Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-2bdfx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-27T21:30:26.642Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predictive role of loneliness on mortality before the age 85 years among mid- to later-life adults in the United States: a 10-year retrospective cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2025

Hui-Ying Fan
Affiliation:
School of Nursing, Hainan Medical University, Haikou, HI, China Unit of Psychiatry, Department of Public Health and Medicinal Administration, Institute of Translational Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, MO, China Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, MO, China
Mu-Rui Zheng
Affiliation:
Unit of Psychiatry, Department of Public Health and Medicinal Administration, Institute of Translational Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, MO, China Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, MO, China
Qinge Zhang
Affiliation:
Beijing Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders & National Center for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
Sha Sha
Affiliation:
Beijing Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders & National Center for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
Yuan Feng
Affiliation:
Beijing Key Laboratory of Mental Disorders, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders & National Center for Mental Disorders, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, China
Zhaohui Su
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, Southeast University, Nanjing, China
Teris Cheung
Affiliation:
School of Nursing, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Kowloon, HK, China
Gabor S Ungvari
Affiliation:
Section of Psychiatry, University of Notre Dame Australia, Fremantle, Australia Division of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Western Australia, Perth, Australia
Lloyd Balbuena
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, SK, Canada
Yu-Tao Xiang*
Affiliation:
Unit of Psychiatry, Department of Public Health and Medicinal Administration, Institute of Translational Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, MO, China Centre for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, University of Macau, Taipa, MO, China
*
Corresponding author: Yu-Tao Xiang; Email: xyutly@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

Loneliness is a common public health concern, particularly among mid- to later-life adults. However, its impact on early mortality (deaths occurring before reaching the oldest old age of 85 years) remains underexplored. This study examined the predictive role of loneliness on early mortality across different age groups using data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS).

Methods

A retrospective cohort study was conducted using data from the 2010–2020 waves of the HRS, restricted to participants aged 50–84 years at baseline. Loneliness was measured using the 11-item UCLA Loneliness Scale, categorized into four levels: low/no loneliness (scores 11–13), mild loneliness (14–16), moderate loneliness (17–20) and severe loneliness (21–33). Cox proportional hazards models and time-varying Cox regression models with age as the time scale were created to evaluate the relationship between loneliness and early mortality, adjusting for sociodemographic, lifestyle, and physical and mental health factors.

Results

Among 6,392 participants, the overall mortality rate before the age of 85 years was 19.1 per 1,000 person-years. A dose–response relationship was observed, with moderate and severe loneliness associated with 23% (adjusted hazard ratio [aHR]: 1.23, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.02–1.48) and 36% (aHR: 1.36, 95% CI = 1.13–1.65) higher mortality risk, respectively. Significant associations existed for the 65–74-year-old (aHR = 1.37, 95% CI = 1.03–1.83) and 75–84-year-old (aHR = 1.77, 95% CI = 1.23–2.56) age groups in the fully-adjusted models, but not for the 50–64-year-old age group. Time-varying Cox models showed a stronger association for severe loneliness (aHR = 1.65, 95% CI = 1.37–1.99).

Conclusions

Loneliness is a significant predictor of mortality among older adults. Preventive and interventional programs targeting loneliness may promote healthy ageing.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press.

1. Introduction

Loneliness refers to the distress experienced when there is a perceived discrepancy between an individual’s desired and actual social connections, often arising when existing relationships fail to meet expectations (Hawkley, Reference Hawkley2022). Unlike social isolation, which refers to an objective lack of social contacts or networks, loneliness is a subjective experience. It is particularly common among mid- to later-life adults. According to the US National Poll on Healthy Aging, the proportion of adults aged 50–80 years who reported feeling socially isolated increased from 27% in 2018 to 56% during the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020. By early 2023, this figure remained notable, with over one-third of older adults in the United States reporting chronic loneliness (ongoing feelings of isolation and/or lack of companionship) (Malani et al., Reference Malani, Singer, Kirch, Solway, Roberts, Smith, Hutchens and Kullgren2023).

The Evolutionary Theory of Loneliness (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, Reference Cacioppo, Cacioppo and Olson2018) proposed that when people feel lonely, they often feel less secure in their social and physical environment, which can make people more sensitive to possible social threats and lead them to expect negative interactions with others. Such expectations can influence social behaviours that reinforce feelings of isolation, which can trigger a range of negative psychological and biological responses. Studies also found that loneliness extends beyond emotional distress and has adverse health consequences. It interferes with mental health, leading to depression (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Hawkley and Thisted2010; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Wang, Pearce, Ma, Schlief, Lloyd-Evans, Ikhtabi and Johnson2022), maladaptive coping behaviours, such as physical inactivity and alcohol abuse (Åkerlind and Hörnquist, Reference Åkerlind and Hörnquist1992; Hawkley et al., Reference Hawkley, Thisted and Cacioppo2009), and accelerates cognitive impairment which is a major contributor to mortality among older adults (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Zhang, Shen, Gao, Li, Lv, Shi and Mao2024; Zhong et al., Reference Zhong, Chen, Tu and Conwell2017). Additionally, loneliness is linked to chronic physical illnesses such as diabetes, cancer, cardiovascular conditions and stroke (Office of the Surgeon General, 2023), which ranks among the leading causes of mortality in the U.S. population (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Kochanek, Xu and Arias2021). Its health impact is comparable to smoking 15 cigarettes/day and exceeds the risks associated with obesity and physical inactivity (Holt-Lunstad et al., Reference Holt-Lunstad, Robles and Sbarra2017). Numerous studies, including a meta-analysis of 70 studies (Holt-Lunstad et al., Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris and Stephenson2015), have shown an association of loneliness with an increased risk of all-cause mortality. Another meta-analysis of 90 cohort studies found that loneliness increased all-cause mortality by 14% and cancer mortality by 9% in the general population (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Gao, Han, Yu, Long, Jiang, Wu, Pei, Cao, Ye, Wang and Zhao2023).

Despite the abundance of evidence linking loneliness and overall mortality, its association with healthy aging is underexplored. Social and medical progress in recent decades have increased life expectancy at birth, and it was now approximately 73.5 years globally and 78.8 years in the United States in 2019 (Collaborators and Ärnlöv, Reference Collaborators and Ärnlöv2020; GBD US Health Disparities Collaborators, 2022). Longevity is a fundamental human aspiration, and there is increased interest in the so-called Blue Zones of exceptional longevity (Kreouzi et al., Reference Kreouzi, Theodorakis and Constantinou2024). Longevity is often viewed as reaching the oldest old stage, typically defined as the age of 85 years or older, a widely accepted threshold for exceptional longevity in developed countries including the United States (National Research Council, 2001). Such longevity implies not only physical health but high levels of subjective well-being (Hitchcott et al., Reference Hitchcott, Fastame and Penna2018), so it is plausible that loneliness is a barrier to healthy ageing. Following prior research (Vaupel et al., Reference Vaupel, Zhang and Van Raalte2011), we defined early mortality as death before the age of 85 years in this study to focus on factors limiting survival to this milestone.

Developmental stages in life modify the relationship between loneliness and mortality. For instance, middle-aged adults (50–64 years) may experience loneliness differently from young-old (65–74 years) and old-old adults (75–84 years) due to differences in life circumstances, social roles and health conditions (Greer et al., Reference Greer, Lynch, Reeves, Falkenbach, Gingrich, Cylus and Bambra2021). Middle-aged adults often face work-related stress, caregiving responsibilities or social transitions, which can exacerbate feelings of loneliness (Das and S Das, Reference Das, Das, Das and Chakraborty2024 ). In contrast, older adults are more likely to experience widowhood, declining health and reduced mobility, all of which contribute to loneliness in distinct ways (Reynolds 3rd et al., Reference Jeste, Sachdev and Blazer2022). However, few studies have conducted stratified analyses to explore how loneliness affects mortality risk across different age groups (Surkalim et al., Reference Surkalim, Luo, Eres, Gebel, Van Buskirk, Bauman and Ding2022). Furthermore, many studies primarily focus on adults aged 65 years and above, overlooking middle-aged individuals.

To fill these gaps, this study aimed to (1) explore the association between loneliness and early mortality among mid- to later-life adults and (2) examine whether this association varies across different age groups.

2. Methods

2.1 Study design and samples

This was a retrospective cohort study based on the most recent 10-year dataset of the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), spanning from the 2010 to 2020 wave (Waves 10–15). The HRS, an ongoing nationally representative steady-state longitudinal study, has biennially surveyed U.S. adults aged 50 years and above since its inception in 1992 (Wave 1) (Sonnega et al., Reference Sonnega, Faul, Ofstedal, Langa, Phillips and Weir2014). The details of the methodology of the HRS have been previously described (Heeringa and Connor, Reference Heeringa and Connor1995). This study made use of both HRS core data (including loneliness-related data) and exit data (including all-cause mortality data). Participants surveyed during the 2010 wave served as the baseline cohort and were followed electronically through the 2020 wave.

To be included in the study sample, participants were required to (1) be 50–84 years old and (2) have completed loneliness assessment at baseline. Individuals were excluded from our sample if (1) they had incomplete sociodemographic or health-related data that could not be imputed or (2) data were collected after their death date. Missing baseline data for sociodemographic, lifestyle or health variables were imputed using the last observation carried forward (LOCF) and next observation carried forward (NOCF) approaches as recommended (Lydersen, Reference Lydersen2019; Nakai et al., Reference Nakai, Chen, Nishimura and Miyamoto2014). Ethical approval for the HRS was granted by the Institutional Review Board of Virginia Commonwealth University (No. HM20023839). Informed consent was obtained from all participants by the HRS investigators. As a secondary analysis of HRS data, the present study was exempt from ethical review.

2.2 Measurements

2.2.1 Loneliness

Loneliness was measured using the 11-item UCLA (University of California, Los Angeles) Loneliness Scale (UCLA-11) (Lee and Cagle, Reference Lee and Cagle2017; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Ryan, Fisher, Sonnega and Weir2017), which is a short and validated version of the 20-item Scale (Version 3) (Russell, Reference Russell1996). Participants rated their frequency of feelings including (1) lacking companionship, (2) left out, (3) isolated from others, (4) in tune with others, (5) alone, (6) having people they could talk to, (7) having people they could turn to, (8) understood by others, (9) close to others, (10) part of a friend group and (11) a sense of shared commonality with others around them. Responses were recorded on a 3-point Likert scale (1 = ‘often’ to 3 = ‘hardly ever or never’). Items 1, 2, 3 and 5 were reversely scored. Total scores ranged from 11 to 33, with higher scores indicating more severe loneliness. Following a previous study of cancer survivors (Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Reese, Han and Yabroff2024), UCLA-11 total scores were classified into four levels based on quartiles of the distribution: 11–13 (low or no loneliness), 14–16 (mild loneliness), 17–20 (moderate loneliness) and 21–33 (severe loneliness), which enables the exploration of loneliness patterns and subgroup comparisons among mid- to later-life adults in the United States.

2.2.2 Ascertainment of mortality before the age 85 years

Mortality data for each HRS wave were collected through exit interviews with next-of-kin and cross-referenced with the National Death Index, ensuring high identification rates (Weir, Reference Weir2016). The outcome was defined as all-cause mortality occurring before the oldest old stage (≤85 years) (National Research Council, 2001), monitored from the 2010 wave to the 2020 wave. Given that HRS interviews in a given wave were not conducted within a single calendar year (e.g. the 2010 wave included interviews spanning 2010 and 2011), the follow-up period for each participant began in the month of their interview during the 2010 wave (conducted in 2010 or 2011) and ended in the month of their interview during the 2020 wave (conducted in 2020 or 2021) (Kezios et al., Reference Kezios, Lu, Calonico, Hazzouri and Al Hazzouri2023). Participants who died ≤85 years old had follow-up terminated at the date of death, while those who survived or died >85 years were censored upon reaching 85 years. Individuals who were alive and under the age of 85 years throughout the follow-up period were censored at the date of their 2020 wave interview, whereas those lost to follow-up under the age of 85 years were censored at the time of their final interview. Person-years were calculated from the start of follow-up to either death or censorship.

2.2.3 Covariates

Sociodemographic and health-related variables potentially related to loneliness symptoms and all-cause mortality were identified at baseline. These variables included age, gender (male vs. female), marital status (categorized as married, divorced/separated, widowed, or never married/other), educational level (≥12 years vs. <12 years) and alcohol use (yes vs. no). Health-related variables included self-reported physician-diagnosed major physical diseases recorded in the HRS, such as diabetes, cancer, heart disease and stroke (absence vs. presence).

Psychiatric problems, including depression, insomnia and cognitive impairment, were also measured. Depression was evaluated using the 8-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD-8) (Van de Velde et al., Reference Van de Velde, Levecque and Bracke2009). The CESD-8 items are (1) feeling depressed, (2) everything was an effort, (3) sleep disturbance, (4) happiness, (5) loneliness, (6) enjoying life, (7) feeling sad and (8) lack of motivation. Each item was scored as 1 or 0 (yes or no), with items 4 and 6 reverse-scored. The total scores ranged from 0 to 8, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. A cut-off score of 4 was used to categorize individuals as depressed (Mojtabai and Olfson, Reference Mojtabai and Olfson2004).

Insomnia symptoms were measured using the 4-item Jenkins Sleep Scale (JSS-4) (Jenkins et al., Reference Jenkins, Stanton, Niemcryk and Rose1988), which assessed (1) difficulty initiating sleep, (2) difficulty maintaining sleep, (3) early morning awakenings and (4) feeling rested in the morning. Each item was rated on a 3-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (‘most of the time’) to 3 (‘rarely or never’). Item 4 was reverse-scored. Higher total scores reflected more severe insomnia symptoms, with a cut-off score of 5 used to identify the presence of insomnia (Von Känel et al., Reference Von Känel, Meister-Langraf, Zuccarella-Hackl, Schiebler, Znoj, Pazhenkottil, Schmid, Barth, Schnyder and Princip2022).

Cognitive function was assessed using the modified version of the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-m) (Brandt et al., Reference Brandt, Spencer and Folstein1988). The TICS-m assesses five cognitive domains: (1) memory (both immediate and delayed recall), (2) attention, (3) calculation, (4) orientation and (5) language. Due to the high rate of missing data in the orientation and language domains, this study focused instead on memory, attention and calculation, following previous research (Crimmins et al., Reference Crimmins, Kim, Langa and Weir2011). Memory was assessed through immediate and delayed recall tasks (each scored out of 10), with a total memory score ranging from 0 to 20. Attention was measured using a backward counting task, scored from 0 to 2. Calculation was assessed with a serial subtraction test, with scores ranging from 0 to 5. The total TICS-m score was calculated by summing the scores of these three domains, ranging from 0 to 27, with higher scores indicating better cognitive performance.

2.3 Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using R (version 4.4.2) (R Core Team, 2020). Frequencies and percentages as well as means and standard deviations (SDs) were used for describing categorical and continuous variables, respectively. All covariates were compared across four loneliness levels using chi-squared and Wilcoxon rank sum tests, as appropriate. Statistical significance was set at an alpha of 0.05 (two-tailed).

2.3.1 Time-to-event analyses

Each participant’s person-time was calculated from the date of their baseline assessment (2010 wave) to either the dates of death ≤85 years or censorship. All-cause mortality rates before the age of 85 years were reported per 1,000 person-years and stratified by loneliness quartiles. To evaluate the association between loneliness severity at baseline and subsequent all-cause mortality before the age of 85 years, Cox proportional hazards regression models were used to calculate hazard ratios (HRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Three models were constructed, adding covariates progressively. Model 1 adjusted for age and sex; Model 2 added marital status, educational level and alcohol use to Model 1 covariates; and Model 3 further adjusted for history of major physical diseases (i.e. diabetes, cancer, heart conditions and stroke), insomnia (i.e. dichotomized by JSS-4 scores ≥5 or <5), depression (i.e. dichotomized by CESD-8 scores ≥4 or <4) and cognitive function (total TICS-m scores). Loneliness severity, the main exposure of interest, was entered as a categorical variable with the lowest quartile as the reference group.

To assess if the relationship between loneliness and pre-age-85 mortality was moderated by age, we created age-stratified Cox proportional hazards regression models. The groups were 50–64, 65–74 and 75–84 years old. Kaplan–Meier survival curves were plotted to examine potential differences in survival rates across four levels of loneliness and across three different age groups.

Following a previous study (Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Reese, Han and Yabroff2024), we performed time-varying Cox regression models using age as the time scale (Korn et al., Reference Korn, Graubard and Midthune1997), which enable a more precise adjustment for age-specific mortality risks and provide a dynamic perspective on the role of loneliness in mortality before the age of 85 years among middle-aged and older populations. Furthermore, a restricted cubic spline regression with three knots was applied to examine potential nonlinear relationships between loneliness symptoms and risk of mortality before the age of 85 years. To further explore the relationship between individual UCLA-11 items and mortality before the age of 85 years, each item was transformed into a binary variable, with responses coded as 1 (often or some of the time) and 0 (hardly ever or never). All 11 items were entered into Model 3 simultaneously for analysis.

2.3.2 Sensitivity analyses

To assess the robustness of the association between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years, four sensitivity analyses were conducted: (1) excluding older individuals at baseline, defined as those within 2 or 5 years of age 85; (2) treating depressive and insomnia symptoms as continuous variables rather than binary variables in Model 3, using revised total CESD-8 scores (excluding item CESD5, ‘Loneliness’, due to its conceptual overlap with the UCLA-11) and the total JSS-4 score, respectively; (3) using the UCLA-3 scale as a binary variable instead of the UCLA-11; and (4) using the complete cases analysis (7,030 participants) without LOCF or NOCF imputations. The UCLA-3 scale is derived from the 20-item UCLA Loneliness Scale and has demonstrated satisfactory reliability and validity consistent with the original scale (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo2004). It assesses participants’ perceptions of (1) lacking companionship, (2) feeling left out and (3) feeling isolated from others. Each item was reverse-scored, with total scores ranging from 3 to 9. Higher scores indicated greater loneliness severity, and a score of 6 or higher was classified as having loneliness symptoms (Qi et al., Reference Qi, Malone, Pei, Zhu and Wu2023).

Results

3.1 Participant characteristics

Of the 22,034 HRS participants in the 2010 wave, we excluded 14,248 participants with incomplete or missing UCLA-11 responses (e.g. partial answers or failure to complete the scale), 273 younger than 50 years, 415 age 85 years or older, 2 with data collected posthumously and 704 with missing sociodemographic or health-related information that made imputation unfeasible. The analytical sample consisted of 6,392 participants.

Baseline sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of participants stratified by the four loneliness levels are summarized in Table 1. The mean age of participants was 65.59 years (SD = 9.40), and the majority were female (n = 3,630; 56.8%), were married (n = 4,131; 64.6%) and had at least a high school education (n = 5,487; 85.8%). Univariate analyses indicated significant differences in loneliness levels across most sociodemographic and health-related characteristics (P < 0.001), except for a history of cancer (P = 0.34).

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of participants by severity of loneliness (N = 6,392)

Notes: Bolded values indicate P < 0.05. SD: standard deviation; CESD-8: 8-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale; JSS-4: 4-item Jenkins Sleep Scale; TICS-m: modified version of the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status Scale.

* Analysed using the Wilcoxon rank sum test.

3.2 The association between loneliness and mortality before age 85 years

3.2.1 Mortality rates before age 85 years

During an average follow-up duration of 7.6 years, a total of 922 deaths were recorded among the 6,392 participants by the 2020 wave. The overall mortality rate before the age of 85 years was 19.1 per 1,000 person-years among midlife and older U.S. adults. Additionally, the mortality rate before the age of 85 years accelerated with age: 9.8 per 1,000 person-years for participants aged 50–64 years, 25.1 per 1,000 person-years for those aged 65–74 years and 42.5 per 1,000 person-years for those aged 75–84 years (Table 3).

3.2.2 The association between loneliness and mortality before oldest old age

Column 3 of Table 2 shows a dose–response relationship between loneliness levels and mortality rate before the age of 85 years, and this relationship persists across all three models. In the fully adjusted model, participants with moderate loneliness (UCLA-11 scores 17–20) and severe loneliness (21–33) had 23% and 36% higher risks of all-cause mortality before the age of 85 years, respectively, compared to those with low/no loneliness (11–13) at baseline (adjusted Hazard ratio [aHR] = 1.23, 95% CI = 1.02–1.48 vs. aHR = 1.36, 95% CI = 1.13–1.65). The Kaplan–Meier survival curve (Fig. 1a) illustrates significant differences in survival rates across different loneliness levels (P < 0.001).

Figure 1. Kaplan–Meier survival curve: (a) Kaplan–Meier survival curve by loneliness score and (b) Kaplan–Meier survival curve by age group.

Table 2. The association between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years (N = 6,392)

Notes: Bolded values: P < 0.05

HR: hazard ratio; CI: confidence interval.

Model 1 was adjusted for age and sex.

Model 2 was adjusted for age, sex, marital status, educational level and alcohol consumption.

Model 3 was adjusted for the same covariates as Model 2 plus history of major physical diseases (diabetes, cancer, heart conditions and stroke), insomnia symptoms, depressive symptoms and cognitive function (total TICS-m scores).

The variance inflation factor (VIF) values for the loneliness item of the CESD-8 scale with each item of the UCLA-11 scale were 1.293–2.695, which suggests an absence of multicollinearity. In general, VIF values greater than 10 indicate problematic multicollinearity, while values between 1 and 5 typically reflect low-to-moderate correlation among the variables. Therefore, we used the total CESD-8 score with the cut-off value of 4 indicating participants with depressive symptoms.

In Table 3, this association is stratified by three age groups: 50–64 years, 65–74 years and 75–84 years. Using participants with low or no loneliness (11–13) as the reference group, no significant associations were observed after adjusting for all confounders in the 50–64 age group (Model 3). For the age group of 65–74 years, mild and moderate loneliness was associated with increased mortality in Models 1 and 2, but only severe loneliness demonstrated a consistent and significant association across all models (aHR = 1.37, 95% CI = 1.03–1.83 in Model 3). In contrast, in the 75–84 age group, all levels of loneliness significantly increased mortality risk, with severe loneliness having the highest risk (aHR = 1.77, 95% CI = 1.23–2.56 in Model 3). As expected, Fig. 1b shows that older age groups had lower survival rates compared with younger age groups (P < 0.001).

Table 3. Associations between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years by different age groups (N = 6,392)

Notes: Bolded values: P < 0.05; HR: hazard ratio; CI: confidence interval.

Model 1 was adjusted for age and sex.

Model 2 was adjusted for age, sex, marital status, educational level and alcohol consumption.

Model 3 was adjusted for the same covariates as Model 2 plus history of major physical diseases (diabetes, cancer, heart conditions and stroke), insomnia symptoms, depressive symptoms and cognitive function (total TICS-m scores).

Restricted cubic spline regression analyses with three knots showed a linear and positive association between total loneliness scores and mortality before the age of 85 years (non-linear P = 0.20, Fig. 2). Supplementary Table S1 shows the results of time-varying Cox proportion hazards models. In Model 3, a stronger association between severe loneliness and mortality was observed (aHR = 1.65, 95% CI = 1.37–1.99) compared to the unstratified Cox model (aHR = 1.36, 95% CI = 1.13–1.65). Supplementary Table S2 shows that none of the specific loneliness symptoms were significantly associated with mortality before the age of 85 years (all P > 0.05).

Graphs show hazard ratios (HRs) according to loneliness symptom scores, adjusted for age, sex, marital status, educational level, living with others, alcohol consumption, insomnia symptoms, depressive symptoms, total TICS-m scores, diabetes, cancer, heart conditions and stroke. Data were analysed using a three-knotted restricted cubic spline Cox proportional hazards regression model. The total UCLA-11 score ranges from 11 to 33, with higher scores indicating more severe loneliness symptoms. Solid lines represent HRs, and shaded areas indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 2. Adjusted hazard ratios of mortality rates according to loneliness symptom scores.

3.3 Sensitivity analyses

Consistent associations between loneliness quartiles and mortality before the age of 85 years were observed in Cox proportional hazards regression models: (1) after excluding participants reaching 85 years within 2 and 5 years at baseline (Supplementary Table S3); (2) using revised total CESD-8 scores (excluding item CESD5, ‘Loneliness’) and the total JSS-4 score (Supplementary Table S4); and (3) utilizing the full dataset of 7,030 participants without applying any imputations (Supplementary Table S5). However, when the UCLA-3 scale was used in place of UCLA-11, the association between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years in Model 3 was no longer significant (aHR = 1.14; 95% CI = 0.98–1.32; Supplementary Table S6).

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this was the first longitudinal study that explored the association between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years among mid- to later-life adults in the United States using a large, nationally representative dataset. The findings revealed a significant dose–response relationship between loneliness and pre-age-85 mortality. After adjusting for a wide range of sociodemographic, lifestyle, physical and mental health factors, moderate and severe loneliness was associated with 23% and 36% higher mortality risks, respectively. Sensitivity analyses confirmed the robustness of these results across various sampling conditions, except when a shorter loneliness instrument was used.

This finding is consistent with previous research showing a significant association between loneliness and an increased risk of all-cause mortality in mid- to later-life populations (Holt-Lunstad et al., Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris and Stephenson2015; Rico-Uribe et al., Reference Rico-Uribe, Caballero, Martín-María, Cabello, Ayuso-Mateos and Miret2018). A prior HRS cohort study based on the 2002–2008 waves with 1,604 participants aged 60 years and older similarly found that loneliness was strongly linked to higher mortality risk (Perissinotto et al., Reference Perissinotto, Stijacic Cenzer and Covinsky2012). However, our finding contrasts with some studies that found no association between loneliness and mortality (Henriksen et al., Reference Henriksen, Larsen, Mattisson and Andersson2019; Steptoe et al., Reference Steptoe, Shankar, Demakakos and Wardle2013; Stessman et al., Reference Stessman, Rottenberg, Shimshilashvili, Ein-Mor and Jacobs2013; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Leng, Zhao, Fleming and Brayne2020). A longitudinal study in the United Kingdom with 2,166 individuals aged 75 years and older found no significant association after adjusting for demographic factors, health conditions and depression (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Leng, Zhao, Fleming and Brayne2020). These discrepancies may arise from differences in covariate adjustment, loneliness assessment and length of follow-up. The present study used two forms of a standard loneliness scale, had a large sample size and had a decade-long follow-up period, which lend credence to our findings.

Our focus on deaths occurring before the age of 85 years may rule out more cases of mortality from purely physical causes, since frailty tends to increase with age. Loneliness likely contributes to early old age mortality through a combination of physiological, behavioural, mental health and social mechanisms. Physiologically, loneliness can induce increased inflammation and elevated stress hormone levels (e.g. cortisol) (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford, Ernst, Burleson, Kowalewski, Malarkey, Van Cauter and Berntson2002), which may impair immune function and raise the risk of chronic diseases like cardiovascular disease and metabolic disorders, ultimately increasing the risk of early mortality (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Bu and Fancourt2021). Behaviourally, individuals experiencing significant loneliness may be more prone to adopting harmful coping strategies such as poor diet, physical inactivity and substance abuse, all of which could have direct negative effects on physical health and increase the risk of early mortality (Hawkley and Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010). Psychosocially, loneliness may worsen mental health conditions such as depression and anxiety, which are well-established risk factors for mortality (Hawkley and Cacioppo, Reference Hawkley and Cacioppo2010; Mann et al., Reference Mann, Wang, Pearce, Ma, Schlief, Lloyd-Evans, Ikhtabi and Johnson2022) including suicide (Motillon-Toudic et al., Reference Motillon-Toudic, Walter, Séguin, Carrier, Berrouiguet and Lemey2022). Loneliness reduces social interaction and limits engagement in preventive health behaviours or care-seeking, thereby worsening chronic conditions and increasing vulnerability to acute health events.

An important finding of this study was the variation in the loneliness–mortality relationship across different age groups. After adjusting for health-related factors, no significant association was found between loneliness and early mortality among middle-aged adults (aged 50–64 years). This indicates that loneliness might not be an independent risk factor for mortality in middle-aged adults when other health-related factors (e.g. chronic diseases, depression and cognitive decline) are accounted for. The relatively better health status, stronger social networks, employment-related social interactions, and family responsibilities and support may mitigate the effect of loneliness on mortality, thus nullifying the association. In contrast, severe loneliness predicted early mortality in young-old (65–74 years) adults, as did all loneliness levels (mild, moderate and severe) in old-old (75–84) adults. The time-varying Cox regression model confirms these findings by revealing a stronger relationship between severe loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years, indicating that the impact of loneliness on mortality increases as individuals age.

Several factors may explain these findings. Although young-old adults may still have relatively stronger social networks and fewer age-related health conditions compared to old-old adults, severe loneliness may still exacerbate underlying health vulnerabilities (e.g. chronic diseases and depression), thereby increasing early mortality risk. In contrast, older-old adults may be facing more health challenges, so a lower level of loneliness can lead to early mortality (World Health Organization, 2023). These health challenges include increased physical comorbidities, cognitive decline, depressive symptoms as well as diminished social networks and support due to retirement or the loss of loved ones (Cudjoe et al., Reference Cudjoe, Roth, Szanton, Wolff, Boyd and Thorpe2020; National Academies of Sciences et al., 2020), which makes them more vulnerable to the negative consequences of loneliness and to a higher mortality risk even at lower levels of loneliness (Luo et al., Reference Luo, Hawkley, Waite and Cacioppo2012).

Our findings have important clinical implications. First, a routine screening for loneliness should be incorporated into healthcare assessments for all mid- to later-life adults, particularly for those undergoing major life transitions. Second, for young-old adults experiencing severe loneliness, early interventions are necessary to prevent or reduce the risk of negative health effects. Unlike old-old adults who are expected to be retired, middle-aged adults are generally expected to have jobs. For example, the loss of employment (or lack of job opportunities) may lead to prolonged social withdrawal, a phenomenon called ‘hikikomori’ that was first observed in Japan but also present in other countries (Kato et al., Reference Kato, Kanba and Teo2019, Reference Kato, Tateno, Shinfuku, Fujisawa, TEO, Sartorius, Akiyama, Ishida, Choi, Balhara, Matsumoto, Umene-Nakano, Fujimura, Wand, Chang, Chang, Shadloo, Ahmed, Lerthattasilp and Kanba2012).

For old-old adults, a more comprehensive care plan is required to actively address all levels of loneliness, as it is a key risk factor for early mortality (National Academies of Sciences et al., 2020). Enhancing social skills, improving social support, increasing social opportunities and altering maladaptive social cognition are recommended as these multi-faceted approaches are found to be more effective than those focusing on a single aspect (Patil and Braun, Reference Patil and Braun2024). Specifically, certain interventions, such as animal therapy, psychological therapy and skill-building activities, have proven to be more effective than strategies centred solely on social facilitation or health promotion (Hoang et al., Reference Hoang, King, Moore, Moore, Reich, Sidhu, Tan, Whaley and Mcmillan2022; Patil and Braun, Reference Patil and Braun2024). These targeted and age-specific approaches are essential to reduce the mortality risks associated with loneliness, ultimately improving quality of life and health outcomes for older adults.

Although the UCLA-11 total score showed a significant association with mortality risk, individual loneliness symptoms were not linked to mortality before the age of 85 years. This finding indicates that the global effect of multiple loneliness symptoms, rather than any individual symptom alone, may be more predictive of early mortality risk. Sensitivity analysis using the UCLA-3 scale as a dichotomous variable revealed no significant associations. This discrepancy aligns with a study (Raymo and Wang, Reference Raymo and Wang2022) that found different associations of loneliness measured by the UCLA-11 and UCLA-3 with sociodemographic factors and health outcomes. Differences in scale length and content may account for these variations. While the UCLA-3 scale is practical for quick assessments, it may not fully capture the cluster of symptoms, focusing solely on social isolation (Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Waite, Hawkley and Cacioppo2004). In contrast, the UCLA-11 scale incorporates additional dimensions, such as available social connections and a sense of belonging (Lee and Cagle, Reference Lee and Cagle2017), and is thus a more comprehensive assessment and more sensitive to subtle associations of loneliness with mortality. These findings indicated that loneliness measurement tools may influence results, highlighting the need for standardized assessments to be used in future research. Future research should consider the choice of suitable measurement scales when examining the impact of loneliness on health outcomes, balancing the need for precision with practicality.

This study has several strengths including the use of a large, representative dataset and robust statistical models that controlled for a wide range of confounders. The longitudinal design enables temporal inference, providing stronger evidence for a potential causal relationship between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years. However, several limitations should be noted. First, certain confounding variables related to both loneliness and mortality, such as social isolation, social support, early-life social experiences and functional limitations, were not included in the analysis due to numerous missing data in the HRS. Second, relying on self-reported data in this study may have introduced potential recall or social desirability biases. Third, although previous research demonstrated that loneliness can be classified into transient and chronic subtypes, with each having distinct impacts on health outcomes (e.g. cognitive decline) (Zhong et al., Reference Zhong, Chen and Conwell2016), our study measured loneliness only at baseline, which may limit the ability to capture its dynamic nature and may potentially underestimate the cumulative or changing effects of loneliness on early mortality. Future studies with repeated assessments of loneliness are needed to better differentiate the health impacts of chronic versus transient loneliness and to clarify their mechanisms in relation to early mortality risk. Fourth, the study population was drawn from the United States, which may limit the generalizability of findings to other cultural contexts. Future research could explore whether these associations existed in diverse populations and consider how cultural factors might shape the interplay between loneliness and health outcomes. Fifth, we examined the interaction between the severity of loneliness and gender and found no significant effect modification, perhaps partly due to limited statistical power. Future research should examine more detailed interaction analyses in larger and more diverse samples or should include more comprehensive covariate data (e.g. socioeconomic status and race/ethnicity) to better understand potential heterogeneity in the association between loneliness and early mortality. Finally, using the age of 85 years as a binary cut-off might introduce artificial censoring and may misclassify mortality outcomes among the oldest old. Although we conducted sensitivity analyses excluding individuals close to the age of 85 years at baseline to mitigate this issue, this approach, which is widely used in both clinical practice and research, may still underestimate mortality risk in very old age and limit the ability to capture mortality dynamics beyond this age.

In conclusion, this study highlights loneliness as a significant predictor of mortality before the age of 85 years, particularly among older adults. By focusing on this critical age threshold, the findings contribute to a deeper understanding of the health impacts of loneliness and emphasize the urgent need to address this public health issue. Future research could explore targeted interventions and underlying mechanisms to mitigate the effects of loneliness and enhance health longevity and quality of life among mid- to later-life populations.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796025100188.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the University of Michigan Health and Retirement Study Platform at https://hrsdata.isr.umich.edu/data-products/public-survey-data.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to all participants and investigators involved in this study.

Author contributions

Study design: H.-Y.F., S.S., Y.F., Q.Z. and Y.-T.X.; data collection, analysis and interpretation: H.-Y.F., M.-R.Z., Z.S., T.C., G.S.U. and L.B.; drafting of the manuscript: H.-Y.F., M.-R.Z. and Y.-T.X.; critical revision of the manuscript: L.B.; and approval of the final version for publication: all co-authors. H.-Y.F., M.-R.Z., Q.Z., S.S. and Y.F. contributed equally to this work.

Financial support

The study was supported by the Hainan Provincial Natural Science Foundation of China (821QN249), the Education Department of Hainan Province (Hnjg2024ZC-55), the Hainan Medical University (HYYB202326), Beijing High Level Public Health Technology Talent Construction Project (Discipline Backbone-01-028), the Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (No. Z181100001518005), the Capital’s Funds for Health Improvement and Research (CFH 2024-2-1174) and the University of Macau (MYRG2022-00187-FHS; MYRG-GRG2023-00141-FHS).

Competing interests

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Ethical standards

Ethical approval for the HRS was granted by the Institutional Review Board of Virginia Commonwealth University (No. HM20023839). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2013.

References

Åkerlind, I and Hörnquist, JO (1992) Loneliness and alcohol abuse: a review of evidences of an interplay. Social Science & Medicine 34, 405414.10.1016/0277-9536(92)90300-FCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, J, Spencer, M and Folstein, M (1988) The telephone interview for cognitive status. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 1, 111117.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, JT and Cacioppo, S (2018) Chapter three - loneliness in the modern age: an evolutionary theory of loneliness (ETL). In Olson, J. M (eds), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Academic Press, 127197.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, JT, Hawkley, LC, Crawford, LE, Ernst, JM, Burleson, MH, Kowalewski, RB, Malarkey, WB, Van Cauter, E and Berntson, GG (2002) Loneliness and health: potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine 64, 407417.10.1097/00006842-200205000-00005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT, Hawkley, LC and Thisted, RA (2010) Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and Aging 25, 453463.10.1037/a0017216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collaborators, G and Ärnlöv, J (2020) Global age-sex-specific fertility, mortality, healthy life expectancy (HALE), and population estimates in 204 countries and territories, 1950–2019: a comprehensive demographic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet 396, 11601203.Google Scholar
Crimmins, EM, Kim, JK, Langa, KM and Weir, DR (2011) Assessment of cognition using surveys and neuropsychological assessment: the health and retirement study and the aging, demographics, and memory study. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 66(Suppl 1): i16271.10.1093/geronb/gbr048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cudjoe, TKM, Roth, DL, Szanton, SL, Wolff, JL, Boyd, CM and Thorpe, RJ (2020) The epidemiology of social isolation: national health and aging trends study. The Journals of Gerontology Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 75, 107113.10.1093/geronb/gby037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Das, P and Das, S (2024) Crises and Challenges in Midlife. In Das, S, and Chakraborty, N (eds), Midlife. London: Routledge India, p. .10.4324/9781003568360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GBD US Health Disparities Collaborators (2022) Life expectancy by county, race, and ethnicity in the USA, 2000-19: a systematic analysis of health disparities. Lancet 400, 2538.10.1016/S0140-6736(22)00876-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greer, SL, Lynch, J, Reeves, A, Falkenbach, M, Gingrich, J, Cylus, J and Bambra, C (2021) Ageing and Health : the politics of better policies. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108973236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkley, LC (2022) Loneliness and health. Nature Reviews Disease Primers 8, .10.1038/s41572-022-00355-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2010) Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 40, 218227.10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkley, LC, Thisted, RA and Cacioppo, JT (2009) Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychology 28, 354363.10.1037/a0014400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heeringa, SG and Connor, JH (1995) Technical description of the health and retirement study sample design. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Available: https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/195622/HRSSAMP.pdf?sequence=1.10.7826/ISR-UM.06.585031.001.05.0001.1995CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henriksen, J, Larsen, ER, Mattisson, C and Andersson, NW (2019) Loneliness, health and mortality. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 28, 234239.10.1017/S2045796017000580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitchcott, PK, Fastame, MC and Penna, MP (2018) More to Blue Zones than long life: positive psychological characteristics. Health, Risk & Society 20, 163181.10.1080/13698575.2018.1496233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoang, P, King, JA, Moore, S, Moore, K, Reich, K, Sidhu, H, Tan, CV, Whaley, C and Mcmillan, J (2022) Interventions associated with reduced loneliness and social isolation in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Network Open 5, .10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.36676CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J, Robles, TF and Sbarra, DA (2017) Advancing social connection as a public health priority in the United States. American Psychologist 72, 517530.10.1037/amp0000103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J, Smith, TB, Baker, M, Harris, T and Stephenson, D (2015) Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science 10, 227237.10.1177/1745691614568352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, QM, Zhang, PD, Shen, D, Gao, J, Li, ZH, Lv, YB, Shi, XM and Mao, C (2024) Analysis of changes in social isolation, loneliness, or both, and subsequent cognitive function among older adults: findings from a nationwide cohort study. Alzheimer’s and Dementia 20, 56745683.10.1002/alz.14080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, ME, Waite, LJ, Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2004) A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging 26, 655672.10.1177/0164027504268574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, CD, Stanton, BA, Niemcryk, SJ and Rose, RM (1988) A scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 41, 313321.10.1016/0895-4356(88)90138-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kato, TA, Kanba, S and Teo, AR (2019) Hikikomori: multidimensional understanding, assessment, and future international perspectives. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 73, 427440.10.1111/pcn.12895CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kato, TA, Tateno, M, Shinfuku, N, Fujisawa, D, TEO, AR, Sartorius, N, Akiyama, T, Ishida, T, Choi, TY, Balhara, YP, Matsumoto, R, Umene-Nakano, W, Fujimura, Y, Wand, A, Chang, JP, Chang, RY, Shadloo, B, Ahmed, HU, Lerthattasilp, T and Kanba, S (2012) Does the ‘hikikomori’ syndrome of social withdrawal exist outside Japan? A preliminary international investigation. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology 47, 10611075.10.1007/s00127-011-0411-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kezios, KL, Lu, P, Calonico, S, Hazzouri, AL and Al Hazzouri, AZ (2023) History of low hourly wage and all-cause mortality among middle-aged workers. JAMA 329, 561573.10.1001/jama.2023.0367CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korn, EL, Graubard, BI and Midthune, D (1997) Time-to-event analysis of longitudinal follow-up of a survey: choice of the time-scale. American Journal of Epidemiology 145, 7280.10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreouzi, M, Theodorakis, N and Constantinou, C (2024) Lessons learned from blue zones, lifestyle medicine pillars and beyond: an update on the contributions of behavior and genetics to wellbeing and longevity. American Journal of Lifestyle Medicine 18, 750765.10.1177/15598276221118494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J and Cagle, JG (2017) Validating the 11-item revised university of California Los Angeles scale to assess loneliness among older adults: an evaluation of factor structure and other measurement properties. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 25, 11731183.10.1016/j.jagp.2017.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luo, Y, Hawkley, LC, Waite, LJ and Cacioppo, JT (2012) Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Social Science & Medicine 74, 907914.10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lydersen, S (2019) Last observation carried forward. Tidsskrift for den Norske Laegeforening 139. doi:10.4045/tidsskr.19.0061.Google ScholarPubMed
Malani, P, Singer, D, Kirch, M, Solway, E, Roberts, S, Smith, E, Hutchens, L and Kullgren, J (2023) Trends in Loneliness among Older Adults from 2018–2023. University of Michigan National Poll on Healthy Aging. doi:10.7302/7011.Google Scholar
Mann, F, Wang, J, Pearce, E, Ma, R, Schlief, M, Lloyd-Evans, B, Ikhtabi, S and Johnson, S (2022) Loneliness and the onset of new mental health problems in the general population. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology 57, 21612178.10.1007/s00127-022-02261-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mojtabai, R and Olfson, M (2004) Major depression in community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults: prevalence and 2- and 4-year follow-up symptoms. Psychological Medicine 34, 623634.10.1017/S0033291703001764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motillon-Toudic, C, Walter, M, Séguin, M, Carrier, JD, Berrouiguet, S and Lemey, C (2022) Social isolation and suicide risk: literature review and perspectives. European Psychiatry 65, .10.1192/j.eurpsy.2022.2320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, SL, Kochanek, KD, Xu, J and Arias, E (2021) Mortality in the United States, 2020. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health StatisticsGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakai, M, Chen, D-G, Nishimura, K and Miyamoto, Y (2014) Comparative study of four methods in missing value imputations under missing completely at random mechanism. Open Journal of Statistics 4(1): 2737.10.4236/ojs.2014.41004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine; Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, Health and Medicine Division; Board on Behavioral, Cognitive, and Sensory Sciences; Board on Health Sciences Policy & Committee on the Health and Medical Dimensions of Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older Adults (2020) Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older Adults: Opportunities for the Health Care System. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US).Google Scholar
National Research Council (2001) Preparing for an Aging World: The Case for Cross-National Research. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Office of the Surgeon General (2023) Publications and Reports of the Surgeon General. Our Epidemic of Loneliness and Isolation: The U.S. Surgeon General’s Advisory on the Healing Effects of Social Connection and Community. Washington (DC): US Department of Health and Human Services. PMID: 37792968.Google Scholar
Patil, U and Braun, KL (2024) Interventions for loneliness in older adults: a systematic review of reviews. Frontiers in Public Health 12, .10.3389/fpubh.2024.1427605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, E, Bu, F and Fancourt, D (2021) Loneliness and risk for cardiovascular disease: mechanisms and future directions. Current Cardiology Reports 23, .10.1007/s11886-021-01495-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perissinotto, CM, Stijacic Cenzer, I and Covinsky, KE (2012) Loneliness in older persons: a predictor of functional decline and death. Archives of Internal Medicine 172, 10781083.10.1001/archinternmed.2012.1993CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qi, X, Malone, SK, Pei, Y, Zhu, Z and Wu, B (2023) Associations of social isolation and loneliness with the onset of insomnia symptoms among middle-aged and older adults in the United States: a population-based cohort study. Psychiatry Research 325, .10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raymo, JM and Wang, J (2022) Loneliness at older ages in the United States: lonely life expectancy and the role of loneliness in health disparities. Demography 59, 921947.10.1215/00703370-9937606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Reynolds 3rd CF, Jeste, DV, Sachdev, PS and Blazer, DG (2022) Mental health care for older adults: recent advances and new directions in clinical practice and research. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA) 21, 336363.Google Scholar
Rico-Uribe, LA, Caballero, FF, Martín-María, N, Cabello, M, Ayuso-Mateos, JL and Miret, M (2018) Association of loneliness with all-cause mortality: a meta-analysis. PLoS One 13, .10.1371/journal.pone.0190033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, DW (1996) UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment 66, 2040.10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J, Ryan, L, Fisher, G and , Sonnega, A and Weir, D (2017) Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire 2006-2016. Available: https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/sites/default/files/biblio/HRS%202006-2016%20SAQ%20Documentation_07.06.17_0.Google Scholar
Sonnega, A, Faul, JD, Ofstedal, MB, Langa, KM, Phillips, JW and Weir, DR (2014) Cohort profile: the health and retirement study (HRS). International Journal of Epidemiology 43, 576585.10.1093/ije/dyu067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steptoe, A, Shankar, A, Demakakos, P and Wardle, J (2013) Social isolation, loneliness, and all-cause mortality in older men and women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, 57975801.10.1073/pnas.1219686110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stessman, J, Rottenberg, Y, Shimshilashvili, I, Ein-Mor, E and Jacobs, JM (2013) Loneliness, Health, and Longevity. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A 69, 744750.Google ScholarPubMed
Surkalim, DL, Luo, M, Eres, R, Gebel, K, Van Buskirk, J, Bauman, A and Ding, D (2022) The prevalence of loneliness across 113 countries: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 376, .Google ScholarPubMed
Van de Velde, S, Levecque, K and Bracke, P (2009) Measurement equivalence of the CES-D 8 in the general population in Belgium: a gender perspective. Archives of Public Health 67, .10.1186/0778-7367-67-1-15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaupel, JW, Zhang, Z and Van Raalte, AA (2011) Life expectancy and disparity: an international comparison of life table data. BMJ Open 1, .10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Känel, R, Meister-Langraf, RE, Zuccarella-Hackl, C, Schiebler, SLF, Znoj, H, Pazhenkottil, AP, Schmid, JP, Barth, J, Schnyder, U and Princip, M (2022) Sleep disturbance after acute coronary syndrome: a longitudinal study over 12 months. PLoS One 17, .10.1371/journal.pone.0269545CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, F, Gao, Y, Han, Z, Yu, Y, Long, Z, Jiang, X, Wu, Y, Pei, B, Cao, Y, Ye, J, Wang, M and Zhao, Y (2023) A systematic review and meta-analysis of 90 cohort studies of social isolation, loneliness and mortality. Nature Human Behaviour 7, 13071319.10.1038/s41562-023-01617-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H, Leng, Y, Zhao, E, Fleming, J and Brayne, C (2020) Mortality risk of loneliness in the oldest old over a 10-year follow-up. Aging and Mental Health 24, 3540.10.1080/13607863.2018.1510897CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weir, D (2016) Validating mortality ascertainment in the health and retirement study. Ann Arbor Michigan: University of Michigan.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. 2023. Mental health of older adults [Online]. Available: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-health-of-older-adults (accessed 25 January 2025 ).Google Scholar
Zhao, J, Reese, JB, Han, X and Yabroff, KR (2024) Loneliness and mortality risk among cancer survivors in the United States: a retrospective, longitudinal study. Journal of the National Comprehensive Cancer Network 22, 244248.10.6004/jnccn.2023.7114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, BL, Chen, SL and Conwell, Y (2016) Effects of transient versus chronic loneliness on cognitive function in older adults: findings from the Chinese longitudinal healthy longevity survey. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 24, 389398.10.1016/j.jagp.2015.12.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, BL, Chen, SL, Tu, X and Conwell, Y (2017) Loneliness and cognitive function in older adults: findings from the Chinese longitudinal healthy longevity survey. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 72, 120128.10.1093/geronb/gbw037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of participants by severity of loneliness (N = 6,392)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Kaplan–Meier survival curve: (a) Kaplan–Meier survival curve by loneliness score and (b) Kaplan–Meier survival curve by age group.

Figure 2

Table 2. The association between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years (N = 6,392)

Figure 3

Table 3. Associations between loneliness and mortality before the age of 85 years by different age groups (N = 6,392)

Figure 4

Figure 2. Adjusted hazard ratios of mortality rates according to loneliness symptom scores.

Graphs show hazard ratios (HRs) according to loneliness symptom scores, adjusted for age, sex, marital status, educational level, living with others, alcohol consumption, insomnia symptoms, depressive symptoms, total TICS-m scores, diabetes, cancer, heart conditions and stroke. Data were analysed using a three-knotted restricted cubic spline Cox proportional hazards regression model. The total UCLA-11 score ranges from 11 to 33, with higher scores indicating more severe loneliness symptoms. Solid lines represent HRs, and shaded areas indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Supplementary material: File

Fan et al. supplementary material

Fan et al. supplementary material
Download Fan et al. supplementary material(File)
File 38.1 KB