findings do not support attributing inherent therapeutic value to crystals. Future work should delineate how expectations, clinician-patient rapport, and related variables interact to shape placebo response and how such mechanisms might be ethically leveraged to enhance evidence-based care without promoting pseudoscientific practices.
Chapter 4 shows how the embodied and enacted psychophysiology of metaphor can explain mechanisms of symbolic healing. Recent research on placebo responding and predictive processing or active inference theories in computational neuroscience suggest models for the physiology effects of placebos, imagery, and imaginative enactments. Examples drawn from traditional shamanistic practices illustrate how healing metaphors and images map bodily physiology, cognition, and experience onto metaphoric landscapes or myths. Movement in these landscapes or along an arc then gives rise to corresponding changes in physiology, cognitive, and social relationships or position, which make use of the dynamics of sensory and affective meaning, including processes of abreaction or catharsis and aesthetic distance. Healing rituals involve a hierarchy of cognitive processes that are structured metaphorically, which reaches down to physiological processes and outward to social interactions. Its multiple levels can operate in tandem to reinforce or subvert processes. This leads to a view of symbolic action and healing ritual as involving multiple parallel levels of causality and communication.
Antidepressant medications are widely prescribed for depression and other uses. They are considered a first-line treatment for major depressive disorder. We examine the lack of support for the mechanistic idea that neurotransmitters affect and are affected by these medications. Few people experience significant benefit from their use when compared with the effects of placebos. We consider several ethical issues associated with antidepressants, including conflicts of interest among the committees recommending their use, and examine a study that suffered from spin and other issues of integrity. The chapter examines potential alternatives to antidepressant medications for those with depression.
Patient expectancy is an important source of placebo effects in antidepressant clinical trials, but all prior studies measured expectancy prior to the initiation of medication treatment. Little is known about how expectancy changes during the course of treatment and how such changes influence clinical outcome. Consequently, we undertook the first analysis to date of in-treatment expectancy during antidepressant treatment to identify its clinical and demographic correlates, typical trajectories, and associations with treatment outcome.
Methods
Data were combined from two randomized controlled trials of antidepressant medication for major depressive disorder in which baseline and in-treatment expectancy assessments were available. Machine learning methods were used to identify pre-treatment clinical and demographic predictors of expectancy. Multilevel models were implemented to test the effects of expectancy on subsequent treatment outcome, disentangling within- and between-patient effects.
Results
Random forest analyses demonstrated that whereas more severe depressive symptoms predicted lower pre-treatment expectancy, in-treatment expectancy was unrelated to symptom severity. At each measurement point, increased in-treatment patient expectancy significantly predicted decreased depressive symptoms at the following measurement (B = −0.45, t = −3.04, p = 0.003). The greater the gap between expected treatment outcomes and actual depressive severity, the greater the subsequent symptom reductions were (B = 0.49, t = 2.33, p = 0.02).
Conclusions
Greater in-treatment patient expectancy is associated with greater subsequent depressive symptom reduction. These findings suggest that clinicians may benefit from monitoring and optimizing patient expectancy during antidepressant treatment. Expectancy may represent another treatment parameter, similar to medication compliance and side effects, to be regularly monitored during antidepressant clinical management.