Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-nx7b4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-03T12:11:27.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Campaigns do matter: The impact of campaigns in reducing framing effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2025

Derek Beach*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Jannik Fenger
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Roman Senninger
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
*
Corresponding author: Derek Beach; Email: derek@ps.au.dk

Abstract

This study examines the role of political campaigns in reducing voter susceptibility to framing effects. The case chosen is the 2022 EU defence opt-out referendum in Denmark. Using a novel survey experiment design that exposed voters to arguments at the start and the end of the campaign, we were able to directly test the impact of a real-world campaign on voter susceptibility to frames. The findings reveal a notable reduction in framing effects. Initially, loss-related framed arguments swayed voters, but this impact waned at the campaign’s end. Our analysis suggests that the campaign provided voters with information and arguments that made them less susceptible to framing effects and instead empowered voters to make informed decisions on the referendum based on their own attitudes towards the EU. In the conclusions, we encourage further research on susceptibility to elite messages in real-world settings.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Consortium for Political Research

Introduction

Are voters susceptible to elite arguments when making choices in referendums? Or are voters competent to make choices based on their underlying attitudes towards an issue? In contrast to elections, referendums are political settings in which the propositions voters face are often complex and novel topics (LeDuc Reference LeDuc2002; Hobolt Reference Hobolt2006, Reference Hobolt2009). When the attitudes of voters towards an issue overlap with those of political elites, elite cues can provide voters with heuristic shortcuts that enable them to choose the option that best matches their underlying attitudes (Bowler and Donovan Reference Bowler and Donovan1998; Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009). More common in a referendum is the situation where a proposition involves issues that do not map neatly onto pre-existing political cleavages. Here voters lack the partisan signposts that they can rely on in elections to make choices, making them in theory more susceptible to how arguments are framed instead of making choices based on underlying attitudes (de Vreese Reference de Vreese and de Vreese2007; Marsh Reference Marsh and de Vreese2007; Schuck and de Vreese Reference Schuck and de Vreese2008; Neijens and de Vreese Reference Neijens and de Vreese2009; Boudreau and MacKenzie Reference Boudreau and MacKenzie2014; Bechtel et al. Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Helbling2015). Framing effects are when an individual exposed to different considerations on an issue takes a different position than they otherwise would have based on their underlying issue attitudes (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; Scheufele and Iyengar Reference Scheufele, Iyengar, Kensi and Jamieson2012; Leeper and Slothuus Reference Leeper and Slothuus2014, Reference Leeper and Slothuus2015; Liu and Scheufele Reference Liu and Scheufele2016).

Voter susceptibility to framing effects can in theory be reduced during the course of a referendum campaign through both voter exposure to information about the proposition and to different arguments that are intended to persuade them to vote in particular ways (LeDuc Reference LeDuc2002; Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009; Bechtel et al. Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Helbling2015; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020; Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021).Footnote 1 During campaigns, voters receive information that enables them to learn enough about the proposition to map it onto their underlying attitudes. Towards the end of the campaign, many voters will have made up their minds, making them less susceptible to frames (Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006; Hill Reference Hill2017). Additionally, repeated exposure to arguments can in effect ‘inoculate’ voters to their effects if the voter has time to reflect on how the argument maps onto their underlying attitudes (Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Peterson and Slothuus2013; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020). Arguments that do not map onto their underlying attitudes will be rejected or accepted if they correspond to attitudes.

However, despite the large literature on framing effects and voting behaviour in referendums, there have been few studies that directly test the impact that exposure to information and arguments can have on voter susceptibility. There are even fewer studies that utilise real-world campaigns as a context. Most studies of framing have deployed survey experiments outside of the context of an actual referendum (see online Appendix 2 for a review of relevant studies). Using a real-world campaign to test framing effects enables us to assess susceptibility in a context that actually matters for voters. Yet the few studies that have been conducted while a campaign was taking place have typically been one-off studies either at the start or the end of a campaign (eg Bechtel et al. Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Helbling2015; Masullo and Morisi Reference Masullo and Morisi2019; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020). While the one-off studies are able to assess the impact of differing levels of information or knowledge at the individual level, they are – by design – unable to test more directly whether the arguments and information provided by a campaign reduce voter susceptibility to framing effects. To do so requires comparing the magnitude of framing effects at the start and the end of a campaign.Footnote 2

This article utilises a unique experimental design that deploys the same framing survey experiment among independent but similar samples of voters at two different stages of a real-world campaign. This enables us to assess the impact of political campaigns on susceptibility to framing effects directly. The first experiment was conducted close to the start of the campaign, while the second experiment was deployed during the final weeks of the campaign. We thereby combine the experimental control of exposure to frames with natural variation in the information environment during the campaign to directly assess the impact of political campaigns on voters’ susceptibility to framing effects.

We utilised a campaign in connection with an EU referendum as a particularly relevant context for assessing voter susceptibility. EU referendums on membership (entry or exit), or in our case, the deepening of integration (eg adoption of new EU treaties, removing opt-outs), are issues where most voters feel that much is at stake. This gives voters motives to gather enough information and process arguments during the campaign to enable them to engage in what is termed ‘issue-voting’, where voters make choices on the proposition based on their underlying EU attitudes (Svensson Reference Svensson2002; Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009; Garry Reference Garry2013; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020; Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021). At the same time, while voters in many EU member states have stable and relatively strongly held underlying attitudes towards the EU,Footnote 3 the propositions sent to approval by referendum are typically both highly complex and novel topics.Footnote 4 Additionally, voters cannot necessarily rely on shortcuts such as partisan cues in many EU member states because many voters have different attitudes towards the EU than their elected representatives (Hobolt and de Vries Reference Hobolt and de Vries2016; Hobolt and Rodon Reference Hobolt and Rodon2020).

The EU referendum we chose was the 2022 EU defence opt-out referendum in Denmark. Shortly after the Russian invasion of Ukraine in February 2022, a majority of Danish parties decided to send the Danish defence opt-out to a referendum. The Danish context is one where voters have relatively stable underlying attitudes towards more or less EU cooperation, meaning that they possess predispositions towards the issue that can guide them when deciding how to vote on the proposition. At the start of the campaign, while a majority of Danish voters believed that the proposition was important for Denmark,Footnote 5 the average voter had little information and prior exposure to arguments for/against removing the opt-out. Given this, we would expect that voters are susceptible to how elite arguments are framed at the start of the campaign. However, by the end of the campaign the average voter could in theory have received enough information and arguments to make them less susceptible to framing effects. In order to test the robustness of our findings, we assessed whether there might be heterogeneous effects based on type of argument frame (losses versus gains) and by the education level of voters.

Our main finding is that the referendum campaign did indeed reduce voter susceptibility to framing effects. In the first experiment deployed in the early campaign stage, we find that voters exposed to loss frames related to the negative consequences of either voting yes or no were more likely to state they would vote no (or yes) than the control group. Interestingly, arguments emphasising positive consequences (gains) had little impact, irrespective of whether they were for the yes or no side. When we repeated the same experiment in the late campaign stage among an independent yet similar sample, we found that voters were not susceptible to framing effects, irrespective of whether the arguments focused on positive or negative consequences. This suggests that the change in the information environment and exposure to arguments during the campaign reduced voter susceptibility to framing effects. Strengthening this conclusion is the finding of countereffects towards one of the frames amongst more educated voters at the end of the campaign. The no-loss frame that we deployed had been widely discredited in the media during the campaign. When we exposed more educated voters to this discredited no-frame at the end of the campaign, exposure made them more likely to say they would vote yes in relation to the control group. Our interpretation of this is that they reacted by in effect saying ‘if that bad [discredited] argument is the best the no-side can muster, I should vote yes’.

The article proceeds in the following steps. In section 2, we review the existing literature on framing effects. Based on this, we develop hypotheses about the impact that exposure to information and arguments during a referendum campaign can have in relation to framing effects. Section 3 describes the two wave survey experimental design that we deploy, along with the formulation of the four different frames. Section 4 describes the context of the 2022 Danish defence opt-out referendum and provides additional evidence that demonstrates that the campaign provided enough information and exposure to arguments that it could in theory have enabled voters to take positions that reflect their underlying attitudes instead of being susceptible to framing effects. Section 5 presents the findings of the survey experiments. Section 6 discusses potential alternative explanations and the broader implications of the findings.

Theory

In this article, we assess the impact of a real-world referendum campaign on voter susceptibility to framing effects. A framing effect occurs when a voter who is exposed to an argument or information shifts how they say they would vote on a proposition, other things equal (Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012: 876; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020:484). Frames can work through either making new information available to individuals or by changing the accessibility of information, changing the considerations that voters use when reasoning about how the proposition maps onto their underlying issue attitudes (Leeper and Slothuus Reference Leeper and Slothuus2015). In contrast, issue-voting occurs when voters take positions based on their underlying attitudes to an issue to make an informed choice about a proposition in a referendum (Svensson Reference Svensson2002; Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009). Note that we distinguish here between a framing effect that brings different considerations to mind for voters when reasoning about an issue, and deeper persuasion effects in which the underlying attitudes of individuals towards an issue itself are changed (Nelson and Oxley Reference Nelson and Oxley1999: 1040–1041).Footnote 6 The proposition facing Danish voters in the EU referendum on security affairs was one in which they initially had very little information or prior exposure to arguments, making them in theory susceptible to framing effects. In contrast, by the end of the campaign, given the stakes involved and the amount of information and debate during the campaign, we would expect that voters would be better able to engage in issue-voting.

In the general literature on framing effects, there are mixed findings as regards the impact of frames on voters. Some scholars have found relatively strong effects, implying that political elites can shape views towards a proposition to serve their own interests (Lupia and McCubbins Reference Lupia and McCubbins1998; Druckman and Nelson Reference Druckman and Nelson2003; Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; Arceneaux and Nickerson Reference Arceneaux and Nickerson2010; Lecheler and de Vreese Reference Lecheler and de Vreese2011; Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012; Masullo and Morisi Reference Masullo and Morisi2019; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020). Other scholars have found framing effects that are weaker, especially when voters are facing real-world political choices on issues that matter to them (Lecheler, de Vreese and Slothuus Reference Lecheler, de Vreese and Slothuus2009; Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2010; Bechtel et al. Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Helbling2015; Tesler Reference Tesler2015; Ciuk and Yost Reference Ciuk and Yost2015; Hopkins Reference Hopkins2018; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020; Amsalem and Zoizner Reference Amsalem and Zoizner2022).

Referendum campaigns can in theory reduce voter susceptibility to framing effects by exposing voters to information and arguments (Masullo and Morisi Reference Masullo and Morisi2019; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020; Beach et al. Reference Beach, Hansen and Larsen2018; Dvořák Reference Dvořák2013). In the literature, there is disagreement about what it is about campaigns that reduce susceptibility.Footnote 7 Some scholars emphasise the impact of increased information by itself, which decreases voter uncertainty about the proposition and strengthens the ability of voters to counter arguments that contradict their underlying attitudes (Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006: 757; Hill Reference Hill2017). Other scholars suggest that prior exposure to arguments reduces their efficacy (Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012; Slothuus Reference Slothuus2016), in particular when voters face competing arguments that can cancel each other out (Druckman and Nelson Reference Druckman and Nelson2003; Sniderman and Theriault Reference Sniderman, Theriault, Saris and Sniderman2004; Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2010; Druckman et al. Reference Druckman, Peterson and Slothuus2013; Kalla and Broockman Reference Kalla and Broockman2018; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020). Goodwin et al. (Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020) found in an EU referendum in the United Kingdom that voters had been exposed to negative arguments about the EU for years, whereas positive arguments about the benefits of the EU had not been widely made until the referendum started. They found framing effects when voters were exposed to the novel positive arguments and limited effects of negative arguments (Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020). However, given that they deployed a one-off study before the start of the campaign itself,Footnote 8 they were unable to assess the impact of information and arguments provided by the campaign itself.

Taken as a whole, the literature suggests that we can expect voters are less susceptible to frames at the end of the campaign. This leads to the first hypothesis:

H1 – framing effects at the beginning of the campaign can be expected to be stronger than at the end of the campaign.

In order to explore whether there are heterogeneous effects of frames in our experimental design, we develop two additional hypotheses related to whether voters are only susceptible to particular types of frames, and whether there are individual-level differences related to levels of political knowledge that might affect voter susceptibility to frames.

Hypothesis 2 relates to whether the type of frame can make a difference. Frames that emphasise gains or losses present different information to voters, thereby potentially altering the weight they attach to different aspects of an issue (Nelson, Oxley, and Clawson Reference Nelson, Oxley and Clawson1997; Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; Scheufele and Iyengar Reference Scheufele, Iyengar, Kensi and Jamieson2012; Leeper and Slothuus Reference Leeper and Slothuus2015; Liu and Scheufele Reference Liu and Scheufele2016).Footnote 9 In relation to a referendum, a gain frame would emphasise the positive benefits of either voting yes or no, whereas a loss frame would highlight the negative effects of either yes or no. There is a large literature that contends that loss frames can potentially have a larger impact, as most people place more weight on information about negative outcomes than positive ones (Druckman Reference Druckman2004; De Vreese et al. Reference de Vreese, Boomgaarden and Semetko2011; Soroka Reference Soroka2014). Given this, we expect that arguments that emphasise losses will have stronger effects than gain frames. For both the pro/Yes and anti/No arguments, we develop gain- and loss-related arguments to assess whether there are differences in effect size depending on the nature and direction of the argument.

This leads to our second hypothesis:

H2loss frames can be expected to have stronger effects than gain frames, irrespective of whether the argument is for or against the proposition.

Hypothesis 3 assesses whether the level of political knowledge of individual voters affects their susceptibility to framing effects. There is a large literature that has found that less knowledgeable voters can be more susceptible to frames for a variety of reasons. One reason is that there is greater likelihood that a frame exposes a less knowledgeable voter to new information, thereby changing how they perceive a proposition (Zaller Reference Zaller1992; Lupia Reference Lupia1994). Another reason can be that less knowledgeable voters lack the informational resources to evaluate when an argument runs counter to their underlying attitudes (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; Bechtel et al. Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Helbling2015: 687; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020).

Taken together, this leads to our third hypothesis:

H3 – framing effects can be expected to be stronger for less knowledgeable voters, other things equal.

Data and design

The three hypotheses are tested using a novel survey experimental design deployed in a real-world political campaign setting. While most experiments on framing effects have a ‘captive’ audience that is exposed to a fixed information set (Chong Reference Chong2019: 13), the real-world setting enables us to assess how the exposure to information and arguments in a campaign potentially affects susceptibility to framing effects. Given that we do not utilise a panel design to assess our two hypotheses, our study is not vulnerable to panel mortality or panel conditioning effects.

In our design, we deployed two identical survey experimental vignette treatments to a sample of voters early in the campaign (five to six weeks prior to the vote),Footnote 10 and a separate but similar sample of voters in the last week before the vote.Footnote 11 The reason for this design choice – in contrast to panel data that would allow to examine within-subject opinion changes – is that we are not in itself interested in whether individual respondents’ opinions changes over time but rather the extent to which the framing effects changes over time during the campaign. This can more credibly be examined with two similar but independent samples, whereas a panel design including re-exposure to treatment would potentially confound general campaign effects with pre-treatment effects.

Sample

Our survey respondents are drawn from the online panel of Epinion, a Danish polling firm, using stratified sampling. The samples are representative for the Danish population regarding age, gender, education, and geography (ie region). Comparisons with the Danish Election Study 2022, based on a high-quality probability sample, confirm that our sample closely aligns with the broader population in terms of age, gender, and geography (see Table SI 5).

The first survey experiment consists of 3295 respondents and the second survey experiment has 3254 respondents. The panels in Figure SI 6 (online Appendix) demonstrate a high degree of similarity between the respondents in the two samples with respect to age, gender, education, and geography. To formally assess the similarity between the two experimental samples, we conducted t-tests and chi-square tests on key background variables (see Table SI 4). The results show a high degree of similarity between the samples in terms of age, interest, trust, EU position, education, and geography. Specifically, the mean values for age, interest, trust, and EU position are nearly identical across both samples, and the distributions of education, region, and party preference are consistent. However, the analysis of gender reveals a slight difference in distribution between the two samples, with the proportion of men being 0.49 in the first experiment and 0.47 in the second.

Randomisation and Experimental Conditions

We used simple randomisation that assigns all subjects to one of four treatment groups and one control group with an equal probability. We did not specify exactly how many individuals are assigned to each condition. The number of observations for individual groups ranges from 615 to 677 in the first survey experiment and 590 to 691 in the second survey experiment. Balance tests on the variables age, gender, education, and region show that respondents are very similar across the five conditions (see Figure SI 45). Only one treatment group in the first survey experiment shows a statistically significant difference regarding age when compared to the control group (see Table SI 23). While we do not include any control variables in our main analysis, we also estimate treatment effects using a specification that includes covariates and show the results in the Appendix. The inclusion of control variables accounts for random differences across groups and might increase the precision of our estimates because variables that strongly predict the outcome help explain more of the variation, leaving less unexplained variability. As the study was embedded in a novel, real-world context, we attempted to ensure that all respondents were aware of the context of the referendum. Therefore, after answering questions on their socio-economic background and general view about the EU, all respondents read the following text: Russia’s invasion of Ukraine has changed the global situation. In light of the new security policy situation that Europe is currently facing, Danish citizens must now decide whether to maintain or abolish the Danish defence opt-out.

Respondents in the control group did not receive any additional information and proceeded directly to the outcome question. Respondents assigned to one of the four treatment groups were asked to carefully read a short text in connection with the upcoming referendum. The treatment frames are shown in Table 1 (translated from Danish). Given that the primary dimension of voter attitudes towards the EU are on a pro/anti-axis (Hobolt and de Vries Reference Hobolt and de Vries2016; Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021), and given that we had no reason to expect a particular direction of argument to be more impactful, we tested both pro (yes) and anti (no) arguments at the start and the end to assess whether similar dynamics are at play in different types of arguments. In contrast to the UK Brexit referendum, neither pro- nor anti-argumentive frames were widely available in the issue of the Danish defence opt-out, as the issue had not been widely debated prior to the referendum campaign as the referendum was announced quite suddenly in the wake of Russia’s invasion of Ukraine.Footnote 12

Table 1. Treatment frames

As H2 explores whether there are different effects depending on the type of frame (gain/loss), we developed four argument frames in total: two arguments in favour of the referendum proposal and two arguments opposed. When developing the two anti-arguments, given that voters in referendums are asked to choose between enacting the proposal or the reversion point if the proposal is rejected (Hobolt Reference Hobolt2006, Reference Hobolt2009), we developed an argument that discussed the negative implications of enacting the proposal (No-loss) and another one about the positive implications of the reversion point (No-gain). The original treatment frames in Danish are shown in Table SI 1 in Appendix 1.To ensure a high degree of experimental realism, we were inspired by the arguments made by both the Yes and No sides in the nascent stage of the campaign from late March to early April 2022, and we drew on the research teams’ expert knowledge of what types of arguments were typically made in past EU referendums in Denmark.Footnote 13

In the next section, we show that political actors actually used the arguments presented in the treatment vignettes in media debates.

Operationalisation and Analytical Strategy

The main outcome is a binary response variable indicating whether the respondent intends to vote ‘yes’ (measure takes the value of 1) or ‘no’ (measure takes the value of 0) in the referendum, using the actual question that was asked of voters in the referendum. The question reads: ‘Do you vote yes or no to Denmark’s participation in the European cooperation on security and defence by abolishing the EU defence opt-out?’ In addition to stating ‘yes’ and ‘no’, respondents could also indicate that they ‘will not vote’ or ‘don’t know’. In our main analysis, we coded these two responses as ‘no’, as both reflect an absence of support for the proposal at the time of the survey. This coding approach allows us to retain more respondents in the analysis and reflects a conservative estimate of support. To assess the robustness of this choice, we also report analyses in the Appendix in which we exclude these two categories entirely. We found no substantive difference in the findings. To analyse our data, we regress our binary outcome variable on the randomly assigned treatment variable, with the control condition as the reference category. In the first experiment, the mean of the binary outcome vote ‘yes’ is 0.45 (SD = 0.50), while in the second experiment, the mean is 0.48 (SD = 0.50). To test our third hypothesis, we had to operationalise respondents’ level of political knowledge. Following Goodwin et al. (Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020: 485–486), we use education as a proxy for political knowledge. Ideally, we would have relied on three political knowledge questions that we included based on our expectations of the campaign’s factual focus. However, two of these questions proved to be overly technical and obscure, with correct answers largely limited to experts in EU defence policy. We expect that most correct responses to these items resulted from random guessing rather than actual knowledge. We believe this is reasonable as the theory behind the hypothesis is that more knowledgeable voters are more competent to use information when reasoning about how their underlying attitudes map onto the proposition under consideration. As an additional check, we also examined whether level of political interest moderates susceptibility to framing effects, finding similar patterns.

Manipulation Check

After the measurement of the main outcome variable, we conducted a manipulation check that asked respondents whether the stated argument was for or against abolishing the EU defence opt-out.Footnote 14 This approach represents a factual manipulation check, that is, an objective question about the key element of the experimental treatment. By placing the question immediately after the outcome measure, we follow the best practice in the literature (Kane and Barabas Reference Kane and Barabas2019). In our main analysis, we rely on data from all respondents. In the Appendix, we report additional analyses in which we dropped respondents who failed the factual manipulation check. This exclusion criterion applies to one-quarter of the participants in both experiments.Footnote 15 The findings without the excluded respondents are similar to those with them included.

The two-wave design at the start and the end of the campaign enables us to assess the impact of information and arguments on voters (H1). At the same time, deploying the treatments in real-world conditions means that there are some potential confounders that we cannot control for. One potential confounder is that voters might have been more informed of the negative consequences of Russia’s invasion of Ukraine for Danish security by the end of the campaign (late May) based on media reports that were not related to the referendum campaign. This can have made voters at the end of the campaign more certain that they would vote yes and therefore less likely to be influenced by argument frames in the treatments. However, there was a large amount of coverage in Danish media in the immediate aftermath of the invasion in February, meaning that these increased threat perceptions more likely formed part of what can be termed the pre-treatment context (Druckman and Leeper Reference Druckman and Leeper2012). Using a simple media analysis with the search word ‘Russia’, there was almost three times as many stories in the period from 22 February (invasion) to the start of the first round of the experiment (22 April) than there were from 23 April until the end of the second round of the experiment (31 May).Footnote 16 Additionally, once media coverage of the referendum campaign itself started, many of the stories related to the Russian invasion of Ukraine formed part of the background for debates.

The 2022 Danish defence opt-out referendum campaign

In the 2022 referendum, Danish voters were asked whether to abolish the country’s opt-out from EU defence policies (Pedersen et al. Reference Pedersen, Beach, Senninger and Fenger2023). Similar attempts to remove other opt-outs – on the euro in 2000 and on justice and home affairs in 2015 – had failed despite broad elite support (Beach Reference Beach, Christiansen, Elklit and Nedergaard2020). As such, approval in 2022 was far from guaranteed. While Danish voters hold relatively stable attitudes towards the EU (Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021), the 2022 vote posed a complex choice, shaped by the post-invasion geopolitical context and technical considerations such as participation in EU military missions and the European Defence Agency. In the wake of the Russian invasion of Ukraine, a new defence agreement was adopted between the Social Democrats, the Liberals, the Conservative People’s Party, the Socialist People’s Party, and the Social Liberal Party on 6 March 2022. The agreement both included a decision to increase defence spending, reduce dependency on Russian oil and gas, but also an ambition to abolish the Danish defence opt-out in a referendum to be convened on 1 June 2022. While not signing the agreement, several opposition parties also endorsed it (Liberal Alliance, the Christian Democrats, and the Independent Greens). The Danish People’s Party, the New Right, and the Red-Green Alliance opposed the abolition of the opt-out and recommended a no-vote.

Political actors advocating a yes vote argued that Denmark needed to stand together with the rest of Europe. Several politicians emphasised that if Denmark keeps its opt-out, it will send the wrong signal to European neighbours in light of an increasingly aggressive and assertive Russia. The main argument put forward by political actors advocating a no vote was that the abolishment of the opt-out could lead to a slippery slope of stronger defence cooperation that might become supranational in the future that could result in Denmark losing the ability to control the deployment of Danish troops. One particular concern was whether Denmark might be forced to take part in a common EU military in the future (an EU army). However, this concern was contested by experts during the campaign (Danmarks Radio 2022), and by the end of the campaign the argument had been widely discredited.

In the following, we present a brief analysis showing that the referendum issue was salient for voters in the weeks prior to the referendum and that voters were exposed to a range of different campaign arguments and information.

Figure 1 shows the coverage of Denmark’s defence opt-out (using the word ‘forsvarforbehold’ [defence opt-out] as the search term) across country-wide Danish newspapers and TV programmes, as tracked by the Infomedia database throughout 2022. There is a clear surge in media interest starting in March with the announcement of the referendum (6 March). There is then a large dip, after which another surge in late May and the beginning of June, around the date of the vote itself (1 June). Before the announcement and after the referendum, the data show very little reporting, illustrating the typical lifecycle of media attention around significant political events.

Figure 1. Media reporting.

Note: The dashed lines represent the start of the first and second survey experiment data collections.

Figure 2 presents the distributions of topics from the headlines of the above newspaper articles. Given that we do not have the rights to use and analyse the main body of the news articles, we use the dates and headlines in our media analysis. To identify topics in headlines, we rely on BERTopic, a topic modelling technique that leverages transformers to create dense clusters representing topic categories.Footnote 17 It converts text into high-dimensional vectors using embeddings, a process that captures the semantic meaning of words and phrases. These embeddings are then used to cluster the text into groups, with each cluster representing a distinct topic. The default model (‘all-MiniLM-L6-v2’) performs very well in English. We translated the Danish newspaper headlines into English using DeepL’s API before we applied topic modelling.Footnote 18

Figure 2. Media content – topic distribution.

Our analysis identifies 48 different topics. The words most associated with these topics show they span a wide range of subjects, including NATO’s defence partnership (topic 6), the Ukraine war’s influence (topic 13), concerns about Russia and Putin (topic 7), the Danish People’s Party and its leader Morten Messerschmidt’s positions (topic 16), consequences of rearmament (topic 11), controversies about the question wording on the ballot paper (topic 32), expert insights on polling (topic 35), and the campaign to win over young voters (topic 8). Figure 2 shows that many of the topics have very similar levels of prominence in our data. Although certain topics were more prominent, it is evident that the public had access to a diverse array of information about the referendum. This variety in coverage likely provided voters with ample opportunity to become well informed during the campaign period.

We draw on data from a separate panel survey, conducted by Epinion using a web-based CAWI design, in which a representative sample of Danish voters (N = 1,249) was interviewed at the beginning of the campaign in April 2022 and re-interviewed immediately after the referendum. The two-wave panel design allows us to track changes in public opinion over the course of the campaign and examine motivations for voting yes or no. The first wave was fielded seven weeks before the vote – prior to the campaign’s informational push across media – and the second wave followed shortly after the referendum. Panel attrition between the two waves was approximately 25%. In the following, we present descriptive and visual evidence comparing the two panel waves to show how respondents’ positions shifted over the course of the campaign. First, as with previous opt-out referendums (Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009; Beach Reference Beach2018, Beach Reference Beach, Christiansen, Elklit and Nedergaard2020; Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021), there is evidence that most voters acquired enough amount of information throughout the campaign, enabling them to make informed choices. In April 2022, 45% of the respondents said they had sufficient information to make an informed choice (ie perceived knowledge), whereas by the vote, 67% believed they had this information (see Figure 3).

Figure 3. Increase in feeling informed throughout the campaign.

Note: Respondents’ perceived knowledge is measured on a 1–5 scale based on their agreement with the statement: ‘I have/had sufficient knowledge to assess what I should vote for in the referendum on the defence opt-out’. Response options range from ‘completely agree’ to ‘completely disagree’.

Figure 4 illustrates the movement of voters during the campaign. Most respondents in the panel survey ended up voting as they stated they intended to vote at the start of the campaign. Undecided voters broke towards voting ‘yes’ in the final weeks of the campaign. In total, 46% of the undecided voters ended up voting ‘Yes’ and 28% voted ‘No’.

Figure 4. Voter movement during the campaign.

Note: Respondents’ vote choice is measured by the question: ‘Did you vote/do you intend to vote “yes” or “no” to the abolition of the Danish opt-out from the EU’s defence policy?’ Response options are: ‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘abstain’, and ‘don’t know’.

To further investigate whether voters’ decisions aligned more closely with their underlying EU attitudes as the campaign progressed, we present Figure 5 based on our panel data from the start and the end of the campaign.

Figure 5. EU position during the campaign.

Note: Respondents’ EU positions are measured on a 1–5 scale, asking whether Denmark has gained more advantages or disadvantages from EU membership. Responses are grouped into ‘More advantages’ (1–2) and ‘More disadvantages’ (4–5).

The figure tracks individual respondents’ EU positions and their reported vote intentions over the course of the campaign. The analysis reveals two key patterns. First, voters’ attitudes towards the EU remained relatively stable throughout the campaign period (see upper panel in Figure 5). Notably, the shifts that did occur moved in both directions with about the same magnitude. Second, as the campaign advanced, EU-positive voters increasingly expressed intentions to vote ‘Yes,’ while EU-negative voters became more likely to indicate a ‘No’ vote (see lower panel in Figure 5). This growing alignment between EU attitudes and vote intentions suggests that voters became more informed over time, allowing them to better connect their pre-existing views with the referendum choice. These findings support the interpretation that the campaign facilitated information processing and helped voters activate their EU positions (ie engage in issue-voting), rather than simply being influenced by elite messaging.

Findings

In this section, we present the results of the analysis of the three hypotheses. We first analyse the extent to which there was a difference in the magnitude of the effects of the four frames at the start and the end of the campaign (H1). This analysis also enables us to assess whether there were heterogeneous effects in terms of frame strength (H2). We then analyse whether less knowledgeable voters were more susceptible to frames both at the start and the end of the campaign (H3).

The first hypothesis predicted that voters would be responsive to frames at the start of the campaign, but that after exposure to information and arguments during the campaign, they would be less susceptible to framing effects. The upper panels in Figure 6 present results using the collapsed treatment categories. While both the Yes and No groups point in the expected direction – suggesting that exposure to either type of frame influenced vote intention – neither effect reaches conventional levels of statistical significance.

Figure 6. Framing effects at the start and the end of the referendum campaign.

Note: First survey experiment at the start of campaign (upper and lower left panels). Second survey experiment at the end of campaign (upper and lower right panels). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

Significant effects of frames first appear when we disaggregate the pro/anti messages according to loss or gain frames. The lower left panel in Figure 6 presents estimates of the effects of all four frames deployed at the start of the campaign. The analysis shows that respondents receiving both loss frames (Yes and No) at the start of the campaign shifted their views towards the proposition as compared to the control group. Both shifts were in the direction expected. Support for abolishing the defence opt-out amongst respondents who received the Yes-Loss frame was 5.7% higher than for the control group (95% CI: 11.2%::0.3%) (Yes-Loss).Footnote 19

In turn, respondents in the treatment group receiving the frame highlighting negative consequences of abolishing the opt-out (No-Loss) showed a lower level of support. The point estimate indicates that support in this group was -4.7% (95% CI: -10.2 %::0.7%) lower as compared to the control group. The changes for voters who received either Yes or No ‘gain’ frames were not significant compared to the control group.

The lower right panel in Figure 6 shows that there were no substantial and statistically significant framing effects at the end of the campaign. The point estimates related to the loss frames were smaller in size compared to the before-campaign experiment. The figures indicate that voters on aggregate were not affected by framing effects at this late stage of the campaign, or, if so, the effects were small.Footnote 20 Strikingly, there are almost no differences in framing effects across the four treatment groups in the second survey experiment. Taken as a whole, the results provide partial confirmation of our hypotheses. While voters were more susceptible to frames at the start than at the end of the campaign, this held only for loss frames.

In order to test whether our results are robust across different specifications, Figure 7 shows that the exclusion of respondents who said that they ‘will not vote’ or ‘don’t know’ does not alter the findings. Instead, this exclusion provides additional evidence indicating that loss frames had a substantial impact on respondents at the beginning of the campaign. However, it also confirms that the framing effect diminished over time as the campaign progressed.

Figure 7. Framing effects – ‘will not vote’ and ‘don’t know’ excluded.

Note: First survey experiment at the start of campaign (left panel). Second survey experiment at the end of campaign (right panel). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

We also test hypotheses one and two more formally through a z-test. The z-value measures the difference between two coefficients (in standard error units) and tells us how many standard errors the difference between the two coefficients is from zero. The p-value measures the strength of evidence against the null hypothesis that there is no difference between the two coefficients. A p-value less than 0.05 (for a two-tailed test) suggests that the evidence against the null hypothesis is strong and that we can reject the null hypothesis in favour of the alternative hypothesis that the two coefficients are different. Table 2 shows that we observe such a difference for the treatment frame No-loss. The frame highlighting the negative consequences of voting for the proposal decreased respondents’ willingness to support the abolishment of the opt-out at the start of the campaign; however, this was no longer the case at the end of the campaign.

Table 2. Z-test results

The third hypothesis dealt with whether there were heterogeneous effects based on levels of knowledge of voters. Were less knowledgeable voters more susceptible to framing effects? Respondents were coded as less knowledgeable if they had attained less than at least a short higher education.Footnote 21 We also ran analyses using political interest instead of education with substantively similar results. Figures 8 and 9 depict the results by education at the start and the end of the campaign.

Figure 8. Framing effects by education at the start of campaign.

Note: Survey experiment for less educated voters (left panel). Survey experiment for more educated voters (right panel). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 9. Framing effects by education at the end of campaign.

Note: Survey experiment for less educated voters (left panel). Survey experiment for more educated voters (right panel). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

As expected, we find that less knowledgeable voters were more susceptible to frames at the start of the campaign when compared with more educated voters, although only for the stronger loss frames. Support for abolishing the defence opt-out amongst respondents who received the Yes-Loss frame was 5.8% higher than for the control group. In turn, respondents in the treatment group receiving the No-loss frame showed a lower level of support, with the point estimate for support reduced by -6.5% compared to the control group.

At the end of the campaign, none of the frames were close to being statistically significant for less educated voters. Interestingly, the No-loss frame was significant for more educated voters – but in the opposite direction of the argument! Support amongst respondents who received the No-loss frame was 11.3% higher than for the control group (with 95% confidence). This appears to be what Chong and Druckman term a ‘countereffect’ (2007: 109-10), which can occur when respondents have the cognitive capacity and enough information to identify implausible arguments. When a respondent is then faced with the implausible argument, this treatment can push them in the opposite direction based on the logic that if that is the ‘best’ argument for a position, then the position itself becomes discredited, making them more likely to support the opposite position.

During the 2022 Danish referendum campaign, the no-side argument that emphasised the likelihood that common EU defence would lead to the (unwanted) creation of a common EU army was widely rejected by experts in the media and debates. As this discredited argument was a prominent part of the No-Loss frame that we used in the experiment, the dismissal of it by experts can during the campaign have contributed to the argument backfiring when we gave it to more educated voters at the end of the campaign, making them more likely to say they will vote yes in comparison to the control group.

Discussion and implications

Overall we found support for the claim that voter susceptibility to framing effects was reduced during the course of the referendum campaign, although only for strong loss-related frames and amongst less knowledgeable voters. At the start of the campaign, voters were found to be susceptible to stronger loss argument frames. By the end of the campaign, none of the argument frames were significant across all of the surveyed voters, demonstrating that even less knowledgeable Danish voters had received enough information and exposure to arguments to make them less susceptible to framing effects. This interpretation is strengthened by the finding of a ‘countereffect’ amongst more knowledgeable voters at the end of the campaign, in which they were able to correctly identify a discredited argument. That these voters became more likely to support the opposition position is not an indication that they became susceptible to a new type of framing effects. Instead, as argued by Chong and Druckman (Reference Chong and Druckman2007), it indicates that exposure to an implausible argument undermines voter assessments of the position itself. One alternative interpretation of our findings could be that voters were only susceptible to frames that were consistent with their underlying attitudes towards the EU. However, we found that voters were susceptible to loss-related frames at the start of the campaign irrespective of argument direction (yes or no), suggesting that framing effects worked independently of underlying attitudes towards the issue.

Our findings show that after being exposed to a campaign, most Danish voters were able to make choices based on their underlying attitudes towards the issue itself, that is, issue-voting. The findings therefore paint a more optimistic picture regarding campaign effects, different from other studies that highlight more pessimistic concerns related to the role of negativity, voter selectivity, and asymmetry in capacities across agents in political campaigns (Nyhuis et al. Reference Nyhuis, Song and Boomgaarden2021; Ramírez-Dueñas and Vinuesa-Tejero Reference Ramírez-Dueñas and Vinuesa-Tejero2021; Prato and Wolton Reference Prato and Wolton2018).

Finding that campaigns reduce voter susceptibility to frames raises an interesting paradox. Political parties and other elites spend considerable time and resources trying to persuade voters during high salience EU referendum campaigns. Why would they do this if our finding holds that elite arguments do not move voters during campaigns? One answer is that when much is at stake, neither side of a referendum battle would want to cede the stage to their opponents. If debates were completely one-sided, we might expect that voters would potentially be more susceptible to elite arguments, as they would lack information and arguments that would enable them to figure out whether their underlying attitudes point in the other direction.

Yet when both sides engage in debate, our findings suggest that they tend to cancel each other out. And in some instances, an argument from the opposing side can be so discredited that it backfires on the campaigners advocating it, as we found with the no-side’s EU army argument. But taken as a whole, when seen from a direct democratic perspective, finding that elite arguments do not work after voters are exposed to information and arguments is a good thing. While they do not persuade voters, the efforts by political parties and other elites are a democratic public good that provide the information and arguments that empower competent voters to make choices that are consistent with their underlying attitudes (Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021: 1501).

Can we expect that the impact of the campaign in reducing voter susceptibility holds in other EU referendums, and referendums on other topics? Given that our more optimistic findings echo those found by other scholars using different designs in other EU referendums (Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020; Beach and Finke Reference Beach and Finke2021) and in other types of referendum (eg Bechtel et al. Reference Bechtel, Hainmueller, Hangartner and Helbling2015 for a Swiss case), this suggests that our findings about campaigns are generalisable. What then are the conditions under which we might expect exposure to information and arguments reduces voter susceptibility to framing effects? Through comparing the context of our study with other studies that find similar patterns, we identify three contextual conditions.

First, for a campaign to reduce susceptibility to framing effects, there has to be a relatively robust stream of communication (Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009). If there is little coverage, or if it is very one-sided, we would not expect voters to be exposed to enough information and arguments to make attitude-consistent choices. Second, voters should feel that the issue is important enough to be motivated to gather and process information and arguments instead of relying on shortcuts such as partisan cues (Svensson Reference Svensson2002; Hobolt Reference Hobolt2009). In both the 2015 and 2022 Danish opt-out referendums for instance, voters in surveys responded that they perceived that the propositions were important. One proxy for importance is turnout. While lower than for national parliamentary elections, turnout in EU referendums tends to be relatively high in countries such as Ireland and Denmark.Footnote 22 Third, voters need to have stable, pre-existing attitudes towards the issue, as well as some degree of knowledge and interest in the issue. This can be due to a history of EU referendums in a country (eg Ireland and Denmark), or when an issue was debated extensively prior to a referendum being convened, as was the case in the British 2016 EU membership referendum (Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020). Fourth, broader systemic factors – such as media environments that ensure balanced information flows, party systems that are not deeply polarised, and campaign regulations that secure relatively equal access for different sides – may also influence the degree to which voters can engage sensibly with campaign content. In contexts such as these, we would expect that voters would be competent to use exposure to campaign information and arguments to become less susceptible to framing effects.

On a final note, our findings also have a broader methodological implication for researchers using survey experiments. The information environment in which an experiment is deployed can affect the findings. If we had only undertaken the initial ‘before campaign’ survey experiment, we would have concluded that framing effects of negative (loss) arguments mattered. However, our findings would have been externally valid only within the pre-campaign context and could not be generalised to the after-campaign context. The importance of the informational environment for what a survey experiment finds is also supported by the findings of Goodwin et al. (Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020), in which they found that at the start of the Brexit referendum campaign, positive arguments mattered because voters had not been exposed to them previously. However, if they had deployed their survey experiment at the end of the Brexit campaign, they would most likely have found that neither positive nor negative arguments mattered. In other words, survey experiment findings should be contextualised based on the information environment in which they were fielded.

Conclusion

Do campaigns matter in reducing voter susceptibility to framing effects? This article used the case of the 2022 Danish defence opt-out referendum to test whether campaigns matter. EU referendums such as the 2022 Danish opt-out vote are a particularly good context in which to assess the impact of exposure to arguments and information during a campaign because potential confounders such as partisanship and prior information on the issue are not present. At the same time, voters in countries such as Denmark have underlying attitudes towards the issue of support/opposition to the EU.

In contrast to existing studies, our design enabled us to test directly the impact that the campaign made for voter susceptibility to framing effects. However, we also acknowledge several limitations with our design. First, given that all the frames contained different information to match the real-world campaign arguments, future work could explore whether more equivalent loss and gain frames matter. While more equivalent arguments might sacrifice a degree of realism, they would be a more precise treatment. Moreover, utilising panel survey data would allow for closer examination of opinion dynamics at the individual level. Additionally, future studies could conduct a more comprehensive media analysis that leverages the wealth of contextual information generated during such campaigns across both online and offline platforms. While our study was limited to analysing media headlines, future research might utilise full articles, social media discourse, and other forms of campaign communication to gain a more nuanced understanding of how political messages are disseminated and resonate with voters. Finally, future studies should collect supplemental information during the experiment that could be used to shed more light on whether the mechanisms behind reduced susceptibility were more related to voters having enough information or due to prior exposure to arguments themselves. For example, respondents at the end of the campaign could be asked about their reasoning through either open-ended response questions or qualitative interviews.

Taken as a whole, the findings suggest however that the 2022 referendum campaign made Danish voters able to make choices based on their underlying EU attitudes instead of being prone to elite arguments. We found that while voters were susceptible to framing effects related to ‘loss’ arguments at the start of the campaign, they were not susceptible at the end of the campaign. This suggests that Danish voters had received enough information and/or were repeatedly exposed to political arguments that enabled them to take positions that reflected their underlying EU attitudes. In other words, the campaign empowered them to actually answer the question they were asked in the referendum.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S147567652510011X.

Data availability statement

Replication material for this study is available at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/AION8Q

Funding statement

The Independent Research Fund Denmark funded the survey instrument for the project ‘DKOPT – DEF – Voter behavior in the Danish defence opt-out referendum’, grant number 2033-00240B. The project was approved on the 11 March 2022.

Footnotes

1 Note that we use the term ‘campaign’ to refer to all communication directed towards voters through different media forms, irrespective of whether the sender is a political actor arguing for or against the proposition, or information from media sources and experts. This is similar to how Chong (Reference Chong2019: 12) defines campaigns.

2 The only before/after comparison in a real-world setting that we identified in a systematic literature review was by Kalla and Broockman (Reference Kalla and Broockman2018). However, this dealt with a political election and not a referendum. The study deployed a set of survey experiments during the last two months of US elections in 2016 to assess susceptibility to direct contact and advertising from campaigns.

3 See Hobolt and de Vries, Reference Hobolt and de Vries2016 for a review of the literature on voter attitudes towards the EU.

4 For example, in the 2015 Danish Justice and Home Affairs (JHA) opt-out referendum, Danish voters were asked to choose between keeping the existing opt-out from all supranational JHA legal acts, or replace it with a new treaty protocol that enabled the Danish parliament to opt-in to individual legal acts.

5 In the first round of our survey at the start of the campaign, 64% of voters stated that they believed the referendum was somewhat or very important (n = 3,295).

6 Given what we know about the stability of underlying attitudes of Danish voters towards the EU during referendum campaigns (eg Beach and Finke, Reference Beach and Finke2021), we do not study potential persuasion effects in this article.

7 Note that information and/or argument that overlaps with underlying attitudes of voters can also reduce voter susceptibility by strengthening pre-existing attitudes (Druckman and Leeper, Reference Druckman and Leeper2012: 877).

8 The survey was conducted between September and November 2015, which was before the official referendum campaign began. See Goodwin et al. (Reference Goodwin, Hix and Pickup2020: 483).

9 Note that gain and loss frames are often associated with another related concept, namely equivalency frames (Druckman, Reference Druckman2004). While the two concepts sometimes overlap, they are far from always similar (Jerit, Reference Jerit2009). Emphasis frames relate specifically to whether information is centred around positive and negative outcomes or consequences per se. Such information on gains and losses will often be different (non-equivalent) as they focus on different policy arguments, goals, or values (Nelson, Oxley, and Clawson, Reference Nelson, Oxley and Clawson1997; Jerit, Reference Jerit2009, 412). In contrast, equivalency frames concern different, but logically equivalent, depictions of a political issue (Tversky and Kahneman, Reference Tversky and Kahneman1981) for example, saying ‘90 percent employment’ rather than ‘10% unemployment’. Given that equivalency frames often do not map onto the arguments actually used in real-world campaign settings that highlight different considerations (Sniderman and Theriault, Reference Sniderman, Theriault, Saris and Sniderman2004), we focus on emphasis frames in this article.

10 Ideally, we would have been able to deploy the first wave in the days immediately after the 6 March 2022 decision to convene a referendum on the opt-out for 1 June. The Danish defence opt-out was not debated outside of a few elite comments prior to the Russian invasion of Ukraine, and the announcement of the referendum was a surprise. Given that there were a variety of practicalities such as securing funding and designing the study, the deployment was delayed until 22 April (five weeks after the decision to convene) and completed by the 29 April. However, the real debate only started in the final month (May 2022), as illustrated in Section 4. One reason for the delayed debate was that all of the political actors were in the same situation as we were. The second wave of our experiment was deployed from 20 to 31 May.

11 The hypotheses and design of the survey experiments are pre-registered. For the OSF registration, see https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/2AN3G. We note that our study did not undergo IRB approval due to time constraints. The IRB at Aarhus University at the time operated on a fixed schedule with infrequent review periods, which made it unfeasible to await a potential approval within the project timeline. Nevertheless, we fully adhered to established research ethics and data protection regulations, including obtaining informed consent, ensuring participant anonymity, and safeguarding all personal data.

12 Despite calls by opposition parties for removing the Danish defence opt-out, the Danish PM Mette Frederiksen ruled out a referendum as late as October 2021 (Altinget, 29 October 2021, https://www.altinget.dk/artikel/mette-frederiksen-danmark-skal-vaere-i-hjertet-af-eu-og-nato).

13 We would also like to acknowledge in particular the assistance of our colleague and expert in Danish opt-outs, associate professor Rasmus Brun Pedersen, in formulating realistic arguments that would plausibly be the most common during the campaign.

14 We opted for this simple manipulation check as it is crucial that respondents register the direction of the argument for framing effects to occur and for us to assess whether the campaign diminishes such effects. Due to the brevity of the survey, we did not include any additional checks (eg a separate attention check) apart from the manipulation check.

15 Specifically 22.6% of the respondents in the treatment groups failed the manipulation check in the first experiment and 21.2% did so in the second experiment. Additionally, comparisons between loss and gain frames within the Yes and No conditions show mostly similar success rates (see Table SI 6).

16 Search using Infomedia using the term ‘Rusland’ [Russia]. From 22 February to 22 April, there were 7,166 national newspaper stories with the keyword Russia, whereas the number fell to 2,594 for the later period (23 April–31 May).

19 We conducted a power analysis prior to the collection of our data assuming roughly 650 respondents per group. Results from the power analysis show that we have 80% power to identify differences in event probabilities (ie voting ‘yes’ in the referendum) of 5.5 percentage points.

20 In the Appendix, we show that the inclusion of covariates does not change the results (see Figure SI 3) and that respondents who passed our manipulation check responded more strongly to framing treatments (see Figure SI 1).

21 As discussed in section 3, we would ideally have used a measure of political knowledge itself. However, the questions that we used in the survey turned out to be very technical and peripheral in the debates.

22 In Denmark, turnout in the JHA opt-out referendum in 2015 was 72%, and in the 2022 defence opt-out it was 66%. In Ireland, turnout in the 2009 Treaty of Nice referendum was 59%.

References

Aarøe, L. (2011). ‘Investigating frame strength: the case of episodic and thematic frames’. Political Communication, 28(2), 207226.10.1080/10584609.2011.568041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amsalem, E. and Zoizner, A. (2022). ‘Real, but Limited: a meta-analytic assessment of framing effects in the political domain’. British Journal of Political Science, 52(1), 221237.10.1017/S0007123420000253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arceneaux, K. and Nickerson, D.W. (2010). ‘Comparing negative and positive campaign messages: evidence from two field experiments’. American Politics Research, 38(1), 5483.10.1177/1532673X09331613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, D. (2018). ‘Referendums in the European Union’. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.503.10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, D. (2020). ‘Referendums in Denmark: influence on politics’, in Christiansen, P.M., Elklit, J., and Nedergaard, P. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Danish Politics. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198833598.013.20.Google Scholar
Beach, D. and Finke, D. (2021). ‘The long shadow of attitudes: differential campaign effects and issue voting in EU referendums’. West European Politics, 44(7), 14821505.10.1080/01402382.2020.1780829CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, D., Hansen, K.M. and Larsen, M.V. (2018). ‘How campaigns enhance European issues voting during European Parliament elections’. Political Science Research and Methods, 6(4), 791808.10.1017/psrm.2017.6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, M., Hainmueller, J. Hangartner, D. and Helbling, M. (2015). ‘Reality bites: the limits of framing effects for salient and contested policy issues’. Political Science Research and Methods, 3(3), 683695.10.1017/psrm.2014.39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boudreau, C. and MacKenzie, S.A. (2014). ‘Informing the electorate? How party cues and policy information affect public opinion about initiatives’. American Journal of Political Science, 58(1), 4862.10.1111/ajps.12054CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowler, S. and Donovan, T. (1998). Demanding Choices: Opinion, Voting and Direct Democracy. University of Michigan Press.10.3998/mpub.15428CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D. (2019). ‘Competitive framing in political decision making’. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics.10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.964CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D. and Druckman, J.N. (2007). ‘Framing theory’. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103126.10.1146/annurev.polisci.10.072805.103054CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D. and Druckman, J.N. (2010). ‘Dynamic public opinion: communication effects over time’. The American Political Science Review, 104(4), 663680.10.1017/S0003055410000493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D, and Mullinix, K.J. (2019). ‘Information and issue constraints on party cues’. American Politics Research, 47(6), 12091238.10.1177/1532673X18803887CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciuk, D.J. and Yost, B.A. (2015). ‘The effects of issue salience, elite influence, and policy content on public opinion’. Political Communication, 33(2), 328345.10.1080/10584609.2015.1017629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danmarks Radio. (2022). Eksperter undsiger Messerschmidt: Skrækscenarie for EU-hær kræver endnu en folkeafstemning (Experts criticize Messerschmidt: Nightmare scenario for EU army requires another referendum). https://www.dr.dk/nyheder/detektor/kan-messerschmidts-forestilling-om-en-eu-haer-lade-sig-goere-nej-siger-eksperter (accessed 4 July 2023).Google Scholar
de Vreese, C.H. (2007). ‘Context, elites, media and public opinion in referendums: why campaigns really matter’, in de Vreese, C.H. (Ed.), The Dynamics of Referendum Campaigns in International Perspective (pp. 120). Palgrave.10.1057/9780230591189_1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vreese, C.H., Boomgaarden, H.G. and Semetko, H.A. (2011). ‘(In)direct framing effects: the effects of news media framing on public support for Turkish membership in the European Union’. Communication Research, 38(2), 179205.10.1177/0093650210384934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, J.N. (2001) ‘On the limits of framing effects: who can frame?Journal of Politics, 63(4), 10411066.10.1111/0022-3816.00100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, J.N. (2004). ‘Political preference formation: competition, deliberation, and the (Ir)relevance of framing effects’. American Political Science Review, 98(November), 671686.10.1017/S0003055404041413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, J.N. and Leeper, T.J. (2012). ‘Learning more from political communication experiments: pretreatment and its effects’. American Journal of Political Science, 56(4), 875896.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2012.00582.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, J.N. and Nelson, K.R. (2003). ‘Framing and deliberation: how citizens’ conversations limit elite influence’. American Journal of Political Science, 47(4), 729745.10.1111/1540-5907.00051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, J.N., Peterson, E. and Slothuus, R. (2013). ‘How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation’. American Political Science Review, 107(1), 5779.10.1017/S0003055412000500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dvořák, T. (2013). ‘Referendum campaigns, framing and uncertainty’. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 23(4), 367386.10.1080/17457289.2013.764531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garry, J. (2013). ‘Direct democracy and regional integration: citizens’ perceptions of treaty implications and the Irish reversal on Lisbon’. European Journal of Political Research, 52 (1), 94118.10.1111/j.1475-6765.2012.02061.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gherghina, S. and Silagadze, N. (2018). ‘And yet it matters: referendum campaigns and vote decision in Eastern Europe’. Contemporary Politics, 25(1), 2946.10.1080/13569775.2018.1543790CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M., Hix, S. and Pickup, M. (2020). ‘For and against Brexit: a survey experiment of the impact of campaign effects on public attitudes toward EU membership’. British Journal of Political Science, 50(2), 481495.10.1017/S0007123417000667CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, S.J. (2017). ‘Learning together slowly: Bayesian learning about political facts’. Journal of Politics, 79(4), 14031418.10.1086/692739CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobolt, S.B. (2006). ‘Direct democracy and European integration’. Journal of European Public Policy, 13(1), 153166.10.1080/13501760500380825CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobolt, S.B. (2009). Europe in Question: Referendums on European Integration. Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199549948.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobolt, S.B. and de Vries, C.E. (2016). ‘Public support for European integration’. Annual Review of Political Science. 19, 413432.10.1146/annurev-polisci-042214-044157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobolt, S.B. and Rodon, T. (2020). ‘Cross-cutting issues and electoral choice. EU issue voting in the aftermath of the Brexit referendum’. Journal of European Public Policy, 27(2), 227245.10.1080/13501763.2019.1701535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, D.J. (2018). ‘The exaggerated life of death panels? The limited but real influence of elite rhetoric in the 2009–2010 health care debate’. Political Behavior, 40(3), 681709.10.1007/s11109-017-9418-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jerit, J. (2009). ‘How predictive appeals affect policy opinions’. American Journal of Political Science, 53(2), 411426.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2009.00378.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalla, J.L. and Broockman, D.E. (2018). ‘The minimal persuasive effects of campaign contact in general elections: evidence from 49 field experiments’. American Political Science Review, 112(1), 148166.10.1017/S0003055417000363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, V.J. and Barabas, J. (2019). ‘No harm in checking: using factual manipulation checks to assess attentiveness in experiments’. American Journal of Political Science, 63,(1), 234249.10.1111/ajps.12396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecheler, S., de Vreese, C. and Slothuus, R. (2009). ‘Issue importance as a moderator of framing effects’. Communication Research, 36(3), 400425.10.1177/0093650209333028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecheler, S. and de Vreese, C.H. (2011). ‘Getting real: the duration of framing effects’. Journal of Communication, 61(5), 959983.10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01580.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDuc, L. (2002). ‘Opinion change and voting behavior in referendums’. European Journal of Political Research, 41, 711732.10.1111/1475-6765.00027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeper, T.J. and Slothuus, R. (2014). ‘Parties and motivated reasoning’. Political Psychology, 35(S1), 129156.10.1111/pops.12164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeper, T.J. andSlothuus, R. (2015). ‘Can citizens be framed? How information more than emphasis changes opinions’, in 1st Gothenburg-Barcelona Workshop on Experimental Political Science, Gothenburg, Sweden, May (pp. 78).Google Scholar
Liu, J. and Scheufele, D. (2016). ‘A revisionist perspective on framing effects’. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. https://oxfordre.com/politics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228637-e-48.Google Scholar
Lupia, A. (1994). ‘Shortcuts versus encylopedias: information and voting behavior in California insurance reform elections’. American Political Science Review, 88(1), 6376.10.2307/2944882CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupia, A. and McCubbins, M.D. (1998). The Democratic Dilemma: Can Citizens Learn What They Need to Know? Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Marsh, M. (2007). ‘Referendum campaigns: changing what people think or changing what they think about?’, in de Vreese, C.H. (Ed.), The Dynamics of Referendum Campaigns in International Perspective (pp. 6383). Palgrave.10.1057/9780230591189_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masullo, J. and Morisi, D. (2019). ‘Risks and opportunities of direct democracy: the effect of information in Colombia’s peace referendum’. Politics and Governance, 7(2), 242267.10.17645/pag.v7i2.1903CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthes, J. and Schemer, C. (2012). ‘Diachronic framing effects in competitive opinion environments’. Political Communication, 29(3), 319339.10.1080/10584609.2012.694985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neijens, P.C. and de Vreese, C. (2009). ‘Helping citizens decide in referendums: the moderating effect of political sophistication on the use of the information and choice questionnaire as a decision aid’. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(3), 521536.10.1093/poq/nfp040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T.E. and Oxley, Z.M. (1999). ‘Issue framing effects and belief importance and opinion’. Journal of Politics, 61(4), 10401067.10.2307/2647553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T.E., Oxley, Z.M. and Clawson, R.A. (1997). ‘Toward a psychology of framing effects’. Political Behavior, 19(September), 221246.10.1023/A:1024834831093CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyhuis, D., Song, H. and Boomgaarden, H. (2021). ‘Disproportionality in media representations of campaign negativity’. Political Science Research and Methods, 9(3), 519531.10.1017/psrm.2020.4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, R.B., Beach, D., Senninger, R. and Fenger, J. (2023). ‘Danish defense opt-out referendum, 1 June 2022’. BLUE, 3(1), 8387.10.3917/e.blue.003.0083CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, E. and Iyengar, S. (2021). ‘Partisan gaps in political information and information- seeking behavior: motivated reasoning or cheerleading?American Journal of Political Science, 65(1), 133147.10.1111/ajps.12535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prato, C. and Wolton, S. (2018). ‘Electoral imbalances and their consequences’. The Journal of Politics, 80(4), 11681182.10.1086/698757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramírez-Dueñas, J.M. and Vinuesa-Tejero, M.L. (2021). ‘How does selective exposure affect partisan polarisation? Media consumption on electoral campaigns’. The Journal of International Communication, 27(2), 258282.10.1080/13216597.2021.1899957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schemer, C., Wirth, W. and Matthes, J. (2012). ‘Value resonance and value framing effects on voting intentions in direct-democratic campaigns’. American Behavioral Scientist, 56(3), 334352.10.1177/0002764211426329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheufele, D.A. and Iyengar, S. (2012). ‘The state of framing research: a call for new directions’, in Kensi, K. and Jamieson, K. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Political Communication Theories (pp. 619632). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schuck, A.R.T. and de Vreese, C. (2006). ‘Between risk and opportunity: news framing and its effects on public support for EU enlargement’. European Journal of Communication, 21(1), 532.10.1177/0267323106060987CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuck, A.R.T. and de Vreese, C.H. (2008). ‘The Dutch no to the EU constitution: assessing the role of EU-skepticism and the campaign’. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties, 18(1), 101128.10.1080/17457280701858656CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slothuus, R. (2016). ‘Assessing the influence of political parties on public opinion: the challenge from pretreatment effects’. Political Communication, 33(2), 302327.10.1080/10584609.2015.1052892CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slothuus, R. and Bisgaard, M. (2021). ‘How political parties shape public opinion in the real world’. American Journal of Political Science, 65(4), 896911.10.1111/ajps.12550CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sniderman, P.M. and Theriault, S.M. (2004). ‘The structure of political argument and the logic of issue framing’, in Saris, W.E. and Sniderman, P.M. (Eds.), Studies in Public Opinion: Attitudes, Nonattitudes, Measurement Error, and Change (pp. 133165). Princeton University Press.10.1515/9780691188386-007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soroka, S.N. (2014). Negativity in Democratic Politics: Causes and Consequences. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781107477971CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svensson, P. (2002). ‘Five Danish referendums on the European Community and European Union: a critical assessment of the Franklin thesis’. European Journal of Political Research, 41(6), 733750.10.1111/1475-6765.00028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taber, C.S. and Lodge, M. (2006). ‘Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs’. American Journal of Political Science, 50(3), 755769.10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesler, M. (2015). ‘Priming predispositions and changing policy positions: an account of when mass opinion is primed or changed’. American Journal of Political Science, 59(4), 806824.10.1111/ajps.12157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1981). ‘The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice’. Science, 211(January), 453458.10.1126/science.7455683CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaller, J. (1992). The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511818691CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Treatment frames

Figure 1

Figure 1. Media reporting.Note: The dashed lines represent the start of the first and second survey experiment data collections.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Media content – topic distribution.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Increase in feeling informed throughout the campaign.Note: Respondents’ perceived knowledge is measured on a 1–5 scale based on their agreement with the statement: ‘I have/had sufficient knowledge to assess what I should vote for in the referendum on the defence opt-out’. Response options range from ‘completely agree’ to ‘completely disagree’.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Voter movement during the campaign.Note: Respondents’ vote choice is measured by the question: ‘Did you vote/do you intend to vote “yes” or “no” to the abolition of the Danish opt-out from the EU’s defence policy?’ Response options are: ‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘abstain’, and ‘don’t know’.

Figure 5

Figure 5. EU position during the campaign.Note: Respondents’ EU positions are measured on a 1–5 scale, asking whether Denmark has gained more advantages or disadvantages from EU membership. Responses are grouped into ‘More advantages’ (1–2) and ‘More disadvantages’ (4–5).

Figure 6

Figure 6. Framing effects at the start and the end of the referendum campaign.Note: First survey experiment at the start of campaign (upper and lower left panels). Second survey experiment at the end of campaign (upper and lower right panels). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 7

Figure 7. Framing effects – ‘will not vote’ and ‘don’t know’ excluded.Note: First survey experiment at the start of campaign (left panel). Second survey experiment at the end of campaign (right panel). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 8

Table 2. Z-test results

Figure 9

Figure 8. Framing effects by education at the start of campaign.Note: Survey experiment for less educated voters (left panel). Survey experiment for more educated voters (right panel). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 10

Figure 9. Framing effects by education at the end of campaign.Note: Survey experiment for less educated voters (left panel). Survey experiment for more educated voters (right panel). Error bars represent 90% and 95% confidence intervals.

Supplementary material: File

Beach et al. supplementary material

Beach et al. supplementary material
Download Beach et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.1 MB