Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-54gsr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-27T13:55:09.592Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of stress on delay discounting in female patients with early-onset bulimia nervosa and alcohol use disorder: functional magnetic resonance imaging study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 September 2025

Nicolas Leenaerts*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Research Group Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven Brain Institute, Leuven, Belgium Department of Neurosciences, Mind-Body Research, Research Group Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Jenny Ceccarini
Affiliation:
Department of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging, Research Nuclear Medicine & Molecular Imaging, KU Leuven, Leuven Brain Institute, Leuven, Belgium
Martin Weygandt
Affiliation:
Experimental and Clinical Research Center, a Cooperation between the Max Delbrück Center for Molecular Medicine in the Helmholtz Association and Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin, Germany Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Corporate Member of Freie Universität Berlin and Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Experimental and Clinical Research Center, Berlin, Germany Max Delbrück Center for Molecular Medicine in the Helmholtz Association (MDC), Berlin, Germany
Stefan Sunaert
Affiliation:
Translational MRI, Department of Imaging and Pathology, Biomedical Sciences Group, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Elske Vrieze
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Research Group Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven Brain Institute, Leuven, Belgium Department of Neurosciences, Mind-Body Research, Research Group Psychiatry, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
*
Correspondence: Nicolas Leenaerts. Email: nicolas.leenaerts@kuleuven.be
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

Stress could increase delay discounting in subjects with bulimia nervosa and alcohol use disorder (AUD), meaning that the short-term benefits of coping through eating or drinking outweigh the long-term negative consequences. Therefore, this study explores differences in delay discounting between patients and healthy controls, the impact of stress on food and alcohol delay discounting and associated changes in brain activity.

Method

A total of 102 female participants (AUD, 27; bulimia nervosa, 25; healthy controls, 50) underwent repeated functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning. Initially, all participants performed a monetary delay discounting task (DDT), followed by a food or alcohol DDT before and after stress induction. Specifically, those with bulimia nervosa completed a food DDT, those with AUD completed an alcohol DDT and healthy controls were randomly allocated to one or either DDT.

Results

Participants with AUD, but not healthy controls, displayed a higher discounting of alcohol after stress. Healthy controls, but not those with bulimia nervosa, had nominally higher discounting rates of food following stress, although not significant following multiple testing correction. Participants with AUD displayed a lower activity of the right supplementary motor area while discounting alcohol after stress. Healthy controls showed a lower activity of the frontal cortex and a higher activity of the motor cortex while discounting food after stress, while those with bulimia nervosa displayed a higher activity of the occipital cortex.

Conclusions

The results suggest that, in subjects with AUD, stress induces neurobiological changes that cause them to prefer more immediately available alcohol. However, the results observed in participants with bulimia nervosa suggest a more complex relation between stress and food.

Information

Type
Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Both bulimia nervosa and alcohol use disorder (AUD) are characterised by binge behaviour (i.e. binge eating and binge drinking), where large amounts of a substance (i.e. food and alcohol, respectively) are consumed within a short period of time. 1 Although treatments for bulimia nervosa and AUD exist, large numbers of individuals are not able to abstain from binge eating or binge drinking following treatment. Reference Linardon and Wade2,Reference Fleury, Djouini, Huỳnh, Tremblay, Ferland and Ménard3 More effective interventions are therefore needed but, in order to develop these, a better understanding of what triggers binge behaviour is required. Most studies have investigated bulimia nervosa and AUD separately to explore these triggers; however, because these conditions share a number of similarities, studying them together could provide more information by identifying both common and unique factors. One factor that is thought to play a role in both bulimia nervosa and AUD is delay discounting, which is the process whereby rewards decrease in value the more delayed they are. Reference Odum4 This means that individuals usually prefer more immediately available rewards over delayed ones. Reference Odum4

From a behavioural standpoint, delay discounting involves both a reward-processing and an impulsive-like component. Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine and Quinn5,Reference Strickland and Johnson6 On the one hand, delay discounting is subsumed under the positive valence systems of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC), where it is regarded as a moderator of reward valuation. Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine and Quinn5 On the other hand, delay discounting is described as a distinct construct of impulsive-like behaviour because it reduces the significance of negative consequences in the distant future, making it more likely for individuals to engage in behaviours that provide immediate gratification. Reference Strickland and Johnson6 With regard to bulimia nervosa and AUD, it is thought that individuals display a steeper delay discounting of rewards, leading them to put more value on short-term benefits than long-term negative consequences. Delay discounting can be investigated using a delay discounting task (DDT), where participants need to choose between a smaller earlier reward and a larger later reward. Reference Odum4 Based on the decisions made by an individual, a delay discounting rate can be calculated where higher values represent a stronger preference for more immediate rewards. Reference Odum4 Previous studies show that those with bulimia nervosa and AUD display higher delay discounting rates for monetary rewards, meaning that they prefer a more immediately available amount of money than a larger delayed amount. Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel and Vedelago7,Reference MacKillop, Amlung, Few, Ray, Sweet and Munafò8 However, with regard to disorder-specific food and alcohol delay discounting, only a few studies have been published. Reference Petry9,Reference Hagan, Jarmolowicz and Forbush10 We identified one study that investigated alcohol delay discounting in AUD, which found higher discounting rates compared with healthy controls. Reference Petry9 However, studies in individuals without AUD report that preferring larger amounts of alcohol during a DDT predicts past-month adverse consequences of drinking, and that individuals who meet more DSM-5 criteria for AUD, or score higher on the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT), also prefer more immediately available alcohol. Reference Naudé, Johnson, Strickland, Berry and Reed11Reference Phung, Snider, Tegge and Bickel13 We identified one study that investigated food delay discounting in bulimia nervosa, but that study found lower discounting rates. Reference Hagan, Jarmolowicz and Forbush10 In summary, those with bulimia nervosa or AUD seem to display a higher delay discounting of money, subjects with AUD may have higher delay discounting rates for alcohol and those with bulimia nervosa could actually have lower delay discounting rates for food, although further research on food and alcohol delay discounting in individuals with bulimia nervosa or AUD is needed.

From a neurobiological standpoint, one model suggests that delay discounting is processed in five subsequent steps involving specific brain regions at each step; Reference Frost and McNaughton14 an overview can be seen in Fig. 1. The key steps are III and IV, corresponding to the attribution of subjective value to the earlier and delayed rewards and the comparison between them. The attribution of subjective value is thought to be performed by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), middle frontal gyrus (MFG), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), insula, nucleus accumbens (NAc) and caudate nucleus. Reference Frost and McNaughton14 The comparison between these subjective values is thought to be performed by a dual system, consisting of a beta (β)-system that is impulsive, reflexive and focused on the immediate reward, and a delta (δ)-system that is controlled and processes both immediate and delayed rewards. Reference Frost and McNaughton14,Reference Schüller, Kuhn, Jessen and Hu15 The β-system is thought to be represented in the ACC and OFC, while the δ-system is encoded in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). Reference Frost and McNaughton14,Reference Schüller, Kuhn, Jessen and Hu15 In regard to the functioning of these brain regions during a DDT, no study to date has compared current subjects with bulimia nervosa and healthy controls. However, a study in remitted patients with bulimia nervosa found lower activity of the caudate nucleus during a monetary DDT after fasting, but higher activity after eating. Reference Bischoff-Grethe, Wierenga, Bailer, MCClure and Kaye16 More studies have been performed in those with AUD. Here, studies report that individuals display a greater deactivation of the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and PCC when making impulsive monetary choices, but a greater activation of the dlPFC, (pre)cuneus, insula and OFC when choosing the delayed option. Reference Amlung, Sweet, Acker, Brown and MacKillop17Reference Claus, Kiehl and Hutchison19 Furthermore, one study in heavy drinkers found that left mOFC activity during an alcohol/money DDT correlated with alcohol-induced craving for alcohol. Reference Lungwitz, Dzemidzic, Shen, Plawecki and Oberlin20 In summary, studies indicate that delay discounting for money could be processed differently in subjects with bulimia nervosa and AUD, although it has not been sufficiently explored whether this is also the case for food or alcohol.

Fig. 1 Neural processing of delay discounting. First, sensory information is transformed into object representations. Second, the object representations are used to establish the consequences of choosing the earlier or delayed reward. Third, the consequences are attributed a subjective value. Fourth, the subjective value between the earlier and delayed reward is compared using a dual system. Fifth, information on the decision is used to produce motor responses to acquire the reward. Regions: 1, insula; 2, superior temporal gyrus; 3, angular gyrus; 4, parietal cortex; 5, occipital cortex; 6, lingual gyrus; 7, thalamus; 8, cingulate cortex; 9, amygdala; 10, hippocampus; 11, middle frontal gyrus; 12, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 13, posterior cingulate gyrus; 14, anterior cingulate gyrus; 15, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; 16, orbitofrontal cortex; 17, caudate nucleus; 18, nucleus accumbens; 19, precentral gyrus; 20, putamen.

Furthermore, although these findings indicate that there are differences in delay discounting between participants and controls, they raise the question as to how these differences lead to periods of binge eating or binge drinking in subjects with bulimia nervosa or AUD. A key factor in this process could be stress, which is thought to be important in both bulimia nervosa and AUD, with most theoretical models positing that binge eating and binge drinking can be a way for the individual to cope with stress. Reference Boness, Watts, Moeller and Sher21,Reference Burton and Abbott22 It could be hypothesised that stress induces functional brain changes in people, resulting in an increase in delay discounting, making them see the short-term benefits of coping through eating or drinking alcohol as more valuable than the long-term benefits of remission. Indeed, studies in healthy volunteers found that acute stress increases the delay discounting of money and makes individuals choose based more on subjective value, although one study in heavy drinkers did not replicate the effect of stress on delay discounting of money. Reference Kimura, Izawa, Sugaya, Ogawa, Yamada and Shirotsuki23Reference Amlung and Mackillop26 However, although these studies indicate that stress could increase the delay discounting of money, its impact on food or alcohol delay discounting is unclear.

In summary, individuals with bulimia nervosa or AUD may display differences in the delay discounting of money, food or alcohol, as well as in how delay discounting is processed in the brain, which could be further amplified under stress. However, there are important gaps in the literature, which is why this study investigated the following hypotheses: (a) subjects with bulimia nervosa and AUD display higher delay discounting rates than healthy controls for money; (b) individuals with bulimia nervosa or AUD, but not healthy controls, display higher delay discounting rates for food and alcohol; and (c) stress increases delay discounting rates for food and alcohol in people with bulimia nervosa and AUD, respectively, but not in healthy controls. Although one previous study found lower delay discounting rates for food in individuals with bulimia nervosa, the current study still hypothesises that those with bulimia nervosa display higher delay discounting rates for food, based on studies looking at delay discounting of money in subjects with bulimia nervosa, and on the theory that these individuals prefer more short-term food rewards. Reference Odum4,Reference Hagan, Jarmolowicz and Forbush10,Reference Pearson, Wonderlich and Smith27 Additionally, differences in delay discounting between healthy controls and those with bulimia nervosa or AUD are hypothesised to be associated with brain activity changes in regions involved in the attribution and comparison of subjective value (i.e. the ACC, PCC, MFG, OFC, insula, dmPFC, dlPFC, NAc and CN).

Method

Participants

A total of 102 female right-handed participants were included in the study (AUD, 27; bulimia nervosa, 25; healthy controls, 50) following removal of 4 participants (bulimia nervosa, 3; healthy controls, 1) due to artefacts and incidental findings. Recruitment ran from September 2019 to February 2022 (eMethods 1 available in the Supplementary material at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2025.10821). The full inclusion and exclusion criteria can be found in the Supplementary materials (eMethods 2). Importantly, patients needed to meet the criteria for bulimia nervosa or AUD of DSM-5, and having a maximum illness duration of 5 years. 1 This maximum illness duration was set for the reason that the role of impulsive-like behaviour is thought to be greatest in the first years following the onset of bulimia nervosa or AUD, because binge eating and binge drinking become more habitual over time. Reference Boness, Watts, Moeller and Sher21,Reference Pearson, Wonderlich and Smith27 Participants with AUD also needed to display a pattern of repetitive binge drinking according to the criteria of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (i.e.,drinking 4 units of alcohol within 2 h for women). 28 Patients could not meet the criteria for both AUD and bulimia nervosa, because the inclusion of a group with both would not be compatible with the design of the DDTs as described below (i.e. performing either a food or alcohol DDT). All participants gave their written consent, and the study was approved by the local ethical committee of UZ/KU Leuven.

Procedure

The procedure of the magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanning can be seen in Fig. 2. Participants were instructed not to eat or drink anything in the 6 h leading up to the scan, and needed to refrain from using substances (e.g. alcohol, cannabis, etc.) in the 24 h before the scan. The median interquartile range (IQR) starting time of the scan was 17:00 (16:00–18:00). Participants were asked whether they had adhered to these instructions on the day of the scanning procedure and, if not, the scan was rescheduled. This was the case for one individual. Participants came in 45 min early to familiarise themselves with the tasks of the study in a practice session. Immediately before scanning, a photoplethysmography (PPG) sensor was placed on the left index finger to measure heart rate. The scan itself was divided into four main parts. First, all participants performed a monetary DDT (DDT1), followed by a disorder-specific (e.g. food or alcohol) DDT (DDT2). This meant that participants with bulimia nervosa completed a DDT with food while those with AUD completed one with alcohol. Healthy controls were randomly allocated to either the food (healthy controlsfood) or alcohol (healthy controlsalcohol) DDT as a comparison for those with bulimia nervosa and AUD, respectively. Third, stress was induced using the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST). Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Khalili Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner29 Fourth, participants repeated the food or alcohol DDT post-MIST (DDT3). The DDT1, DDT2 and STRESS blocks were separated by other MRI sequences not analysed in this manuscript. Further information on the study procedure can be found in the supplementary materials (eMethods 3 and eFig. 1).

Fig. 2 Study design. Participants fasted in the 6 h prior to the magnetic resonance imaging scan. They came in 45 min early to practise the tasks. The scan was divided into four main parts. First, all participants performed a monetary delay discounting task (DDT1). Second, those with bulimia nervosa completed a DDT with food while those with alcohol use disorder (AUD) completed one with alcohol. Healthy controls (HC) were randomly allocated to either the food or alcohol DDT (DDT2 pre-stress). Third, stress was induced with the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST; STRESS). Fourth, participants repeated the food or alcohol DDT (DDT3 post-stress). During the scan, participants reported on their stress level; their heart rate was measured with a photoplethysmography sensor. ASL, arterial spin labelling; BN, bulimia nervosa; DWI, diffusion-weighted imaging; Nu, now; rsfMRI, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging; Weken, weeks.

Measures

Baseline measures

The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID-5-S) was used to confirm the diagnosis of bulimia nervosa or AUD and to screen for other psychiatric disorders. 30 Bulimia nervosa and AUD severity were assessed with the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) and AUDIT, respectively. Reference Fairburn and Beglin31,Reference Saunders, Aasland, Babor, De La Fuente and Grant32 EDE-Q had excellent internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.95, and AUDIT had good internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.89. Of those participants with bulimia nervosa, 19 (76%) had an EDE-Q score over the clinical cut-off (2.8); among those with AUD, 26 (96%) had an AUDIT score over the threshold of medium-level problems (8) and 13 (48%) had an AUDIT score over the threshold of high-level problems (15). Additionally, using the DSM-5 criteria, there were 7 (26.9%) participants with mild AUD (2–3 positive criteria), 9 (34.6%) with moderate AUD (4–5 positive criteria) and 8 (30.8%) with severe AUD (>6 positive criteria). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS) was used to inventorise depression, anxiety and stress symptoms. Reference Lovibond and Lovibond33 This clinical assessment took place on a separate study visit before functional MRI (fMRI) scanning. The median (IQR) number of days between clinical assessment and fMRI scanning was 15 (9–42) days.

Delay discounting tasks

The DDTs were adapted from a food DDT that was used in a previous study. Reference Weygandt, Spranger, Leupelt, Maurer, Bobbert and Mai34 In each DDT, the participants chose between an amount of money, food or alcohol that was immediately available and a larger amount of the same reward that was available following a delay. The immediate rewards were either 5 euros, around 250 kcal of food or 1 unit of alcohol, while the delayed rewards were multiples (2- to 5-fold) of the immediate reward. The rewards were hypothetical, which is a valid alternative for real rewards in studies on delay discounting. Reference Madden, Begotka, Raiff and Kastern35 The type of food and alcohol used in the DDT (one type each for the DDT before and after stress) was selected by each participant from a list of possible food items and alcoholic beverages in the practice session (eMethods 4). Each of the multiples was paired with 1 of 10 delays for each decision, resulting in 40 trials per DDT. These delays were the deciles of a maximally tolerated delay level plus 10%, which was determined in the practice session (eMethods 5). Delays for the DDT with money were expressed in weeks, while those for the DDTs with food and alcohol were expressed in minutes. This is similar to previous studies, and was done so that the choices concerning food and alcohol more closely resembled those of people in evertday life. Reference Hendrickson, Rasmussen and Lawyer36 Each trial started with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) that varied between 3.5 and 5.0 s. Next, participants were shown the immediate and delayed options and were allowed 6 s to make their choice using a button box. A red arrowhead then appeared beneath their chosen option for the remainder of the 6 s before the next trial started. The ISI, as well as the magnitude, delay and position of the delayed reward, were determined pseudo-randomly for each trial. The total duration of each DDT (money, food or alcohol before stress, and food or alcohol after stress) was 6 min and 50 s.

MIST

MIST is a task that uses mental arithmetic, failure and negative social evaluation to induce stress in participants. Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Khalili Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner29,Reference Noack, Nolte, Nieratschker, Habel and Derntl37 It typically consists of a rest condition (i.e. only the interface), a control condition (i.e. only mental arithmetic) and an experimental condition (i.e. mental arithmetic with the stress components). Because the purpose of using MIST in this study was to induce stress, participants completed only the experimental condition with the scanner. Under this condition, participants were given mathematical problems and were required to respond before a certain amount of time had expired. Participants saw their own performance and a fictive average performance of all previously included subjects. They were then instructed to better this average, but the task adapted the difficulty of the mathematical problems so that participants performed poorly. In addition, negative feedback was given to participants emphasising their poor performance and urging them to perform better. The difficulty level for each participant with the scanner was established in the practice session (eMethods 6). Reference Wheelock, Harnett, Wood, Orem, Granger and Mrug38 The total duration of MIST in the scanner was 6 min.

Subjective stress

Participants rated their stress levels at the beginning of the scan, before each task (DDT1, DDT2, STRESS, DDT3) and at the end of the scan with a visual analogue scale (VAS). This VAS had 10 levels, ranging from 0 (not stressed at all) to 10 (never experienced such stress before).

Heart rate

PPG data were gathered at 500 Hz with the wireless pulse oximeter of the MRI system. These were then preprocessed with SCANPHYSLOG_Tools (Matlab R2019a). Reference Welch39 First, peaks were identified in the pulse waveforms. Second, the data were divided into 1-min epochs and heart rate for each epoch was calculated. Third, implausible heart rates below 30/min or above 200/min were filtered out.

MRI sequences

Scanning was performed on a 3T Achieva dStream Philips MRI scanner with a 32-channel receiver head coil. T2*-weighted echo-planar images were acquired during every DDT (275 volumes, 46 slices, repetition time 1.5 s, echo time 33 ms, flip angle 80°, voxel size 2.14 × 2.14 × 3.00 mm3, multiband acceleration factor 2, acquisition plane axial). A high-resolution, T1-weighted image was acquired during MIST using a three-dimensional turbo field echo sequence (208 slices, repetition time 5.9 ms, echo time 2.7 ms, flip angle 8°, voxel size 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm3, acquisition plane axial).

Data analysis

The data were analysed and reported in accordance with the guidelines of Frank et al, Reference Frank, Favaro, Marsh, Ehrlich and Lawson40 which were specifically developed to help set up and analyse neuroimaging studies that include patients with an eating disorder. A checklist can be found in the Supplementary material. The data are available upon request.

Baseline group comparisons

For continuous baseline measures, differences between groups were evaluated using either a t-test or Wilcoxon rank sum test, depending on whether the variable followed a normal distribution; normality was evaluated using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. For categorical baseline measures, Fisher’s exact test was used. These analyses were conducted separately for participants with AUD or bulimia nervosa and their respective control groups.

Delay discounting

For every DDT, a k-value (i.e. a delay discounting rate) was estimated by fitting the choice data to a hyperbolic discounting model (Matlab R2019a for iOS; MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA; https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html) (eMethods 7). Reference Weygandt, Spranger, Leupelt, Maurer, Bobbert and Mai34 The k-value describes how steeply value is degraded by delay, and is widely used in the literature to study delay discounting. Reference Odum4,Reference Odum, Becker, Haynes, Galizio, Frye and Downey41 The k-values were logarithmically transformed due to their non-normal distribution. The log(k)-values at each DDT were compared between groups with robust linear regression models. These models included the log(k)-values as the outcome and included group as the main effect (bulimia nervosa, AUD, healthy controls for the monetary DDT; bulimia nervosa, healthy controlsfood for the food DDT before and after stress; AUD, healthy controlsalcohol for the alcohol DDT before and after stress). The impact of stress was evaluated within groups with robust linear mixed models. These models included random intercepts for the participants, the log(k)-values of the disorder-specific DDT as the outcome as well as group (bulimia nervosa, healthy controlsfood for the food DDT; AUD, healthy controlsalcohol for the alcohol DDT) and time (before, after MIST) as main and interaction effects. All models included age and body mass index (BMI) as covariates. The model results were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure. Corrections were applied separately for AUD versus control comparison (six tests) and bulimia nervosa versus control comparison (six tests). Findings with P corrected < 0.05 were considered significant, while those with P corrected > 0.05 and P uncorrected < 0.05 were considered nominally significant.

Subjective and physiological stress response

The impact of MIST on subjective stress ratings and heart rate was evaluated with robust linear mixed models, similar to the models described above, but these included either subjective stress ratings or heart rate as outcome. For subjective stress, only the data pre- and post-MIST were used. For heart rate, only the data from the 6 min pre-MIST (i.e. during a resting-state arterial spin labelling sequence) and 6 min during the MIST were used. The results from the models were corrected for multiple testing using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure. This was done separately for the results concerning heart rate (five tests) and subjective stress (five tests). Findings with P corrected < 0.05 were considered significant, while those with P corrected > 0.05 and P uncorrected < 0.05 were considered nominally significant.

fMRI data

The fMRI data of each DDT were initially preprocessed with fmriprep (version 21.0.1, iOS; https://fmriprep.org/en/stable/), after which they were smoothed with an 8-mm, full-width at half- maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel in SPM12 (Matlab R2019a for iOS; Functional Imaging Laboratory, London, UK; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) (eMethods 8). Reference Esteban, Markiewicz, Blair, Moodie, Isik and Erramuzpe42 These smoothed images were then used in a first-level analysis in SPM12 (eMethods 9). On the one hand, this analysis included two boxcar regressors that separately modelled the decision and feedback stages. The decision stages started with the presentation of the rewards and ended when participants submitted their choice through the response box. The feedback stages followed immediately afterwards and ended 6 s following the initial presentation of the rewards. These boxcar regressors were convolved with the canonical haemodynamic response function. On the other hand, the first-level analysis included three rotation, three translation, six derivatives, five wCompCor, five cCompCor and five cosine variables as nuisance regressors. Reference Behzadi, Restom, Liau and Liu43Reference Parkes, Fulcher, Yücel and Fornito45 Further information on the nuisance regressors can be found in the Supplementary material (eMethods 8). From the first-level analysis, contrast images were calculated for the decision stages (i.e. decision stage, 1; feedback stage, 0).

These contrast images were used in a second-level analysis in SPM12 (Matlab R2019a). First, whole-brain analyses compared brain activity at each DDT between groups. A whole-brain analysis was chosen rather than a region-of-interest-based analysis, because this study is the first to investigate the effect of stress on brain activity during delay discounting, and due to the large number of brain areas involved in delay discounting. This was carried out with an analysis of variance design (group: bulimia nervosa, AUD, healthy controls) for the monetary DDT and a t-test design for the food (group: bulimia nervosa, healthy controlsfood) or alcohol (group: AUD, healthy controlsalcohol) DDTs before and after stress. Second, whole-brain analyses investigated the impact of stress on brain activity during the food or alcohol DDT within groups. This was performed with a full factorial design that included both group (bulimia nervosa, healthy controlsfood for the food DDT; AUD, healthy controlsalcohol for the alcohol DDT) and time (before, after MIST) as main and interaction effects. All designs included age and BMI as covariates of no interest, because these factors have a significant impact on neuroimaging, with age also being recommended as a covariate in fMRI analyses by Frank et al, Reference Frank, Favaro, Marsh, Ehrlich and Lawson40 and due to the lack of an active matching procedure for age and BMI. Reference Cole and Franke46,Reference Carnell, Gibson, Benson, Ochner and Geliebter47 The two-tailed statistical contrasts were tested for significance using cluster-level inference with an uncorrected cluster-defining threshold of P < 0.001 and a family-wise error (FWE)-corrected cluster threshold of P < 0.05. Third, underlying contrast values of significant clusters were extracted with the MarsBaR toolbox (Matlab R2019a) and related to relevant participant characteristics. As advised by the guidelines given in Frank et al, the contrast values were related to the log(k)-values, AUDIT and EDE-Q scores, binge eating and binge drinking frequency, age, BMI and use of contraceptives. Reference Frank, Favaro, Marsh, Ehrlich and Lawson40 An exploration of the effect of ethnicity, menstrual cycle, presence of comorbidities or history of anorexia nervosa was not possible due to a lack of observations. An exploration of illness duration was also not performed due to the restricted range in the current study. The analyses were performed with robust regression models using the robustlmm package (R, version 4.4.1 for iOS; R Foundation, Vienna, Austria; https://www.r-project.org/), which included the contrast values as the outcome and included a patient characteristic as predictor. Because the whole-brain analysis included age and BMI as covariates, these variables were also entered as covariates in the robust regression models. For this reason, the relations between the contrast values and age or BMI were investigated with one model that included both age and BMI as predictors.

Results

Sample characteristics

The characteristics of participants with bulimia nervosa (n = 25) and AUD (n = 27) and their respective controls (healthy controlsfood, n = 25 and healthy controlsalcohol, n = 25) are provided in Table 1. The characteristics of the pooled healthy controls group (n = 50) are provided in the Supplementary materials (eTable 1). There were no significant differences in age, BMI, years of education or ethnicity between participants and their respective control groups. Notably, patients with bulimia nervosa had higher scores on the depression (mean [s.d.] bulimia nervosa 10.6 [8.8], mean [s.d.] healthy controlsfood 2.6 [2.4], P < 0.001), anxiety (mean [s.d.] bulimia nervosa 6.6 [6.9], mean [s.d.] healthy controlsfood 2.1 [2.1], P = 0.008) and stress (mean [s.d.] bulimia nervosa 12.9 [9.0], mean [s.d.] healthy controlsfood 6.0 [4.1], P = 0.005) subscales of the DASS-42, as well as higher total scores (mean [s.d.] bulimia nervosa 30.1 [21.1], mean [s.d.] healthy controlsfood 10.7 [6.8], P < 0.001).

Table 1 Sample characteristics

AN, anorexia nervosa; AUD, alcohol use disorder; AUDIT, alcohol use disorders identification test; BMI, body mass index; BN, bulimia nervosa; DASS-42, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale; EDE-Q, Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire; MDD, major depressive disorder; PD, panic disorder; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; SAD, social anxiety disorder; SSRI, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.

a Patients were in different treatment modalities (i.e. ambulatory psychologist, psychiatrist, dietician or out-patient treatment programme).

b Significant result.

Behavioural data

Results for the statistical tests for DDTs can be found in Table 2. A summary of the log(k)-values from the DDTs can be found in the Supplementary material (eTable 2).

Table 2 Differences in delay discounting

AUD, alcohol use disorder; β, estimate; BN, bulimia nervosa; HC, healthy control; HCalcohol, healthy controls who performed the alcohol delay discounting task; HCfood, healthy controls who performed the food delay discounting task; MIST, Montreal Imaging Stress Task; P uncorrected, uncorrected P-value; P corrected, corrected P-value using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure.

a Nominally significant result.

b Significant result.

Delay discounting of money

There were no significant differences between the log(k)-values of the different groups (AUD versus bulimia nervosa; AUD versus healthy controls; bulimia nervosa versus healthy controls).

Delay discounting of food

There were no significant differences between the log(k)-values of participants with bulimia nervosa and healthy controlsfood before MIST. There were nominally higher log(k)-values following MIST in healthy controlsfood compared with pre-MIST (β = 0.060, s.e. = 0.028, P uncorrected = 0.039, P corrected = 0.233), but this difference did not remain significant after correcting for multiple testing. There was no significant difference in log(k)-values following MIST in participants with bulimia nervosa (β = 0.020, s.e. = 0.028, P corrected = 0.574) compared with pre-MIST. Furthermore, there was no significant interaction effect between MIST task and group concerning log(k)-values. There were no significant differences between the log(k)-values of participants with bulimia nervosa and healthy controlsfood following MIST.

Delay discounting of alcohol

There were no significant differences between the log(k)-values of participants with AUD and healthy controlsalcohol before MIST. Compared with pre-MIST, there were significantly higher log(k)-values post-MIST in participants with AUD (β = 0.073, s.e. = 0.19, P corrected = 0.002), but not in healthy controlsalcohol (β = 0.006, s.e. = 0.019, P corrected = 0.761). In other words, participants with AUD chose the immediately available alcohol more often following the induction of stress. There was a significant interaction effect between MIST task and group concerning log(k)-values, with participants with AUD having a significantly higher increase in values compared with healthy controlsalcohol (β = 0.067, s.e. = 0.027, P corrected = 0.049).

Subjective and physiological stress response

There was a significant increase in subjective stress ratings for all groups post-MIST compared with pre-MIST (healthy controls β = 3.369, s.e. = 0.270, P corrected < 0.001; bulimia nervosa β = 4.654, s.e. = 0.381, P corrected < 0.001; AUD β = 4.335, s.e. = 0.367, P corrected < 0.001), but this was more pronounced in patients (bulimia nervosa β = 1.30, s.e. = 0.467, P corrected = 0.008; AUD β = 0.967, s.e. = 0.456, P corrected = 0.036). There was also a significant increase in heart rate during MIST compared with pre-MIST in all groups (healthy controls β = 10.084, s.e. = 0.613, P corrected < 0.001; bulimia nervosa β = 10.416, s.e. = 0.857, P corrected < 0.001; AUD β = 8.077, s.e. = 0.872, P corrected < 0.001), but this did not differ significantly between groups.

fMRI data

The results for fMRI data can be seen in Figs. 3 and 4.

Fig. 3 Whole-brain between- and within-group differences during the delay discounting tasks (DDTs). (a) During the food DDT before the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST, pre-stress), participants with bulimia nervosa (BN) showed a weaker deactivation of the left insula and right insula compared with healthy controlsFOOD (HCFOOD); (b) during the food DDT following MIST (post-stress), those with BN displayed a weaker deactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex compared with HCFOOD; (c) post-MIST, compared with pre-MIST, participants with BN displayed a higher activity of the left occipital cortex and right occipital cortex. HCFOOD showed higher activity of the left and right postcentral gyrus and left and right supplementary motor area, but lower activity of the middle and superior frontal gyrus and posterior cingulate cortex. Participants with alcohol use disorder (AUD) displayed lower activity of the right supplementary motor area. HCFOOD, healthy controls who performed the food DDT.

Fig. 4 Associations between brain activity during delay discounting tasks and behavioural measures. (a) In participants with alcohol use disorder (AUD), following stress, brain activity in the right supplementary motor area during the alcohol delay discounting task (DDT) was negatively associated with log(k)-values (β = −0.679, s.e. = 0.201, P = 0.004); (b) in healthy controlsFOOD, following stress, brain activity in the anterior cingulate cortex/ventromedial prefrontal cortex during the food DDT was negatively associated with log(k)-values (β = 0.733, s.e. = 0.356, P = 0.048). β, estimate; HCFOOD, healthy controls who performed the food DDT; MIST, Montreal Imaging Stress Task.

Delay discounting of money

There were no significant differences in brain activity between the different groups (AUD versus bulimia nervosa; AUD versus healthy controls; bulimia nervosa versus healthy controls).

Delay discounting of food

Before MIST, participants with bulimia nervosa displayed a weaker deactivation of the left posterior insula (Montréal Neurological Institute (MNI): x = −47, y = −12, z = 8; k = 213, t 46 = 4.31; P FWE = 0.005) and right posterior insula (MNI: x = 36, y = −21, z = 2; k = 131, t 46 = 4.27; P FWE = 0.039) than healthy controlsfood. Furthermore, for bulimia nervosa, BMI was negatively associated with brain activity in the left posterior insula (β = −0.046, s.e. = 0.220, P = 0.049) and right posterior insula (β = −0.040, s.e. = 0.012, P = 0.004). In other words, weaker deactivation of the left and right posterior insulae was more pronounced in participants with a lower BMI. When comparing brain activity pre- and post-MIST, the healthy controlsfood group displayed higher activity following MIST in the left postcentral gyrus (MNI: x = −26, y = −60, z = 60, t 48 = 5.07, P FWE < 0.001), right postcentral gyrus (MNI: x = 0, y = 36, z = 54, t 48 = 4.49, P FWE = 0.009), left SMA (MNI: x = −11, y = 7, z = 38, t 48 = 5.15, P FWE = 0.003) and right SMA (MNI: x = 17, y = −10, z = 72, t 48 = 4.82, P FWE = 0.040), but lower activity of the medial MFG/SFG (MNI: x = 2, y = 63, z = 18, t 48 = 6.70, P FWE < 0.001) and PCC (MNI: x = 4, y = −45, z = 38, t 48 = 5.97, P FWE < 0.001). Furthermore, participants with bulimia nervosa showed higher activity following MIST of the left inferior occipital, superior occipital, lingual and fusiform gyrus (MNI: x = −30, y = −66, z = −6, k = 556, t 48 = 5.13, P FWE ≤ 0.001) and right lingual and fusiform gyrus (MNI: x = 24, y = −79, z = −6, k = 137, t 48 = 4.19, P FWE = 0.021). There was no significant interaction effect between MIST task and group concerning brain activity. Comparing brain activity between groups following MIST, participants with bulimia nervosa displayed a weaker deactivation of the ACC (MNI: x = −2, y = 22, z = −4, k = 203, t 46 = 4.78, P FWE = 0.008) than healthy controlsfood. Furthermore, a lower activity of the ACC was associated with higher log(k)-values in healthy controlsfood (β = −0.733, s.e. = 0.356, P = 0.048) and higher BMI (β = −0.083, s.e. = 0.029, P = 0.009) in participants with bulimia nervosa. This means that a lower activity of the ACC was related to a higher preference for more immediately available food in healthy controlsfood.

Delay discounting of alcohol

There were no significant differences in brain activity between participants with AUD and healthy controlsalcohol before MIST. Compared with pre-MIST, the AUD group displayed lower activity following MIST of the right SMA (MNI: x = 13, y = 5, z = 56, k = 123, t 48 = 5.23, P FWE = 0.007). Furthermore, lower activity of the right SMA was associated with higher log(k)-values following stress (β = −0.682, s.e. = 0.190, P = 0.003) in participants with AUD. This indicates that a lower activity of the right SMA was related to a higher preference for more immediately available alcohol in participants with AUD. There was no significant interaction effect between MIST task and group concerning brain activity. Comparing brain activity between groups following MIST, no significant differences were found between participants with AUD and healthy controlsalcohol.

Discussion

This study investigated four hypotheses. First, that those with bulimia nervosa or AUD have higher delay discounting rates for money than healthy controls. Second, that those with bulimia nervosa or AUD have higher delay discounting rates for food or alcohol than healthy controls. Third, that those with bulimia nervosa or AUD, but not healthy controls, display higher delay discounting rates for food or alcohol when stressed. Fourth, that these behavioural differences are related to brain activity changes in regions involved in the attribution and comparison of subjective value.

With regard to behaviour, this study finds no significant difference between groups concerning the delay discounting of money, food or alcohol before stress. However, it does find a nominally higher preference for more immediately available food in healthy controls following stress compared with before stress, but not in subjects with bulimia nervosa, although this did not remain after correcting for multiple testing. It also finds a significantly higher preference for more immediately available alcohol in individuals with AUD following stress compared with before stress, but not in healthy controls. Concerning brain activity, the results show that individuals with bulimia nervosa display a weaker deactivation of the left and right posterior insula while delay discounting food than healthy controls. They also show that healthy controls demonstrated a lower activity of the frontal cortex and higher activity of the motor cortex while delay discounting food following stress compared with before stress, but that those with bulimia nervosa displayed a higher activity of the occipital cortex. Furthermore, the results show that subjects with AUD display a lower activity of the right SMA while delay discounting alcohol following stress. This absence of a difference between participants and controls in the delay discounting of money, food and alcohol is unexpected, because such a difference has been found in previous studies. Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel and Vedelago7,Reference MacKillop, Amlung, Few, Ray, Sweet and Munafò8,Reference Hagan, Jarmolowicz and Forbush10 These negative findings could be due to our relatively small sample size; although this study meets the sample size requirements of the guidelines and includes a similar number of participants to previous studies, sample size is still limited. Reference Amlung, Marsden, Holshausen, Morris, Patel and Vedelago7,Reference MacKillop, Amlung, Few, Ray, Sweet and Munafò8,Reference Hagan, Jarmolowicz and Forbush10 Future studies should therefore explore behavioural differences in food or alcohol delay discounting with a larger number of participants.

The finding that participants with AUD, but not healthy controls, prefer more immediately available alcohol following stress compared with before stress is in accordance with our hypotheses. It expands our knowledge from previous studies which show that stress can increase the value of alcohol and make individuals prefer this over other commodities such as money. Reference Amlung and Mackillop26,Reference Rousseau, Irons and Correia48 This could be the reason why stress causes individuals to drink more alcohol and why it is an important predictor of relapse. Reference Bresin, Mekawi and Verona49,Reference Brown, Vik, McQuaid, Patterson, Irwin and Grant50 Unexpectedly in this study, the higher preference for more immediately available alcohol is related to a lower activity of the right SMA, which is involved in step V (response) of the neural processing of delay discounting. Indeed, the SMA is known for its role in regulating goal-directed motor activity, but is also important for cognitive and inhibitory control. Reference Matsumoto, Suzuki and Tanaka51,Reference Weafer, Crane, Gorka, Phan and de Wit52 The lower activity of the SMA following stress could therefore reflect a loss of control over alcohol in those with AUD. Indeed, problematic drinkers have a higher tendency to lose control and act rashly when stress levels are high. Reference Fischer, Settles, Collins, Gunn and Smith53 Future studies should explore whether this relation between stress and alcohol delay discounting is predictive of treatment outcome, and whether it can be impacted by interventions.

The lack of a significant difference in food delay discounting between healthy controls and those with bulimia nervosa raises the question of what is signified by the weaker deactivation of the posterior insula in those with bulimia nervosa. In general, the insula is important in step II (consequences of approach) of the neural processing of delay discounting. Furthermore, previous studies show that the insula plays a role in the neural processing of food rewards, especially in the encoding of the intensity and aversity of food. Reference Gehrlach, Dolensek, Klein, Roy Chowdhury, Matthys and Junghänel54Reference Rolls56 For example, lesions of the posterior insula cause food to be perceived as less intense or unpleasant. Reference Balleine and Dickinson57,Reference Mak, Simmons, Gitelman and Small58 Taken together, the findings of the current study suggest that those with bulimia nervosa experienced choosing the food items as either more intense or aversive. One reason why this study may have reached this conclusion is that participants were asked to select an item of food with which they could have a binge eating episode. Indeed, previous studies report that food items consumed during a binge eating episode can be ‘forbidden’ outside of such an episode. Reference Guertin59 This could have made participants in the current study more inclined to restrict their food intake. If so, this would be in line with a previous study reporting that individuals with bulimia nervosa have lower delay discounting rates for food than healthy controls, meaning that they prefer the delayed food option over the immediately available one. Reference Hagan, Jarmolowicz and Forbush10

In the current study, there was no significant change in the delay discounting of food following stress in participants with bulimia nervosa. Although previous studies have found that stress causes individuals who binge eat to eat more, most of these studies were performed in individuals with binge eating disorder who did not display compensatory behaviours such as fasting. Reference Cardi, Leppanen and Treasure60 To our knowledge, there is only one study that has investigated the impact of stress on food intake in individuals with bulimia nervosa, and it reported no effect. Reference Westwater, Mancini, Shapleske, Serfontein, Ernst and Ziauddeen61 This suggests that the acute type of stress that is typically induced in a laboratory or neuroimaging setting does not make participants with bulimia nervosa lose control. Indeed, one previous study found that such acute stress did not reduce inhibitory control in participants with bulimia nervosa. Reference Westwater, Mancini, Gorka, Shapleske, Serfontein and Grillon62 However, studies in daily life have found that negative emotions such as stress increase before a binge eating episode in those with bulimia nervosa. Reference Haedt-Matt and Keel63,Reference Mikhail64 They also found that some emotions are more related to binge eating than others (i.e. guilt versus nervousness) and that not acute stress, but the accumulation of stress, is predictive of binge eating. Reference Berg, Crosby, Cao, Peterson, Engel and Mitchell65,Reference Smith, Mason, Reilly, Hazzard, Borg and Dvorak66 Together, these findings suggest that the relation between negative emotions and binge eating in subjects with bulimia nervosa could be dependent on the underlying emotions and their dynamics. Future neuroimaging studies should explore this by investigating the effect of various negative emotions with different designs (e.g. longer or repeated stress induction).

Although there was no significant change in food delay discounting found following stress in participants with bulimia nervosa in the current study, there was a significant change concerning brain activity. Namely, following stress, participants with bulimia nervosa showed higher activity of the occipital cortex, which is involved in step I (object representations) of the neural processing of delay discounting. This is in line with a study showing that individuals with bulimia nervosa display higher activity of the occipital cortex when viewing images of food following stress. Reference Collins, Breithaupt, McDowell, Miller, Thompson and Fischer67 Indeed, previous studies report that stress can lead to a higher activity of the occipital cortex and that this could be a sign of hypervigilance or amplified sensory processing. Reference Shackman, Maxwell, McMenamin, Greischar and Davidson68,Reference Waugh, Hamilton, Chen, Joormann and Gotlib69 Therefore, these results suggest that stress makes those with bulimia nervosa process food differently, but they do not explain how. Future studies should explore how stress changes the sensory processing of food in subjects with bulimia nervosa and how this is related to certain cognitions about food.

In contrast to participants with bulimia nervosa, this study finds a nominally higher preference for immediately available food in healthy controls following stress compared with before stress. Such a difference could be due to the absence of fasting and other compensatory behaviours in healthy controls, compared with participants with bulimia nervosa. However, this should be interpreted with caution because the finding did not remain significant following correction for multiple testing. In addition, healthy controls also displayed lower activity of the PCC and medial MFG/SFG following stress compared with before stress. These regions play an important role in step III (subjective value) of the neural processing of delay discounting. A decrease in their activity could indicate that the delayed food option has less value to healthy controls following stress. If so, this could be the reason why healthy controls would be more likely to choose the immediately available food option, and would explain why lower activity in the frontal cortex following stress was related to higher log(k)-values.

This study has several limitations. First, the relatively small sample size could have limited the power to detect differences between participants and healthy controls, and possibly impacted the replicability of results. Reference Turner, Paul, Miller and Barbey70 Second, the order of the different DDTs was not randomised within a session or separated across sessions, which could have led to practice or habituation effects. However, the decision to place the monetary DDT before the food or alcohol DDT is based on previous studies reporting that exposure to cues can impact reward processing in participants. Reference Kambouropoulos and Staiger71 Also, the tasks were not split across sessions to limit within-person variability. Third, as participants had not been randomised between stress and control conditions, it is possible that some effects in this study are due to fatigue or sensory-specific satiety due to the repeated use of the DDT. Fourth, most participants in this study were young Caucasian women with a short illness duration, and this limits the generalisability of the results to all those with bulimia nervosa or AUD. Future studies should aim to replicate these findings in other samples. Additionally, the young age of the participants could have influenced the absence of a difference in the delay discounting of money between participants and healthy controls, because young individuals already display higher delay discounting rates. Reference Steinberg, Graham, O’Brien, Woolard, Cauffman and Banich72 Relatedly, a number of individuals in the healthy controlsalcohol group engaged in binge drinking, which could explain the similar delay discounting rates for alcohol in participants with AUD and healthy controls. Fifth, like most studies investigating the neurobiological reward system in binge eating and binge drinking, this study looked at voxel-wise brain activity; Reference Leenaerts, Jongen, Ceccarini, Van Oudenhove and Vrieze73 however, reward processing is more than a simple hyper- and hypoactivation of brain areas. Reference Leenaerts, Jongen, Ceccarini, Van Oudenhove and Vrieze73 Future studies should also either explore connectivity or perform multivariate analyses to examine neurobiological differences in delay discounting. Sixth, the decision and feedback stages were modelled with boxcar regressors, but these could also be modelled in other ways (e.g. with parametric regressors), and this may have influenced the results. Seventh, only the delay discounting of money was compared between participants with AUD and bulimia nervosa, because the commodities of the other DDTs differed between groups. Future studies could use cross-commodity DDTs to better compare both participant groups. Eighth, delay discounting was studied using k-values from a hyperbolic discounting model, although other measures of delay discounting exist (e.g. the area under the curve), which could have provided a different perspective on the results. Ninth, in the first-level analysis of the fMRI data, the decision and feedback stages were modelled separately and contrast images were calculated for the decision stage. This approach was chosen because combining both stages in a single boxcar regressor would be problematic, since they represent distinct cognitive processes. However, a potential limitation is the overlap of hemodynamic response functions between these stages. Additionally, alternative fMRI modelling approaches exist, which could provide additional insights in future research.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2025.10821

Data availability

The data and scripts that support the findings of this study are available upon request.

Author contributions

N.L. had full access to all data in the study and takes responsibility for their integrity and the accuracy of data analysis. Concept and design: N.L., M.W., J.C., E.V. Acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data: N.L., S.S., E.V. Drafting of the manuscript: N.L. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: N.L., S.S., M.W., J.C., E.V. Funding acquisition: E.V., J.C. Supervision: E.V.

Funding

A C1 grant (no. ECA-D4671-C14/18/096) of the Special Research Fund KU Leuven to E.V. and J.C. served as a PhD Scholarship for N.L. J.C. is supported by a postdoctorate grant from FWO (no. 12R1619N).

Declaration of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation, and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 as revised in 2013.

References

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn). APA, 2013.Google Scholar
Linardon, J, Wade, TD. How many individuals achieve symptom abstinence following psychological treatments for bulimia nervosa? A meta-analytic review. Int J Eat Disord 2018; 51: 287–94.10.1002/eat.22838CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleury, MJ, Djouini, A, Huỳnh, C, Tremblay, J, Ferland, F, Ménard, JM, et al. Remission from substance use disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend 2016; 168: 293306.10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.08.625CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odum, AL. Delay discounting: I’m a k, You’re a k. J Exp Anal Behav 2011; 96: 427–39.10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Insel, T, Cuthbert, B, Garvey, M, Heinssen, R, Pine, DS, Quinn, K, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 748–51.10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strickland, JC, Johnson, MW. Rejecting impulsivity as a psychological construct: a theoretical, empirical, and sociocultural argument. Psychol Rev 2021; 128: 336–61.10.1037/rev0000263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amlung, M, Marsden, E, Holshausen, K, Morris, V, Patel, H, Vedelago, L, et al. Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2019; 76: 1176–86.10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKillop, J, Amlung, MT, Few, LR, Ray, LA, Sweet, LH, Munafò, MR. Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011; 216: 305–21.10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petry, NM. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 154: 243–50.10.1007/s002130000638CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagan, KE, Jarmolowicz, DP, Forbush, KT. Reconsidering delay discounting in bulimia nervosa. Eat Behav 2021; 41: 101506.10.1016/j.eatbeh.2021.101506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naudé, GP, Johnson, MW, Strickland, JC, Berry, MS, Reed, DD. At-risk drinking, operant demand, and cross-commodity discounting as predictors of drunk driving in underage college women. Behav Process 2022; 195: 104548.10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104548CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moody, LN, Tegge, AN, Bickel, WK. Cross-commodity delay discounting of alcohol and money in alcohol users. Psychol Rec 2017; 67: 285–92.10.1007/s40732-017-0245-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phung, QH, Snider, SE, Tegge, AN, Bickel, WK. Willing to work but not to wait: individuals with greater alcohol use disorder show increased delay discounting across commodities and less effort discounting for alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2019; 43: 927–36.10.1111/acer.13996CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R, McNaughton, N. The neural basis of delay discounting: a review and preliminary model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2017; 79: 4865.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schüller, CB, Kuhn, J, Jessen, F, Hu, X. Neuronal correlates of delay discounting in healthy subjects and its implication for addiction: an ALE meta-analysis study. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2019; 45: 5166.10.1080/00952990.2018.1557675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bischoff-Grethe, A, Wierenga, CE, Bailer, UF, MCClure, SM, Kaye, WH. Satiety does not alter the ventral striatum’s response to immediate reward in bulimia nervosa. J Abnorm Psychol 2021; 130: 862–74.10.1037/abn0000712CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amlung, M, Sweet, LH, Acker, J, Brown, CL, MacKillop, J. Dissociable brain signatures of choice conflict and immediate reward preferences in alcohol use disorders. Addict Biol 2014; 19: 743–53.10.1111/adb.12017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boettiger, CA, Mitchell, JM, Tavares, VC, Robertson, M, Joslyn, G, D’Esposito, M, et al. Immediate reward bias in humans: fronto-parietal networks and a role for the Catechol-O-Methyltransferase 158Val/Val Genotype. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 14383–91.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2551-07.2007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claus, ED, Kiehl, KA, Hutchison, KE. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of impulsive choice in alcohol use disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011; 35: 1209–19.10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01455.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lungwitz, EA, Dzemidzic, M, Shen, YI, Plawecki, MH, Oberlin, BG. Brain response in heavy drinkers during cross-commodity alcohol and money discounting with potentially real rewards: a preliminary study. Drug Alcohol Depend Rep 2023; 8: 100175.10.1016/j.dadr.2023.100175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boness, CL, Watts, AL, Moeller, KN, Sher, KJ. The etiologic, theory-based, ontogenetic hierarchical framework of alcohol use disorder: a translational systematic review of reviews. Psychol Bull 2021; 147: 10751123.10.1037/bul0000333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, AL, Abbott, MJ. Conceptualising binge eating: a review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Behav Change 2017; 34: 168–98.10.1017/bec.2017.12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimura, K, Izawa, S, Sugaya, N, Ogawa, N, Yamada, KC, Shirotsuki, K, et al. The biological effects of acute psychosocial stress on delay discounting. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013; 38: 2300–8.10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.04.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, MJ, Morris, CP, Lawford, BR, Young, RMD. Behavioral phenotypes of impulsivity related to the ANKK1 gene are independent of an acute stressor. Behav Brain Funct 2008; 4: 54.10.1186/1744-9081-4-54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, L, Jiryis, T, Admon, R. Now or later? Stress-induced increase and decrease in choice impulsivity are both associated with elevated affective and endocrine responses. Brain Sci 2021; 11: 1148.10.3390/brainsci11091148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amlung, M, Mackillop, J. Understanding the effects of stress and alcohol cues on motivation for alcohol via behavioral economics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2014; 38: 1780–9.10.1111/acer.12423CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, CM, Wonderlich, SA, Smith, GT. A risk and maintenance model for bulimia nervosa: from impulsive action to compulsive behavior. Psychol Rev 2015; 122: 516–35.10.1037/a0039268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Alcohol’s Effects on Health. NIAAA, 2022 (https://www.niaaa.nih.gov/alcohol-health/overview-alcohol-consumption/moderate-binge-drinking).Google Scholar
Dedovic, K, Renwick, R, Khalili Mahani, N, Engert, V, Lupien, SJ, Pruessner, JC, et al. The Montreal Imaging Stress Task: using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2005; 30: 319–25.10.1139/jpn.0541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. SCID-5-S gestructureerd klinisch interview voor DSM-5 syndroomstoornissen. Nederlandse vertaling van Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5® Disorders–Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV) [SCID-5-CV: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders–Clinician Version]. APA, 2017.Google Scholar
Fairburn, CG, Beglin, SJ. Assessment of eating disorders: interview or self-report questionnaire? Int J Eat Disord 1994; 16: 363–70.10.1002/1098-108X(199412)16:4<363::AID-EAT2260160405>3.0.CO;2-#3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, JB, Aasland, OG, Babor, TFE, De La Fuente, JR, Grant, M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption--II. Addiction 1993; 88: 791804.10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovibond, PF, Lovibond, SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav Res Ther 1995; 33: 335–43.10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-UCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weygandt, M, Spranger, J, Leupelt, V, Maurer, L, Bobbert, T, Mai, K, et al. Interactions between neural decision-making circuits predict long-term dietary treatment success in obesity. Neuroimage 2019; 184: 520–34.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, GJ, Begotka, AM, Raiff, BR, Kastern, LL. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2003; 11: 139–45.10.1037/1064-1297.11.2.139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendrickson, KL, Rasmussen, EB, Lawyer, SR. Measurement and validation of measures for impulsive food choice across obese and healthy-weight individuals. Appetite 2015; 90: 254–63.10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noack, H, Nolte, L, Nieratschker, V, Habel, U, Derntl, B. Imaging stress: an overview of stress induction methods in the MR scanner. J Neural Transm 2019; 126: 1187–202.10.1007/s00702-018-01965-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheelock, MD, Harnett, NG, Wood, KH, Orem, TR, Granger, DA, Mrug, S, et al. Prefrontal cortex activity is associated with biobehavioral components of the stress response. Front Hum Neurosci 2016; 10: 583.10.3389/fnhum.2016.00583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, GKW, Favaro, A, Marsh, R, Ehrlich, S, Lawson, EA. Toward valid and reliable brain imaging results in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2018; 51: 250–61.10.1002/eat.22829CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odum, AL, Becker, RJ, Haynes, JM, Galizio, A, Frye, CCJ, Downey, H, et al. Delay discounting of different outcomes: review and theory. J Exp Anal Behav 2020; 113: 657–79.10.1002/jeab.589CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esteban, O, Markiewicz, CJ, Blair, RW, Moodie, CA, Isik, AI, Erramuzpe, A, et al. fMRIPrep: a robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods 2019; 16: 111–6.10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behzadi, Y, Restom, K, Liau, J, Liu, TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage 2007; 37: 90101.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muschelli, J, Nebel, MB, Caffo, BS, Barber, AD, Pekar, JJ, Mostofsky, SH. Reduction of motion-related artifacts in resting state fMRI using aCompCor. Neuroimage 2014; 96: 2235.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkes, L, Fulcher, B, Yücel, M, Fornito, A. An evaluation of the efficacy, reliability, and sensitivity of motion correction strategies for resting-state functional MRI. Neuroimage 2018; 171: 415–36.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.073CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, JH, Franke, K. Predicting age using neuroimaging: innovative brain ageing biomarkers. Trends Neurosci 2017; 40: 681–90.10.1016/j.tins.2017.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carnell, S, Gibson, C, Benson, L, Ochner, CN, Geliebter, A. Neuroimaging and obesity: current knowledge and future directions. Obesity Rev 2012; 13: 4356.10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00927.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rousseau, GS, Irons, JG, Correia, CJ. The reinforcing value of alcohol in a drinking to cope paradigm. Drug Alcohol Depend 2011; 118: 14.10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.02.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bresin, K, Mekawi, Y, Verona, E. The effect of laboratory manipulations of negative affect on alcohol craving and use: a meta-analysis. Psychol Addict Behav 2018; 32: 617–27.10.1037/adb0000383CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, SA, Vik, PW, McQuaid, JR, Patterson, TL, Irwin, MR, Grant, I. Severity of psychosocial stress and outcome of alcoholism treatment. J Abnorm Psychol 1990; 99: 344–8.10.1037/0021-843X.99.4.344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, K, Suzuki, W, Tanaka, K. Neuronal correlates of goal-based motor selection in the prefrontal cortex. Science 2003; 301: 229–32.10.1126/science.1084204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weafer, J, Crane, NA, Gorka, SM, Phan, KL, de Wit, H. Neural correlates of inhibition and reward are negatively associated. Neuroimage 2019; 196: 188–94.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, S, Settles, R, Collins, B, Gunn, R, Smith, GT. The role of negative urgency and expectancies in problem drinking and disordered eating: testing a model of comorbidity in pathological and at-risk samples. Psychol Addict Behav 2012; 26: 112–23.10.1037/a0023460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gehrlach, DA, Dolensek, N, Klein, AS, Roy Chowdhury, R, Matthys, A, Junghänel, M, et al. Aversive state processing in the posterior insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 2019; 22: 1424–37.10.1038/s41593-019-0469-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y, Kakusa, BW, Feng, A, Gattas, S, Shivacharan, RS, Lee, EB, et al. The insulo-opercular cortex encodes food-specific content under controlled and naturalistic conditions. Nat Commun 2021; 12: 3609.10.1038/s41467-021-23885-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, ET. Taste and smell processing in the brain. Handb Clin Neurol 2019; 164: 97118.Google ScholarPubMed
Balleine, BW, Dickinson, A. The effect of lesions of the insular cortex on instrumental conditioning: evidence for a role in incentive memory. J Neurosci 2000; 20: 8954–64.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08954.2000CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mak, YE, Simmons, KB, Gitelman, DR, Small, DM. Taste and olfactory intensity perception changes following left insular stroke. Behav Neurosci 2005; 119: 1693–700.10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1693CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guertin, TL. Eating behavior of bulimics, self-identified binge eaters, and non-eating-disordered individuals: what differentiates these populations? Clin Psychol Rev 1999; 19: 123.10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00017-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardi, V, Leppanen, J, Treasure, J. The effects of negative and positive mood induction on eating behaviour: a meta-analysis of laboratory studies in the healthy population and eating and weight disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 57: 299309.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westwater, ML, Mancini, F, Shapleske, J, Serfontein, J, Ernst, M, Ziauddeen, H, et al. Dissociable hormonal profiles for psychopathology and stress in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Psychol Med 2021; 51: 2814.10.1017/S0033291720001440CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westwater, ML, Mancini, F, Gorka, AX, Shapleske, J, Serfontein, J, Grillon, C, et al. Prefrontal responses during proactive and reactive inhibition are differentially impacted by stress in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J Neurosci 2021; 41: 4487–99.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2853-20.2021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haedt-Matt, AA, Keel, PK. Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: a meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychol Bull 2011; 137: 660–81.10.1037/a0023660CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikhail, M. Affect dysregulation in context: implications and future directions of experience sampling research on affect regulation models of loss of control eating. Front Psychiatry 2021; 12: 747854.10.3389/fpsyt.2021.747854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, KC, Crosby, RD, Cao, L, Peterson, CB, Engel, SG, Mitchell, JE, et al. Facets of negative affect prior to and following binge-only, purge-only, and binge/purge events in women with bulimia nervosa. J Abnorm Psychol 2013; 122: 111–8.10.1037/a0029703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, KE, Mason, TB, Reilly, EE, Hazzard, VM, Borg, SL, Dvorak, R, et al. Examining prospective mediational relationships between momentary rumination, negative affect, and binge eating using ecological momentary assessment. J Affect Disord Rep 2021; 5: 100138.Google ScholarPubMed
Collins, B, Breithaupt, L, McDowell, JE, Miller, LS, Thompson, J, Fischer, S. The impact of acute stress on the neural processing of food cues in bulimia nervosa: replication in two samples. J Abnorm Psychol 2017; 126: 540–51.10.1037/abn0000242CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, AJ, Maxwell, JS, McMenamin, BW, Greischar, LL, Davidson, RJ. Stress potentiates early and attenuates late stages of visual processing. J Neurosci 2011; 31: 1156–61.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waugh, CE, Hamilton, JP, Chen, MC, Joormann, J, Gotlib, IH. Neural temporal dynamics of stress in comorbid major depressive disorder and social anxiety disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2012; 2: 11.10.1186/2045-5380-2-11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, BO, Paul, EJ, Miller, MB, Barbey, AK. Small sample sizes reduce the replicability of task-based fMRI studies. Commun Biol 2018; 1: 62.10.1038/s42003-018-0073-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kambouropoulos, N, Staiger, PK. The influence of sensitivity to reward on reactivity to alcohol-related cues. Addiction 2001; 96: 1175–85.10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.968117510.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L, Graham, S, O’Brien, L, Woolard, J, Cauffman, E, Banich, M. Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Dev 2009; 80: 2844.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leenaerts, N, Jongen, D, Ceccarini, J, Van Oudenhove, L, Vrieze, E. The neurobiological reward system and binge eating: a critical systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Int J Eat Disord 2022; 55: 1421–58.10.1002/eat.23776CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Neural processing of delay discounting. First, sensory information is transformed into object representations. Second, the object representations are used to establish the consequences of choosing the earlier or delayed reward. Third, the consequences are attributed a subjective value. Fourth, the subjective value between the earlier and delayed reward is compared using a dual system. Fifth, information on the decision is used to produce motor responses to acquire the reward. Regions: 1, insula; 2, superior temporal gyrus; 3, angular gyrus; 4, parietal cortex; 5, occipital cortex; 6, lingual gyrus; 7, thalamus; 8, cingulate cortex; 9, amygdala; 10, hippocampus; 11, middle frontal gyrus; 12, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 13, posterior cingulate gyrus; 14, anterior cingulate gyrus; 15, ventromedial prefrontal cortex; 16, orbitofrontal cortex; 17, caudate nucleus; 18, nucleus accumbens; 19, precentral gyrus; 20, putamen.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Study design. Participants fasted in the 6 h prior to the magnetic resonance imaging scan. They came in 45 min early to practise the tasks. The scan was divided into four main parts. First, all participants performed a monetary delay discounting task (DDT1). Second, those with bulimia nervosa completed a DDT with food while those with alcohol use disorder (AUD) completed one with alcohol. Healthy controls (HC) were randomly allocated to either the food or alcohol DDT (DDT2 pre-stress). Third, stress was induced with the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST; STRESS). Fourth, participants repeated the food or alcohol DDT (DDT3 post-stress). During the scan, participants reported on their stress level; their heart rate was measured with a photoplethysmography sensor. ASL, arterial spin labelling; BN, bulimia nervosa; DWI, diffusion-weighted imaging; Nu, now; rsfMRI, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging; Weken, weeks.

Figure 2

Table 1 Sample characteristics

Figure 3

Table 2 Differences in delay discounting

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Whole-brain between- and within-group differences during the delay discounting tasks (DDTs). (a) During the food DDT before the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST, pre-stress), participants with bulimia nervosa (BN) showed a weaker deactivation of the left insula and right insula compared with healthy controlsFOOD (HCFOOD); (b) during the food DDT following MIST (post-stress), those with BN displayed a weaker deactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex compared with HCFOOD; (c) post-MIST, compared with pre-MIST, participants with BN displayed a higher activity of the left occipital cortex and right occipital cortex. HCFOOD showed higher activity of the left and right postcentral gyrus and left and right supplementary motor area, but lower activity of the middle and superior frontal gyrus and posterior cingulate cortex. Participants with alcohol use disorder (AUD) displayed lower activity of the right supplementary motor area. HCFOOD, healthy controls who performed the food DDT.

Figure 5

Fig. 4 Associations between brain activity during delay discounting tasks and behavioural measures. (a) In participants with alcohol use disorder (AUD), following stress, brain activity in the right supplementary motor area during the alcohol delay discounting task (DDT) was negatively associated with log(k)-values (β = −0.679, s.e. = 0.201, P = 0.004); (b) in healthy controlsFOOD, following stress, brain activity in the anterior cingulate cortex/ventromedial prefrontal cortex during the food DDT was negatively associated with log(k)-values (β = 0.733, s.e. = 0.356, P = 0.048). β, estimate; HCFOOD, healthy controls who performed the food DDT; MIST, Montreal Imaging Stress Task.

Supplementary material: File

Leenaerts et al. supplementary material

Leenaerts et al. supplementary material
Download Leenaerts et al. supplementary material(File)
File 289.9 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.