Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-c75p9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-06T22:14:30.543Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Second Language Pragmatics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2022

Wei Ren
Affiliation:
Beihang University, China

Summary

This Element introduces the areas that second language (L2) pragmatics research has investigated. It begins with a theme-based review of the field with respect to L2 pragmatics learning, teaching, and assessing. The section on pragmatics learning examines studies on learners' pragmatic production and perception, and analyzes research modalities in this field. The section on pragmatics teaching examines the effects of and different approaches to L2 pragmatics instruction; and the section on pragmatics assessing examines the aspects involved in testing learners' pragmatic competence, and studies on issues related to validity and rating in pragmatics assessing. The Element then analyzes studies exploring learners' cognitive processes during pragmatic performance, and case studies are provided to showcase two ongoing projects, one investigating advanced learners' self-praise on social media and the other investigating lingua franca pragmatics among children. Finally, the Element offers some topics and questions for future research in L2 pragmatics.

Information

Type
Element
Information
Online ISBN: 9781009082709
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication: 14 July 2022

Second Language Pragmatics

1 Second Language Pragmatics: An Introduction

Second language (L2) pragmatics is an interdisciplinary field involving pragmatics and L2 research. Pragmatics is a key domain in linguistics; there are many published definitions, but the most commonly adopted one in L2 pragmatics was provided by Reference CrystalCrystal (1997: 301), who defined it as “the study of language from the point of view of users, especially of the choices they make, the constraints they encounter in using language in social interaction and the effects their use of language has on other participants in the act of communication.” This definition highlights that both speaker meaning and recipient uptake should be included in pragmatics research (Reference Kasper, Ross, Ross and KasperKasper and Ross, 2013). Pragmatics is generally distinguished into pragmalinguistics and sociopragmatics, with the former addressing the relations between linguistic forms and their function, while the latter addresses the relations between linguistic choices and social constraints (Reference Ren, Sanz and Morales-FrontRen, 2018a). Following the convention in applied linguistics and L2 pragmatics (Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose, 2002; Reference Ross and KasperRoss and Kasper, 2013; Reference Taguchi and RoeverTaguchi and Roever, 2017), in this Element I use L2 as a cover term to refer to an additional language of any status, whether foreign, second, third, fourth, heritage language, or lingua franca.

Second language pragmatics investigates learners’ pragmatic competence, which is defined by Reference ThomasThomas (1983: 92) as “the ability to use language effectively in order to achieve a specific purpose and to understand language in context.” Although many L2 pragmatics studies have examined pragmatic competence from a stable perspective in terms of individuals’ abilities (see reviews in Reference Kasper, Bardovi-Harlig, Felix-Brasdefer and OmarKasper, 2006; Reference Taguchi, Barron, Gu and SteenTaguchi, 2017, Reference Taguchi and Taguchi2019), Thomas’ definition actually points to both speaker and hearer meaning in interaction. Nevertheless, it neglects other semiotic and multimodal resources that are used in interaction, such as emoticons and emojis in digital communication. To reflect this, I propose a downgrading of the language part in Thomas’ definition and the addition of an appropriateness dimension, thus defining pragmatic competence as the ability to use linguistic, semiotic, and multimodal resources effectively and appropriately to achieve a communicative purpose, and to understand such uses in interaction. It has to be explained that language is also a semiotic resource, but here I would like to highlight it and therefore use linguistic and semiotic in parallel. I agree with Reference Taguchi and TaguchiTaguchi (2019) that pragmatic competence is best understood as a multi-dimensional and multi-layered construct that entails three main components: “(1) linguistic and sociocultural knowledge of what forms to use in what context; (2) interactional abilities to use the knowledge in a flexible, adaptive manner corresponding to changing context; and (3) agency to make an informed decision on whether or not to implement the knowledge in the community” (Reference Taguchi and TaguchiTaguchi, 2019: 4). However, I would like to add other semiotic and multimodal knowledge to the first component.

Although many studies focus on acquisitional aspects in L2 pragmatics (e.g., Reference González-Lloret, Schneider and IfantidouGonzález-Lloret, 2020; Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose, 2002; Reference Ren, Sanz and Morales-FrontRen, 2018a), the field is in fact much broader, consisting of pragmatic learning, pragmatic teaching, and pragmatic assessing (see Figure 1).

Figure 1 Branches of L2 pragmatics

The main aims of this Element are twofold. First, it will provide an up-to-date overview of the current research on learning, teaching, and assessing L2 pragmatics, as well as learners’ cognitive processes during such procedures. However, I do not intend to provide a diachronic overview of the history of the field, but rather to focus on some of its key topics. In addition, when reviewing existing works I have deliberately skewed towards publications in this century. Second, it will explore topics that need particular attention in L2 pragmatics to develop the field both in scope and in depth, in turn to reflect and promote the advancement of its two higher-order fields: pragmatics and L2 research. I also showcase two projects in progress before discussing the future directions and suggestions.

2 Second Language Pragmatics Learning

This section introduces works investigating learners’ L2 pragmatic learning, starting with pragmatic production and followed by pragmatic perception. Research modality in L2 pragmatics learning studies is also examined in this section.

2.1 Pragmatic Production

Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose (2002: 117) pointed out that L2 pragmatics was “heavily outweighed by the proliferation of studies on pragmatic production.” This trend remains and reflects the preference for production in pragmatics in general. The skew towards production is understandable, since production can provide more nuanced and easily observed data than perception, and in many cases production involves perception of the other interlocutor’s communication.

Production studies often investigate learners’ production across different influential factors, such as L2 proficiency, learning environment (study abroad vs. study at home), and length of stay abroad. They have yielded controversial and sometimes contradictory findings with respect to the impact of the factors on learners’ pragmatic production, so that our understanding of the effects of these influential factors is incomplete. For instance, partially because different pragmatic targets are examined in different studies, the findings relating to the association between pragmatic competence and linguistic proficiency are often inconsistent. Although most researchers agree that learners’ pragmatic production and their linguistic proficiency do not show a linear correlation, generalization becomes difficult when studies do not employ standardized tests for determining learners’ proficiency levels. Another example is the common finding that learners perform differently from native speakers. However, since a study can only focus on a small cohort of participants, it cannot investigate multiple factors that may influence learners’ pragmatic learning. Given that the number of influencing factors and the way in which they may interact are often different for different learners, it is very complex to compare the findings. For example, Reference Takahashi and BeebeTakahashi and Beebe (1987) found that learners who had studied abroad for an average of four years had a broader range of expressions and more flexibility to adjust their level of directness according to different situations than those who had studied abroad for an average of seven months. Reference BellaBella (2011) demonstrated that even learners with a 4.5-year residence-abroad experience still displayed an underdeveloped pragmatic ability in relation to mitigation devices. Reference RenRen (2019b) found that with respect to request strategies, learners with two years of study-abroad experience resembled native speakers more, while learners with longer study-abroad experience of two to four years showed more non-target-like development. Therefore, this section focuses on what L2 pragmatics production research has investigated, rather than detailed findings in particular areas.

2.1.1 Speech Acts

Second language pragmatics production has mainly investigated speech acts (Reference Bardovi-Harlig, Tyler, Takada, Kim and MarinovaBardovi-Harlig, 2005; Reference Kasper and DahlKasper and Dahl, 1991; Reference Ren, Sanz and Morales-FrontRen, 2018a). However, this does not mean that speech acts are sufficiently understood. Indeed, only a couple of speech acts have attracted much attention, with many more being only occasionally explored or even neglected altogether.

Requests. Requests are the most widely investigated speech act in L2 pragmatics research. Among the target languages, English is the most frequently examined (e.g., Reference AchibaAchiba, 2003; Economidou-Kogetsidis and Reference Woodfield, Felix-Brasdefer and KoikeWoodfield, 2012; Reference SchauerSchauer, 2009), receiving much more attention than any other language. Spanish seems to be the second most frequently examined target language (e.g., Reference Czerwionka and CuzaCzerwionka and Cuza, 2017; Reference KuriscakKuriscak, 2015; Reference ShivelyShively, 2011), followed by Arabic (e.g., Reference Al MasaeedAl Masaeed, 2017; Reference Al-Gahtani and RoeverAl-Gahtani and Roever, 2012), Chinese (e.g., Reference LiLi, 2014; Reference RenRen, 2019b), and Greek (e.g., Reference BellaBella, 2012b; Reference Vassilaki and SelimisVassilaki and Selimis, 2020). German (e.g., Reference BarronBarron, 2003; Reference CunninghamCunningham, 2017), Japanese (e.g., Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2015a), French (e.g., Reference Lundell and ErmanLundell and Erman, 2012), and Indonesian (e.g., Reference HassallHassall, 2003) have also been examined in request production studies, but not frequently.

Many studies examine learners from diverse first language (L1) backgrounds. When learners are from a homogenous L1 background, American English learners appear to be the most frequently examined (e.g., Reference LiLi, 2014; Reference ShivelyShively, 2011; Reference Su and RenSu and Ren, 2017), followed by Japanese learners (e.g., Reference Economidou-Kogetsidis and HalenkoEconomidou-Kogetsidis and Halenko, 2022; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2007b; Reference TakahashiTakahashi, 1996) and Chinese learners (e.g., Reference LiLi, 2000; Reference SuSu, 2010). Learners from other languages have also been examined, but less frequently, including Arabic (e.g., Reference Al-Ali and AlawnehAl-Ali and Alawneh, 2010), French (e.g., Reference BéalBéal, 1994), German (e.g., Reference SchauerSchauer, 2009), Persian (e.g., Reference Ghavamnia, Tavakoli and RezazadehGhavamnia et al., 2012), Serbian (e.g., Reference SavicSavic, 2014), and Swedish (e.g., Reference Lundell and ErmanLundell and Erman, 2012).

The studies have examined learners’ production of request strategies, and their external and internal modification of requests. The most commonly used coding scheme was developed by Reference Blum-Kulka, House and KasperBlum-Kulka et al. (1989) in the well-known Cross-cultural Speech Act Realization Project (CCSARP), although it is often used with adaptations. Request strategies are coded in terms of levels of directness in the head act, which are divided into direct, conventionally indirect, and nonconventionally indirect requests. External and internal modification refer to pragmatic strategies which mitigate or intensify the request but do not in themselves carry the request force, with the former falling out of the head act while the latter within the head act (full details can be found in the CCSARP coding manual in Reference Blum-Kulka, House and KasperBlum-Kulka et al., 1989).

Refusals. Refusals may be the second most investigated speech act in L2 pragmatics production research, although the number of studies on refusals is much lower than the number on requests. Refusal studies predominantly focus on English as the target language (e.g., Reference Al-Gahtani and RoeverAl-Gahtani and Roever, 2018; Reference RenRen, 2015; Reference Taguchi, Martí-Arnándiz and Salazar-CampilloTaguchi, 2013b), with only a few studies investigating other languages such as Arabic (e.g., Reference MorkusMorkus, 2021), Greek (e.g., Reference BellaBella, 2011, Reference Bella2014b), Hebrew (e.g., Reference Stavans and Webman ShafranStavans and Webman Shafran, 2018), Chinese (e.g., Reference HongHong, 2011), and Spanish (e.g., Reference Félix-BrasdeferFélix-Brasdefer, 2003, Reference Félix-Brasdefer2004, Reference Félix-Brasdefer, Martí-Arnándiz and Salazar-Campillo2013).

Studies on L2 refusals often examine learners from homogenous L1 backgrounds, although some studies do include learners with a mixture of L1s (e.g., Reference Bardovi-Harlig and HartfordBardovi-Harlig and Hartford, 1993). Interestingly, this research shows a different trend from request studies. When English is the target language, Chinese learners are the most frequently examined (e.g., Reference ChangChang, 2011; Reference LeeLee, 2016; Reference RenRen, 2013), followed by Iranian learners (L1 Persian) (e.g., Reference Allami and NaeimiAllami and Naeimi, 2011; Reference Shishavan and SharifianShishavan and Sharifian, 2013), Saudi Arabic learners (e.g., Reference Al-Gahtani and RoeverAl-Gahtani and Roever, 2018), Japanese learners (e.g., Reference Gass and HouckGass and Houck, 1999; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2007b), Javanese learners (e.g., Reference WijayantoWijayanto, 2016), and Thai learners (e.g., Reference WannarukWannaruk, 2008). When languages other than English are the focal target languages, the learners are almost always from English-speaking countries.

Most studies investigate refusal strategies and adjuncts to refusals (i.e., external modification), following the coding scheme developed by Reference Beebe, Takahashi, Uliss-Weltz, Scarcella, Anderson and KrashenBeebe et al. (1990). Unlike request studies that often analyze internal modification, only a few studies have examined the internal modification of refusals (e.g., Reference BarronBarron, 2003; Reference BellaBella, 2011; Reference RenRen, 2013). This may be due in part to two reasons. On the one hand, the speech act of refusal is often realized in indirect strategies, which increases the difficulties and complexities involved in identifying internal modifiers. On the other hand, there is no well-accepted coding scheme for the internal modifiers of refusals, as there is for studies on requests.

Apology. Apology is another often-examined speech act in L2 pragmatic production, although it is examined much less frequently than requests. Studies in this area either investigate how English L1 speakers apologize in a target language, such as Arabic (e.g., Reference Al Masaeed, Waugh and BurnsAl Masaeed et al., 2018), German (e.g., Reference BarronBarron, 2019), and Russian (e.g., Reference ShardakovaShardakova, 2005), or how learners from other languages apologize in English, for example, Catalan learners (e.g., Reference Barón and OrtegaBarón and Ortega, 2018), Chinese learners (e.g., Reference ChangChang, 2010), Saudi Arabic learners (e.g., Reference El-DakhsEl-Dakhs, 2018), and Serbian learners (e.g., Reference SavicSavic, 2014). Only Reference Warga and SchölmbergerWarga and Schölmberger (2007) diverge from this trend, examining Austrian German learners’ apologies in French.

These studies not only investigate learners’ apology strategies but also how they upgrade their apologies, based on earlier coding schemes for apologies (e.g., Reference Blum-Kulka, House and KasperBlum-Kulka et al., 1989; Reference Olshtain, Blum-Kulka, House and KasperOlshtain, 1989). The notion of the illocutionary force indicating device (IFID) is important for coding apologies, which is the formulaic form containing “the explicit, performative verbs which express an apology” (Reference Olshtain, Blum-Kulka, House and KasperOlshtain, 1989: 157). The IFID can be strengthened internally by various means such as intensifying adverbials and emotional expressions (see more in the CCSARP coding manual in Reference Blum-Kulka, House and KasperBlum-Kulka et al., 1989).

Other speech acts. More speech acts have been investigated, but most of them have only been examined sporadically. Many have only been investigated with English as the target language. For example, learners’ production of advice/suggestions has been explored in a few studies, all focusing on English (e.g., Reference Bardovi-Harlig and HartfordBardovi-Harlig and Hartford, 1993). Among these, Chinese learners seem to be the most frequently examined (e.g., Reference HinkelHinkel, 1997; Reference LiLi, 2010; Reference Taguchi, Fernández and JiangTaguchi et al., 2021), and Japanese learners have also been studied (e.g., Reference MatsumuraMatsumura, 2001, Reference Matsumura2003).

Further speech acts that have been investigated with English and other languages as the target language include complaints (e.g., Reference Nguyen, Pham, Economidou-Kogetsidis, Savić and HalenkoNguyen and Pham, 2021; Reference TatsukiTatsuki, 2000; Reference Yuan and ZhangYuan and Zhang, 2018), compliment responses (e.g., Reference HuthHuth, 2006; Reference Shimizu and TaguchiShimizu, 2009), criticisms (e.g., Reference NguyenNguyen, 2008, Reference Nguyen2017), greeting responses (e.g., Reference JaworskiJaworski, 1994; Reference Ying and RenYing and Ren, 2021), offers (e.g., Reference BellaBella, 2016), and sympathy after bereavement (e.g., Reference Meiners, Parvaresh and CaponeMeiners, 2017). The studies have enlarged our understanding of learners’ acquisition of speech acts, addressing the call for investigating more speech acts and expanding the pool of target languages. However, the findings would be more mutually inferable if the studies could be guided by similar frameworks of speech act taxonomy and employ some standardized measurements for determining learners’ proficiency. On the other hand, it is beneficial for L2 pragmatics to follow the trend of pragmatics research to investigate speech acts in a bigger picture of interaction in a more dynamic manner. These achieved, L2 pragmatics can draw more robust generalizations and compare its achievement with other branches of pragmatics and L2 research to contribute more to theoretical constructions of the two fields. It is encouraging to see that several studies have explored advanced learners’ uses of speech acts to express their identities and enhance their social relations. For example, Reference HabibHabib (2008) examines how four female near-native users of English employ teasing and disagreement to display personal identity, while Reference Ren, Liu, Economidou-Kogetsidis, Savic and HalenkoRen and Liu (2021) explore how Chinese graduate learners of English express their phatic communion in gratitude emails to professors.

2.1.2 Conventional Expressions

Some studies have investigated learners’ production of formulaic expressions. Although different scholars use different terms for these, such as conventional expressions (Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2009), formulas (Reference Bardovi-Harlig and HanBardovi-Harlig, 2008), and pragmatic routines (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2013a), the terms are often used interchangeably and refer to almost the same concept, that is, of idiomatic multi-word expressions (see Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2012 for a review of the differences among the terms).

Empirical studies often examine both learners’ production and recognition of L2 conventional expressions. Production studies have mostly investigated English conventional expressions, apart from a couple of studies on Chinese (e.g., Reference Bardovi-Harlig and SuBardovi-Harlig and Su, 2018; Reference Taguchi, Li and XiaoTaguchi et al., 2013). Although conventional expressions have been discussed in other languages (e.g., Reference KecskesKecskes, 2003), few empirical studies have been conducted.

Production research on conventional expressions generally undergoes two preparatory phases prior to collecting data from learners. In the first phase, researchers select candidate formulaic expressions by various methods, including authentic conversations (Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2009), field notes (Reference Taguchi, Li and XiaoTaguchi et al., 2013), discourse completion tasks (DCTs) (Reference EdmondsEdmonds, 2014), or textbooks and reference works (Reference Taguchi, Li and XiaoTaguchi et al., 2013; Reference YangYang, 2016). The candidate expressions are then tested with a group of native speakers to decide the “correct” answer that will be used to test learners. Through this checking process the research establishes criteria for the formulae using a cut-off point. Most research chooses a 50 percent cut-off for native speaker preference (e.g., Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2009; Reference Bardovi-Harlig and SuBardovi-Harlig and Su, 2018; Reference Taguchi, Li and XiaoTaguchi et al., 2013), although Reference YangYang (2016) employs a 67 percent cut-off.

2.1.3 Address Terms

Address terms express sociopragmatic values and affect interpersonal relationships. They are often examined as a type of modification in L2 pragmatics research, either as an alerter (e.g., Reference RenRen, 2019b; Reference Taguchi, Xiao and LiTaguchi et al., 2016b) and attention-getter (Reference Martínez-Flor, Economidou-Kogetsidis and 70WoodfieldMartínez-Flor, 2012), or as a type of internal modifier (Reference Hassall, Economidou-Kogetsidis and WoodfieldHassall, 2012; Reference Pan, Economidou-Kogetsidis and WoodfieldPan, 2012). However, this section focuses on studies taking address terms as their specific research targets.

The production of address terms in L2 has been investigated in a range of languages. The most often examined types are the T/V address forms in French (tu vs. vous), German (du vs. sie), and Spanish ( vs. usted) (e.g., Reference BloodBlood, 2018; Reference Kinginger and BelzKinginger and Belz, 2005; Reference VillarrealVillarreal, 2014). Studies have also examined learner use of address terms in Korean (e.g., Reference Brown and KingingerBrown, 2013; Reference Kim and BrownKim and Brown, 2014) and Indonesian (e.g., Reference HassallHassall, 2013). Studies have explored the difficulties of and the study-abroad effect on English learners’ use of address forms in another language, with findings generally indicating a positive effect of study abroad and learners’ agency in using those address terms (Reference Ishihara and TaguchiIshihara, 2019).

2.1.4 Pragmatic Markers

Pragmatic markers have attracted increasing attention in L2 pragmatics research, with researchers investigating the frequency and functions of learners’ production of one or a group of pragmatic markers. Not surprisingly, English pragmatic markers constitute the majority of the targets, among which like is the most investigated (e.g., Reference LiuLiu, 2016; Reference Magliacane and HowardMagliacane and Howard, 2019), followed by well, you know, and I mean (e.g., Reference BuysseBuysse, 2015; Reference Fernández-PoloFernández-Polo, 2014; Reference WeiWei, 2011). Other English pragmatic markers that have been examined include so, I don’t know, and, just, sort of/kind of, I think, yes/yeah, and please (e.g., Reference BuysseBuysse, 2012; Reference Gablasova, Brezina, Mcenery and BoydGablasova et al., 2017; Reference Hosoda and AlineHosoda and Aline, 2022).

Studies have also examined pragmatic markers in L2 Spanish, such as pues (close to well, so, then) and bueno (close to anyway, okay, so, well), and the turn-initial discourse markers y and (Reference García GarcíaGarcía García, 2021), Italian, such as (yes, ok), allora (so, then), quindi (then, therefore), and dunque (therefore, be, well) (Reference De Cristofaro and BadanDe Cristofaro and Badan, 2021), and Japanese, such as the contrastive markers demo, kedo, and ga (all can be translated into English as but, though, although) (Reference GeyerGeyer, 2007). Chinese markers have also been examined, mostly focusing on utterance-final particles (e.g., Reference DiaoDiao, 2016; Reference Diao and ChenDiao and Chen, 2021).

2.1.5 Conversation Management

Studies on learners’ abilities to manage conversations investigate a range of topics under the framework of conversation analysis (Reference SchegloffSchegloff, 2007; Reference SidnellSidnell, 2010), such as repair, sequential organization, turn taking, turn design, and preference organization (Reference Kasper, Ross, Ross and KasperKasper and Ross, 2013). For example, Reference ItakuraItakura (2002) examines the effect of gender on topic development in informal conversations by Japanese learners of English, Reference Al-Gahtani and RoeverAl-Gahtani and Roever (2012) analyze the sequential organizations of requests of Arabic learners of English across four proficiency levels, and Reference Al-Gahtani and RoeverAl-Gahtani and Roever (2018) examine preference organization of Arabic learners of English across three proficiency levels. In addition, Reference Savić, Đorđević, Economidou-Kogetsidis, Savić and HalenkoSavić and Đorđević (2021) investigate relational practices in emails in English by Norwegian speakers.

Languages other than English have also been studied. Reference GuillotGuillot (2009, Reference Guillot2012) investigates interruption and overlaps in English learners’ L2 French talk, while Reference HuthHuth (2006) examines English learners’ sequence organization during compliment response in L2 German. Reference ShivelyShively (2015) analyzes English learners’ listener responses in L2 Spanish conversations. Preference structure is often examined in requests among learners across different proficiency levels – for example, in L2 Arabic (Reference Al-Gahtani and RoeverAl-Gahtani and Roever, 2014), L2 German (Reference Taleghani-Nikazm and HuthTaleghani-Nikazm and Huth, 2010), and L2 Chinese (Reference Su and RenSu and Ren, 2017).

2.1.6 Prosody

Prosodic features may influence pragmatic meaning. However, despite the importance of prosody in conveying meaning, it has been neglected in L2 pragmatics (see Reference Kang, Kermad and TaguchiKang and Kermad, 2019 for an overview). This lack of research on prosody in L2 pragmatic production may be due to “the lack of a systematic analytical methodology” (Reference Romero-TrilloRomero-Trillo, 2019: 91). Therefore, Reference Romero-TrilloRomero-Trillo (2019) proposes an approach to explore the prosodic aspects of pragmatic markers as feedback produced by native and non-native speakers in corpus pragmatics.

Reference Verdugo and Romero-TrilloVerdugo and Romero-Trillo (2005) carried out one of the few studies that compares the intonation in reading tag questions between Spanish learners of English and native speakers of English. The results revealed that the learners and the native speakers used a similar rising intonation pattern in questioning tags, but, in confirmation tags, while the native speakers used an unmarked tone to indicate certainty and demand agreement, the learners again used a rising intonation. The authors concluded that a lack of pragmatic knowledge and awareness of the functions of tag questions led to the Spanish learners’ overuse of a rising intonation.

2.1.7 Concluding Remarks

In summary, pragmatic production studies have investigated a variety of pragmatic features, although with different degrees of attention. However, the studies predominantly examine learners at university level. An underdeveloped area concerns investigation of learners of different ages. Reference RoseRose (2000) investigated pragmatic development of requests, apologies, and compliment responses among three groups of primary school EFL (English as a Foreign Language) learners in Hong Kong (aged approximately 7, 9, and 11). Reference Barón Parés, Amaya, López and MorónBarón Parés (2012) examined request development in Catalan-Spanish bilingual EFL learners aged 10, 12, 16, and 18–19. Reference Savić, Economidou-Kogetsidis and MyrsetSavić et al. (2021) investigated the request production of young EFL learners in Greek Cypriot and Norwegian aged 9, 11, and 13. More acquisition studies are needed to explore the pragmatic development of learners across different ages.

More importantly, pragmatic production studies tend to focus on nuanced data coding but lack a solid theoretical orientation. It is often documented that learners may underuse or overuse certain pragmatic features compared with native speakers. However, studies should also explore why such divergence exists by investigating a multiplicity of factors simultaneously, particularly concerning the pragmatic norms of L1 and L2, learners’ L2 proficiency, and pragmatic agency.

2.2 Pragmatic Perception

Studies on learner pragmatic perception can be roughly divided into two categories, namely pragmatic awareness and pragmatic comprehension, which will be reviewed in the following two subsections respectively.

2.2.1 Pragmatic Awareness

Pragmatic awareness refers to learners’ conscious knowledge or evaluation of certain pragmatic features or practice. This line of research mainly investigates learners’ ability to detect pragmatically (in)appropriate language uses and reflexivity on pragmatic phenomena. The following sections will introduce the key themes investigated in the pragmatic awareness literature.

Appropriateness of Speech Acts

Reference Bardovi-Harlig and DörnyeiBardovi-Harlig and Dörnyei (1998) pioneered investigation into the relationship between pragmatic awareness and grammatical awareness. They focused on awareness in the sense of noticing in Schmidt’s noticing hypothesis (Reference Bardovi-Harlig and Hartford1993, Reference Brown, Rose and Kasper2001), which refers to “registering the simple occurrence of some event” (Reference Schmidt, Kasper and Blum-KulkaSchmidt, 1993: 26). They compared EFL learners in Hungary with ESL (English as a Second Language) learners in the United States using a video-prompted task they developed, which included refusals, apologies, requests, and suggestions. The task required participants to distinguish grammatical errors and pragmatic infelicitous items, and to rate both for severity. The findings indicated that both learning contexts and L2 proficiency influenced the learners’ performance, and study-abroad length also played an important role in developing learner pragmatic awareness.

Bardovi-Harlig and Dörnyei’s seminal work was replicated in many different contexts, sometimes yielding different results. Also using the video-prompted task, Reference SchauerSchauer (2009) investigated German learners’ pragmatic awareness and found that the learners increased their pragmatic awareness significantly during study abroad in the UK. In contrast, Reference Niezgoda, Roever, Rose and KasperNiezgoda and Roever (2001) compared EFL learners in the Czech Republic with ESL learners at a US language school; using the same video-prompted instrument, they found that overall the EFL learners outperformed the ESL learners at error identification and severity rating for both grammatical and pragmatic items. Study-abroad length had a clear effect on learners’ pragmatic awareness, but L2 proficiency did not have an impact. Surprisingly, however, the longer the learners stayed in the target community, the more tolerant they were to pragmatic infelicities.

Some studies adapted the video-prompted task developed by Reference Bardovi-Harlig and DörnyeiBardovi-Harlig and Dörnyei (1998) into questionnaires. For example, Reference Xu, Case and WangXu et al. (2009) used a paper-and-pencil version of the conversations and investigated learners with a mix of L1s living in the United States with different study-abroad lengths and L2 proficiency levels. Reference RenRen (2015) compared the development of pragmatic awareness among EFL learners and ESL learners, only focusing on pragmatic items. Ren did not ask the learners to detect whether an item was infelicitous; instead, he asked the learners to directly evaluate the appropriateness of the item. Some researchers translated the questionnaire into other languages to explore learner pragmatic awareness in different target languages; Reference RodriguezRodriguez (2001) used the questionnaire in Spanish to investigate the effect of study abroad in Spain on learners’ awareness of request strategies. The participants were asked to decide whether utterances were appropriate, and if not, to rate how inappropriate they were using a four-point scale with no neutral point. Reference BellaBella (2012a) adapted the questionnaire to investigate the effect of residence length in Greece on learners’ pragmatic awareness in L2 Greek.

Several researchers have modified Reference Bardovi-Harlig and DörnyeiBardovi-Harlig and Dörnyei’s (1998) methodology in order to fit their own investigations. For example, Reference Yang, Yuan and LiYang (2019) developed fourteen scenarios for expressing gratitude and examined the effect of proficiency on L2 Chinese learners’ pragmatic and grammatical awareness. Reference Bardovi-Harlig and GriffinBardovi-Harlig and Griffin (2005) went a step further by requiring learners not only to detect the inappropriateness in roleplays but also to repair it. Reference Yang and RenYang and Ren (2019) used the pragmatic awareness questionnaire to explore the relationship between pragmatic awareness and Chinese EFL learners’ motivation to learn English, while Reference Lv, Ren and LiLv et al. (2021) translated the questionnaire into Chinese and explored the relationship between willingness to communicate and pragmatic awareness among L2 learners of Chinese with a mix of L1s.

Researchers have also designed other methods to explore learners’ pragmatic awareness. Reference TakahashiTakahashi (2005) investigated whether the pragmatic awareness of Japanese EFL learners was related to their learning motivation and language proficiency. Learners’ awareness of the target pragmalinguistic features, including three forms of request head acts, discourse markers, idiomatic expressions, and non-idiomatic expressions, was assessed using a seven-point Likert scale questionnaire. The findings indicated that the learners’ pragmatic awareness correlated with their intrinsic motivation but not with their L2 proficiency. Reference Economidou-KogetsidisEconomidou-Kogetsidis (2016) investigated differences in the evaluations of the (im)politeness of student email requests and the personality of the sender between English lecturers and Greek-Cypriot L2 university students. The learners were found to be unaware of the negative consequences of an inappropriate academic email, as they seemed to equate politeness with formality. The author pointed out that the learners viewed the student–faculty emails as purely transactional, while the English lecturers viewed them as both transactional and interactional.

Conventional Expressions

A number of perception studies have explored learners’ pragmatic awareness of conventional expressions. Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig (2009) examined the production and recognition of conventional expressions among English learners with mixed L1s through an oral production task and an aural recognition task. Recognition was detected by a three-level familiarity estimate in the form “I often/sometimes/never hear this”. The results showed that recognition of conventional expressions was a necessary but not sufficient condition for production.

Using the same dataset, Reference Bardovi-Harlig and BastosBardovi-Harlig and Bastos (2011) investigated the effects of proficiency, length of study abroad, and intensity of interaction on L2 English learners’ recognition and production of conventional expressions. Only intensity of interaction was found to have a significant influence on the recognition of conventional expressions. However, it is worth noting that the majority of the learners in Reference Bardovi-Harlig and BastosBardovi-Harlig and Bastos (2011) had studied abroad in the United States for less than eight months. The relatively narrow range of study-abroad length may have limited the benefit of study abroad (Reference Taguchi and LiTaguchi and Li, 2019).

Based on her earlier work (Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2009), Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig (2014) investigated L2 learners’ awareness of the meaning of conventional expressions in English using an aural Vocabulary Knowledge Scale questionnaire. It was found that the learners’ awareness of the meaning of the conventional expressions influenced their uses of such expressions. In addition, it was likely that the learners acquired the form of a conventional expression first, gradually grasping its plausible meaning and refining it rather than acquiring the form and correct meaning at the same time. Reference Alcón Soler and Sánchez-HernándezAlcón Soler and Sánchez-Hernández (2017) compared the development of learners’ production and recognition of conventional expressions during study abroad. They found that the ability to recognize the conventional expressions seemed to be a first step for the latter acquisition of the productive ability, echoing the findings in Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig (2014). Also using a Vocabulary Knowledge Scale questionnaire, Reference Sánchez-Hernández and Alcón SolerSánchez-Hernández and Alcón Soler (2019) explored the development of Brazilian learners of English in terms of their recognition of pragmatic routines during study abroad, and examined how sociocultural adaptation and intensity of interaction influenced their development. The findings revealed that the learners’ recognition of pragmatic routines significantly improved after a semester study abroad, influenced by both sociocultural adaptation and intensity of interaction. L2 exposure and learners’ willingness to socialize in the target environment were determinant factors in recognizing pragmatic routines.

A couple of studies have investigated learners’ pragmatic awareness of conventional expressions in L2 French. Reference Kinginger, Blattner, Ortega and ByrnesKinginger and Blattner (2008) reported on three L2 French learners’ awareness of colloquial phrases during a study-abroad semester in France. In a language awareness interview, participants were asked to comment on their knowledge of the use of French colloquial phrases. The findings suggested that the learners developed their awareness of sociopragmatic variability during study abroad, and their performance was related to their identity and their participation in French-speaking contexts. Reference EdmondsEdmonds (2014) investigated the effect of study-abroad length in France on L2 French learners’ evaluation of naturalness of conventional expressions across a long-stay group (over a year) and a short-stay group (four to six months). The results showed that as with native speakers, the two groups of learners judged the conventional expressions similarly and significantly different from grammatical but not from conventional strings. However, native speakers could detect conventional expressions more quickly than nonconventional expressions, but the learners’ reaction times for the two types were not significantly different.

Address Terms

Similar to production studies, research on learners’ pragmatic awareness of address terms has tended to investigate languages other than English. Reference Kinginger and FarrellKinginger and Farrell (2004) focused on the development of eight American French learners’ sociopragmatic awareness of French address terms (tu vs. vous) using language awareness interviews, biweekly journals, and observer field notes. In the interviews the learners were presented with a series of six social situations illustrating different parameters influencing address term use, for example, setting, age, and familiarity of interlocutor, and were asked to choose an address term for each of the situations and explain the rationales underlying their choices. The findings showed that the learners developed their awareness of French address terms but the development did not relate to their linguistic competence.

Reference VillarrealVillarreal (2014) investigated pragmatic production and awareness of Spanish address forms ( vs. usted) among English learners of Spanish. Data were collected using roleplays and metapragmatic judgment questionnaires. Analysis of the judgment questionnaire revealed that the learners were aware of the indexical function of the second person address forms, although some learners failed to consistently produce their desired address forms. Finally, Reference BloodBlood (2018) examined the pragmatic awareness of Australian learners of German with respect to the second-person pronoun du during a six-week language course in Germany. The findings revealed that even a short length of study abroad, such as six weeks in this study, could facilitate improvement in learners’ pragmatic awareness of address terms.

2.2.2 Pragmatic Comprehension

Research on pragmatic comprehension investigates learners’ abilities to understand the implied meaning of an utterance, which may be different to what is said. Studies in this area generally employ multiple-choice questionnaires to examine learners’ understanding of a range of pragmatic aspects.

Comprehension of Conversation Implicature

Studies frequently compare learners’ abilities to infer different types of conversation implicature. Reference BoutonBouton (1994) was one of the first to investigate the effect of study-abroad length on learners’ comprehension of implicatures, showing that learners became more proficient in their interpretation of implicatures to a statistically significant degree. Nevertheless, even after seventeen months of study-abroad experience, the learners still had difficulties in comprehending understated criticism, sequence implicatures, and Pope Q implicature (this type of implicature answers one question with another highly formulaic question, for example, “Is the Pope Catholic?”), which did not consistently cause trouble for learners with over 4.5 years of study-abroad experience. Reference Cook and LiddicoatCook and Liddicoat (2002) investigated the effect of proficiency on L2 English learners’ comprehension of request strategies, revealing significant differences in the comprehension accuracy of request types between two learner groups (low vs. high proficiency), with the high proficiency learners providing a significantly higher number of interpretations of conventional and nonconventional indirect requests than the low proficiency learners.

A series of studies were conducted to investigate learner comprehension of indirect meaning. For example, Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2005) investigated the effect of proficiency on L2 learners’ comprehension of indirect utterances in dialogues in terms of accuracy and speed of comprehension. Participants were asked to complete a computerized listening task consisting of two types of conversation implicatures that were different in terms of their conventionality (conventionally indirect request or refusal vs. idiosyncratic implicature). The results revealed that the learners processed the more conventional utterances more quickly and more accurately than the less conventional utterances. In contrast, native speakers could process the two types almost equally in terms of accuracy and speed. These findings suggested a significant effect of L2 proficiency on learners’ comprehension accuracy but not on comprehension speed for both types of implied meaning. Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2008b, Reference Taguchi and Taguchi2009a) also found a proficiency effect on comprehension accuracy but not on speed in L2 Japanese learners’ comprehension of indirect refusals and indirect opinions (conventional vs. nonconventional). Both studies found that refusals were the easiest to comprehend, followed by conventional and nonconventional indirect opinions.

Taguchi also examined the development of the comprehension of implied refusals and opinions among Japanese EFL (Reference Taguchi2007a) and ESL (Reference Taguchi2008a) learners at beginner level. The results showed that the Japanese EFL learners’ comprehension accuracy and speed improved significantly over seven weeks, although comprehension speed increased much less compared to the accuracy of comprehension (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2007a). The Japanese ESL learners made significant progress in terms of comprehension speed, but showed little improvement in comprehension accuracy over four months (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2008a). The development of comprehension accuracy did not correlate with the amount of language contact for any skills (speaking, listening, reading, writing), whereas the development of comprehension speed correlated significantly with the amount of speaking and reading.

To further explore the effect of learning environment on learners’ pragmatic comprehension of indirect refusals and indirect opinions, Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2008c) compared Japanese EFL and ESL learners at the beginner level over seven weeks. The results showed that both groups’ comprehension accuracy and speed improved significantly over time, suggesting that pragmatic comprehension developed over time regardless of the learning environment. Comparisons of effect sizes revealed that for the EFL group, the magnitude of effect was much less for speed than for accuracy, whereas the ESL group showed a significant improvement in speed but only marginal improvement in accuracy. The EFL group showed a significantly greater improvement compared to the ESL group in the comprehension accuracy of indirect refusals but not of indirect opinions, indicating that “different environments may support different aspects of pragmatic comprehension ability” (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2008c: 444). In another study adopting a similar design, Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2011a) examined the effect of proficiency and study-abroad experience on Japanese English learners’ pragmatic comprehension of indirect refusals and routines. The results of this study indicated a significant effect of proficiency, but no effect of study-abroad experience, on the learners’ comprehension speed. Both proficiency and study-abroad experience had significant effects on the learners’ comprehension accuracy, but separate analyses of indirect refusals and routines revealed a significant effect of study abroad on the learners’ comprehension accuracy of routines but not indirect refusals.

Reference KöylüKöylü (2018) moved a step further by asking learners to orally report their inferences of conversational implicatures in English in an audio-visual interpretation task. The results showed that the learners’ comprehension of conversational implicatures had a positive correlation with their L2 proficiency. Reference Chen and LinChen and Lin (2021) used multiple-choice DCTs to compare Chinese EFL learners’ comprehension of conversational implicatures between individual and collaborative work. They found that the individual and collaborative learners performed similarly.

A couple of studies have explored learners’ abilities to determine the illocutionary force of indirect speech acts. Reference GarciaGarcia (2004) examined L2 learners across low and high proficiency levels in terms of their comprehension of nonconventional indirect speech acts and detection of the illocutionary force of an utterance, using a four-option multiple-choice task: requesting, offering, suggesting, and correcting. The results showed that the high proficiency learners were more successful than the low proficiency learners, reflecting a positive effect of proficiency on speech act recognition. Linguistic features such as explicit agents and modal use helped the learners to detect the illocutionary force of a nonconventional indirect speech act. Meanwhile Reference HoltgravesHoltgraves (2007) pursued this issue from a different angle, exploring whether learners could automatically recognize the illocutionary force when they read indirect speech act utterances. Participants were asked to read conversational utterances and perform a lexical decision task to determine whether a target string of letters on a screen was a word. It was found that native speakers performed the task more quickly when the string was the speech act associated with the preceding utterance, but learners did not show any difference, suggesting that they could not yet activate automatic comprehension of speech acts.

Although the above studies all explored conversation implicatures in English, some studies focused on L2 Spanish to examine learners’ comprehension of implicature. For example, Reference Koike, Gass and NeuKoike (1996) investigated whether learners of Spanish across various proficiency levels could understand the speech act of suggestion. Learners were asked to identify the types of speech act expressed in seven video-taped situations, to reproduce the speech acts, and to evaluate the speakers in terms of five aspects: aggressive vs. passive, rude vs. polite, noncommunicative vs. communicative, strong vs. weak, and unfriendly vs. friendly. It was found that two lower-proficiency groups had difficulties in comprehending the intent of the speech acts whereas more advanced learners succeeded in recognizing the suggestions, indicating a clear proficiency effect. Reference Taguchi, Gomez-Laich and Arrufat-MarquesTaguchi et al. (2016a) explored L2 Spanish learners’ comprehension of three types of indirect meaning, indirect refusals, indirect opinions, and irony, in terms of comprehension accuracy and speed. The results showed that the learners comprehended indirect refusals and opinions the same in terms of their accuracy, but they comprehended indirect opinions much faster than indirect refusals and irony, suggesting that conventionality did not facilitate the learners’ pragmatic comprehension. Irony was found to be the most difficult to comprehend among the three types for the L2 Spanish learners; this may be due to the difficulty of comprehending irony in a second language.

Comprehension of Scalar Implicature

Scalar implicature is a particular type of implicature in which an additional meaning is inferred with respect to a gradable scale, for example <some, all>. Reference SlabakovaSlabakova (2010) examined how Korean English learners processed the quantifiers some and all in sentences with and without contexts, with results showing that the learners had no difficulties in interpreting the scalar implicatures. Reference Snape and HosoiSnape and Hosoi (2018) investigated the influence of L2 proficiency on learners’ understanding of the scalar implicatures some and all by examining intermediate and advanced Japanese English learners. This study found no significant differences between either the native speakers and the learners or the two learner groups, indicating that L2 proficiency did not have an effect on the learners’ acquisition of scalar implicatures.

Using oral rather than written stimuli, Reference Mazzaggio, Panizza and SurianMazzaggio et al. (2021) tested Italian university students’ understanding of the scalar implicatures some and all in English and their equivalents in Italian and Spanish. All the learners were tested using utterances spoken in either their L1 Italian or an L2 (English or Spanish). The results showed that learners were less likely to derive an underinformative interpretation in L2 than in L1 condition, suggesting that deriving such pragmatic interpretations was costly and nonautomatic in an L2.

Unlike English, Spanish has two indefinite determiners, unos and algunos, which have nearly identical meanings (close to some in English) but differ in terms of their felicitousness in different contexts (Reference Miller, Giancaspro, Iverson, Fuente, Valenzuela and SanzMiller et al., 2016). Comparing English learners of Spanish at a near-native level with Spanish native speakers, Reference Miller, Giancaspro, Iverson, Fuente, Valenzuela and SanzMiller et al. (2016) found that the advanced English learners were able to achieve native-like judgment of the two Spanish scalar terms.

Comprehension of Irony

Only a few studies have focused on L2 pragmatic comprehension of irony. Reference KimKim (2014) examined how Korean professionals understood sarcasm in spoken English in five video clips of the sitcom Friends. Participants were asked to complete three tasks: identify the instance of sarcasm, interpret the speaker intention and the communicative goal, and report the cues and their thoughts during their detection and comprehension. The majority of the Korean participants reported that they had to use a significant amount of guesswork to complete the tasks, suggesting the difficulty of understanding sarcasm in L2. In addition, the results also revealed that the Korean learners identified English sarcastic utterances and interpreted speaker intention by drawing on their L1 pragmalinguistic and general linguistic knowledge.

Reference Ellis, Zhu, Shintani and RoeverEllis et al. (2021) investigated Chinese university students’ ability to identify irony in English using an irony test. The results showed that the Chinese learners’ scores were much lower than those of native speakers. The learners performed better in identifying negative irony than positive irony, and both L2 proficiency and study-abroad length showed statistically significant relationships with irony recognition, although these relationships were found to be weak.

Comprehension of Humor

Besides irony, humor is also believed to be difficult to grasp in L2. Reference Chen and DewaeleChen and Dewaele (2019) asked Chinese learners of English to rate the funniness and the ease of understanding of two short video extracts containing humorous utterances. The findings showed that there was a significant difference between the Chinese learners and the English native speakers in terms of their funniness and ease of understanding ratings. It was revealed that proficiency and funniness ratings were unrelated at the lower and upper intermediate levels, but related at the advanced level.

Reference Bell, Shardakova, Shively, Félix-Brasdefer and ShivelyBell et al. (2021) developed a paper-and-pencil version of a DCT to solicit learners’ responses to ten scenarios encouraging them to respond with humor. The learners were asked to fill in the DCT and evaluate whether they felt their responses were funny. The results showed that humor target (self vs. other) as well as learners’ L1 and L2 affected their perceptions of humor.

Comprehension of Contextual Cues

Nonlinguistic features such as contextual cues can also influence speakers’ utterance meanings. Reference Nakamura, Arai, Hirose and FlynnNakamura et al. (2020) investigated whether there was an integrative function of contrastive pitch accent and visual contextual cues in assisting the processing of referential ambiguous syntactic structure for English native speakers and Japanese L2 English learners. This study found that a visually presented context was helpful for both the native speakers and the learners in comprehending ambiguous utterances. Prosodic cues helped the native speakers to resolve referential ambiguity with visual cues present, but the learners were unable to integrate prosody with reference to visual cues. Reference Cho and DewaeleCho and Dewaele (2021) investigated Korean learners’ pragmatic difficulties when perceiving English emotional intonation. In this study, the learners were required to rate the valence (positive vs. negative) and intensity (weak vs. strong) of emotional sentences on a nine-point scale. The results indicated that the Korean learners had difficulties in perceiving intentional meanings through pitch as a prosodic cue in English interactions.

Reference IshidaIshida (2006) investigated L2 Japanese learners’ receptive knowledge of contextual cues. Data were collected using stimulated recall, a multiple-choice task, and a five-point rating scale for the importance of the cues. The findings revealed differences between the Japanese native speakers and the L2 Japanese learners in terms of their perception and interpretation of the contextual cues.

2.2.3 Concluding Remarks

Evaluation of the (in)appropriateness of language uses is one of the often-examined issues in pragmatic awareness research. This topic was pioneered by Reference Bardovi-Harlig and DörnyeiBardovi-Harlig and Dörnyei (1998) to examine learners’ ability to detect pragmatically (in)felicitous speech acts, tapping both pragmalinguistic and sociopragmatic knowledge. The methodology was developed more than two decades ago and has been replicated and adapted in many contexts. More innovative designs are needed to further explore this issue. In addition, learners’ reflexivity on conventional expressions has also been investigated, which is examined in terms of familiarity or meanings of such expressions. That is, pragmatic awareness research on conventional expressions focuses more on pragmalinguistic knowledge, while sociopragmatic knowledge is barely touched upon.

Pragmatic comprehension research has covered a range of topics, including conversation implicature, scalar implicature, irony, humor and contextual cues such as pitch and intonation. However, the research would increase its rigour if it were conducted under the theoretical frameworks in pragmatics. For example, the studies divided conventionality of conversation implicature differently from Reference GriceGrice’s (1989) framework on implicature. Future research may want to follow the classification of Reference GriceGrice (1989) to link better L2 pragmatic comprehension literature to general pragmatics. In addition, pragmatic comprehension research is often conducted in a lab setting. Future studies should investigate learners’ pragmatic comprehension in authentic communication. This applies to pragmatic awareness research as well.

2.3 Research Modality

Second language pragmatics learning has predominantly investigated learners’ pragmatic competence in face-to-face oral communication, although many studies collect data using written-for-oral tasks (Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2018) – for example, using written DCTs to explore oral pragmatic competence. Only a couple of studies have examined learners’ written pragmatic competence; Reference YounYoun (2014) investigated the relationships among learners’ L2 pragmatics in writing tasks, syntactic complexity, and L2 proficiency. Syntactic complexity was analyzed in three aspects: mean length of T-unit, mean length of clause, and mean number of clauses per T-unit. The results showed that the three measures of syntactic complexity were not strongly related to increases in L2 proficiency and pragmatic competence. Comparatively, the increases in mean length of T-unit were more related to different levels of proficiency and pragmatics than the other two measures. Similarly, Reference ChenChen (2020) analyzed Chinese EFL writers’ use of single-author in academic writing, while Reference Taguchi, Fernández and JiangTaguchi et al. (2021) examined Chinese EFL learners’ advice-giving in written texts.

The fact that learners’ writing pragmatic competence is under-researched may be due in part to two reasons. On the one hand, pragmatists, including L2 pragmatists, still value oral communication over written texts. Thus, except for historical pragmatics, pragmatics research prefers oral communication over written. On the other hand, many scholars investigate L2 learners’ writing such as formulaic expressions (e.g., Reference Pérez-LlantadaPérez-Llantada, 2014) and meta-discourse (e.g., Reference HylandHyland, 2019; Reference Qin and UccelliQin and Uccelli, 2019), but they conduct research under the perspectives of L2 writing and academic discourse rather than L2 pragmatics.

Compared with written communication, L2 pragmatics has paid more attention to learners’ pragmatic competence in internet-mediated communication. This stream of work can be traced at least to the beginning of this century; for example, Reference Belz and KingingerBelz and Kinginger (2002) showcased the learning and use of informal vs. formal T-/V-form second person pronouns by two American students, one studying French and one studying German, in web-based contact including email and synchronous chat with their key pals in France and Germany, respectively.

However, most of the L2 pragmatics studies on internet-mediated communication have been conducted in this decade. Email is a frequently explored communicative genre (e.g., Reference Chen and LiuChen et al., 2016; Reference Economidou-Kogetsidis, Savic and HalenkoEconomidou-Kogetsidis et al., 2021), in both pragmatic production (e.g., Reference Barón and OrtegaBarón and Ortega, 2018; Reference Economidou-KogetsidisEconomidou-Kogetsidis, 2018; Reference Savić, Đorđević, Economidou-Kogetsidis, Savić and HalenkoSavić and Đorđević, 2021) and perception (e.g., Reference Economidou-KogetsidisEconomidou-Kogetsidis, 2016; Reference Ren, Liu, Economidou-Kogetsidis, Savic and HalenkoRen and Liu, 2021; Reference WinansWinans, 2020). Researchers often employ electronic DCTs (EDCTs) to investigate email literacy, and this is more legitimate than using written DCTs for exploring oral communication; although email is a hybrid of oral and written, it is essentially more like a written genre.

Researchers have also investigated learners’ pragmatic competence in various types of internet-mediated communication, including digital stories (Reference García-PastorGarcía-Pastor, 2020), web conferences (Reference CunninghamCunningham, 2017), gameplay (Reference Tang and TaguchiTang and Taguchi, 2021), and social media such as Facebook (Reference Blattner and FioriBlattner and Fiori, 2012), Reddit (Reference Yeh and SwinehartYeh and Swinehart, 2020), Skype (Reference Barron and BlackBarron and Black, 2015), and WhatsApp (Reference García-GómezGarcía-Gómez, 2020). Most research has documented the benefit of internet-mediated communication in helping learners to develop their L2 pragmatic competence. However, Reference García-GómezGarcía-Gómez (2020) found that without careful design or training, social media platforms such as WhatsApp may also be detrimental for learners’ L2 pragmatic development because they may encounter more cases of misunderstanding on WhatsApp.

As the above reviews show, L2 pragmatics learning has investigated oral, written, and internet-mediated communication. It is vital for researchers to “converge on modality” (Reference Bardovi-HarligBardovi-Harlig, 2018: 16) in L2 pragmatics research. That is, if researchers wish to investigate learners’ pragmatic competence in oral communication, it must be investigated by oral tasks; in written communication, it must be investigated by written tasks; and in internet-mediated communication, it must be investigated in the context of online platforms. However, there were some studies that used written tasks to measure learners’ oral production, which posed a serious threat to the validity of the findings (see Section 4.2 for more discussion on validity).

2.4 Summary

This section reviewed studies investigating learners’ acquisition in pragmatic production and perception. Some studies on conventional expressions examined both facets. In contrast, learners’ production and perception were often analyzed separately in other pragmatic features. Future studies are encouraged to investigate the two facets of pragmatic competence in the same groups of learners to explore the relation between productive and receptive pragmatic competences. In addition, except for studies on perception of contextual cues, the research only examines the linguistic resource in pragmatic competence. The semiotic and multimodal resources have not attracted attention, even in the internet-mediated communication where such resources are used pervasively.

Furthermore, although increasingly researchers have investigated learners’ pragmatic learning, there are still a large number of studies only examining learners’ pragmatic use. They investigate learners’ pragmatic performance at the time of data collection, and thus they can be classified as single-moment studies (Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose, 1999). The focus on pragmatic use rather than learning is a partially cause of the peripheral position of L2 pragmatics in general L2 research (Reference RenRen, 2015). Future studies should stop treating learners as a homogenous group and simply comparing their pragmatic performance with that of native speakers. It is more insightful to investigate the pattern of learners’ pragmatic development and the association between pragmatic learning and linguistic learning (i.e., vocabulary, grammar) for uncovering the underlying mechanisms of pragmatic learning and suggesting useful implications for pragmatic instruction. The next section reviews the role of pragmatic teaching.

3 Second Language Pragmatics Teaching

This section introduces research on L2 pragmatics instruction, which is much less common than work on L2 pragmatics learning. Research in this field has mainly focused on two questions: 1) Is teaching effective in L2 pragmatics? and 2) Which approach is more effective in teaching L2 pragmatics? We will analyze these two questions respectively in the following subsections.

3.1 Effects of Teaching Second Language Pragmatics

Literature on L2 pragmatics learning has documented that mere exposure is not sufficient for pragmatic development in an L2, even after a long stay in the target language community (e.g., Reference BarronBarron, 2003; Reference RenRen, 2019b; Reference SchauerSchauer, 2009), because “pragmatic functions and relevant contextual factors are often not salient to learners and so not likely to be noticed despite prolonged exposure” (Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose, 2002: 237). Therefore, much of the literature on pragmatics learning calls for pragmatics intervention to assist learners to develop their L2 pragmatic competence (e.g., Reference RenRen, 2013; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2015b), which is generally neglected in L2 classrooms or textbooks (Reference Ren and HanRen and Han, 2016).

Early studies on L2 pragmatics teaching were stimulated by Reference KasperKasper’s (1997) seminal talk, “Can Pragmatic competence be taught?”. After summarizing a dozen prior studies on teaching pragmatics, Kasper’s answer to this question was affirmative. The next few years saw increasing numbers of empirical studies on L2 pragmatics teaching (see Section 3.2) and synthetic works, including narrative reviews (Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose, 1999; Reference RoseRose, 2005; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2011c), syntheses (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2015b; Reference Takahashi and TrosborgTakahashi, 2010), and meta-analyses (Reference Badjadi, Tang and LogonnathanBadjadi, 2016; Reference Jeon, Kaya, Norris and OrtegaJeon and Kaya, 2006; Reference Plonsky, Zhuang and TaguchiPlonsky and Zhuang, 2019; Reference Yousefi and NassajiYousefi and Nassaji, 2019), all of which showed that pragmatics is teachable and pragmatic instruction can facilitate learners’ L2 pragmatic development. It is fair to say that L2 pragmatics scholars have reached a consensus regarding the question of whether teaching is effective in pragmatics; therefore, readers who are interested in L2 pragmatics instruction should shift their attention from exploring whether teaching is helpful in pragmatics to how to teach pragmatics effectively. This is the topic of the next section.

3.2 Approaches in Second Language Pragmatics Teaching

To date, the most investigated teaching approaches in L2 pragmatics have been explicit and implicit teaching. Researchers adopt definitions given by Reference Kasper, Rose and KasperKasper (2001), in which explicit teaching involves metapragmatic explanations of pragmalinguistic uses and/or sociopragmatic rules of target pragmatic features, while implicit teaching does not include such metapragmatic explanation but rather employs various methods to facilitate learners’ unconscious reflection on pragmatic uses of target pragmatic features by themselves.

Implicit teaching consists of a range of different approaches, of which some focus on input, some on output, and others on interaction in completing tasks. This section starts with studies on interventions in terms of input enhancement, which refers to the manipulation of input to make specific target features more salient to learners (Reference Sharwood-SmithSharwood-Smith, 1993), either phonologically or typographically. In L2 pragmatics teaching, input enhancement is manipulated through various forms such as input flood, textual enhancement, and consciousness raising, which are at different positions along the implicit–explicit continuum. As many studies combine different teaching approaches in various ways, it may be more beneficial to investigate the instructional methods separately. Therefore, I will begin with the most implicit tactic and move on to consider the less implicit ones. If two teaching interventions are combined, I will review them in terms of the less implicit teaching method.

3.2.1 Input Flood

Input flood is a pedagogical technique that attempts to make specific pragmatic features more frequent in input without doing anything else to draw the learner’s attention to the features (Reference WongWong, 2005). The input may be written or aural. The expectation is that the artificial increase in the frequency of the pragmatic feature will aid the learner in noticing it.

Not many studies have employed input flood in teaching pragmatics. However, a couple of studies have reported its effectiveness. Reference HernándezHernández (2011) examined the effects of input flood with and without explicit instruction on learners’ use of Spanish discourse makers to narrate an event in the past time frame. Both groups were provided with the opportunity for communicative practice, although only the group that was exposed to explicit teaching received feedback as well. The results suggested that input flood had a positive impact on both learner groups’ use of discourse markers. Reference FordyceFordyce (2014) investigated the effect of input flood and explicit teaching on learners’ uses of epistemic stance in writing, showing the effectiveness of an input flood intervention even if it did not deliberately encourage learners to notice the target forms. Reference Fakher AjabshirFakher Ajabshir (2022) investigated the effects of input flood, textual enhancement, and output-based instruction on Iranian EFL learners’ comprehension and production of external and internal modification of requests. The findings revealed the effectiveness of input flood in teaching the request modification. However, examining adolescent German L2 English learners’ use of pragmatic markers, Reference HaselowHaselow (2021) reported that input flood with communication practice did not improve the frequency and range of the learners’ use of the markers.

Studies disagree more on the comparative effects of input flood when combined with other teaching techniques. Reference HernándezHernández (2011) reported that although learners receiving a combination of explicit teaching and input flood used slightly more discourse markers than learners with input flood alone, the two methods did not lead to a significant difference in developing the learners’ use of Spanish discourse markers. In contrast, employing a similar design to Reference HernándezHernández (2011) involving a comparison of input flood with and without explicit instruction, Reference HaselowHaselow (2021) found that only explicit teaching led to German English learners’ improvement in the use of pragmatic markers. Reference Fakher AjabshirFakher Ajabshir (2022) also concluded that input flood yielded the smallest effect on learners’ improvement with respect to the comprehension and production of external and internal modification of requests, compared with textual enhancement and output-based instruction.

Although we cannot draw conclusions based on these studies, it is fair to say that different pragmatic markers and external vs. internal request modification may be different in terms of their salience and processing difficulty, which may affect the relative effects of input flood. In addition, the potential influences of the number of pragmatic features in input flood and the length of the teaching cannot be excluded.

3.2.2 Textual Enhancement

Textual enhancement makes certain pragmatic features more salient in the input through different forms, such as boldface, underline, and color coding for special stress in written input, and intonation in spoken input. For example, Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler (2005) investigated the effect of instruction on Spanish high school EFL learners’ awareness and production of requests, putting request strategies in bold on the scripts and putting sociopragmatic factors involved in requesting in capital letters and bold. The results showed that both learners’ awareness and production of requests improved after the instruction.

Some studies combine input from different modalities to strengthen the potential effects. Reference Fukuya and Martínez-FlorFukuya and Martínez-Flor (2008) investigated the interactive effects of teaching approach (explicit and implicit) and assessment tasks (email and phone) on Spanish EFL learners’ use of suggestion strategies and hedges. The implicit group received enhanced input by watching videotaped situations with captions of targeted suggestions in boldface and the social factors involved in each situation. The results showed that the explicit group made significantly more improvement than the implicit group in the phone task, but the improvement of the two groups was similar in the email task. Likewise, in Reference Fakher AjabshirFakher Ajabshir (2022) a textual enhancement group watched short video dialogues depicting request acts that included captions with the target feature bold and underlined. After watching the video clips they were also provided with a handout including transcripts of the dialogues with the target features highlighted. The results indicated an effect of textual enhancement on learners’ production and comprehension of request modification.

3.2.3 Consciousness-raising

Consciousness-raising implies that the learner’s mental state is altered by the activity (Reference Sharwood-SmithSharwood-Smith, 1993). It is positioned at the more explicit end of the implicit–explicit continuum and is more frequently investigated in L2 pragmatics than input flood and textual enhancement.

Many studies have demonstrated the benefits of consciousness-raising activities on learners’ pragmatic development. Reference TakimotoTakimoto (2008, Reference Takimoto2009) compared the effects of three teaching approaches on Japanese EFL learners’ production and awareness of request strategies: consciousness-raising tasks including comparing and identifying forms of requests, and structured input tasks asking students to consciously notice the use of a request with, and without, explicit information. The results showed that all the teaching groups performed significantly better than the control group, who performed equally well on request production tested by DCTs and roleplays and on pragmatic appropriateness judgment tasks measured by listening tests and acceptability judgments. Reference TakimotoTakimoto (2012) investigated the effects of consciousness-raising tasks with and without metapragmatic discussion on Japanese learners’ recognition and production of English request downgraders, finding that the two groups made improvements in their recognition and production of request downgraders. In terms of recognition, the two teaching conditions did not differ in terms of improvements in the students’ performance, indicating that regardless of metapragmatic discussion, consciousness-raising tasks can enhance learners’ pragmatic recognition of request downgraders. In terms of production, it was found that the group receiving metapragmatic discussion improved more than the group without such discussion.

Researchers have also examined this teaching technique in languages other than English. Reference NaritaNarita (2012) investigated the effects of consciousness-raising teaching on L2 Japanese learners’ acquisition of evidential markers such as ‘rashii (I heard that)’. The learners were given training in comparing target features in English and Japanese on the one hand, and learners’ and Japanese native speakers’ uses on the other hand. The findings showed the learners’ development in both their production and metapragmatic awareness of the target evidential markers.

Reference AlsuhaibaniAlsuhaibani (2020) compared the effects of consciousness-raising and corpus-based instruction on EFL learners’ compliment responses. The consciousness-raising activities included comparing types and frequencies of compliment responses in L1 and L2, collecting L2 compliment responses from the media, comparing felicitous and infelicitous uses of compliment responses, and roleplays. The corpus group searched the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA) to retrieve authentic compliment responses and compared L1 and L2 compliment responses using the corpus. The results showed the effectiveness of both teaching interventions to the learners in terms of their compliment responses, which did not yield significant differences between the two approaches. This finding is feasible because both approaches raised the learners’ awareness and helped them to notice the use of the target pragmatic features.

The effects of consciousness-raising on the acquisition of conventional expressions have also been examined. Reference Bardovi-Harlig and VellengaBardovi-Harlig and Vellenga (2012) investigated the effect of activities designed to help learners to notice the function of target expressions on recognition and the use of conventional expressions. The results showed that the presentation of contextualized examples with guided noticing activities seemed to promote the learners’ use and recognition of conventional expressions that were relatively transparent and consistent with their grammatical competence.

Reference FurnissFurniss (2016) examined the effect of a corpus-assisted website on L1 English learners’ aural recognition and comprehension of Russian routine formulas. The website was designed to help learners to notice the functions of routine formulas in authentic situations, including activating background knowledge of target routines, translation activities, description of the formula’s functions with examples, cloze activities, dialogue turn matching, and free and integrated production practice. The results indicated that the instruction improved the learners’ awareness of the target routine formulas and helped them detect nonsensical phrases.

Recast is generally defined as a reformulation of non-target-like utterances into more target-like forms while preserving the original meaning (Reference Long, Ritchie and BhatiaLong, 1996), which is in essence a form of consciousness-raising. Many studies combine textual enhancement with recast; for example, Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler (2007) investigated the effect of textual enhancement and recast during roleplays on learners’ awareness of suggestions. The results revealed that the combination of the two implicit teaching techniques was as effective as explicit teaching in improving learners’ pragmatic awareness. Similarly, Reference Nguyen, Pham and PhamNguyen et al. (2017) examined the effects of visual textual enhancement in the format of bolded target structures combined with recasts on Vietnamese EFL learners’ performance of criticism between peers. The learners were tested by three production tasks, including DCT, roleplay, and oral peer-feedback task. The results indicated that the learners improved their pragmatic performance in terms of appropriateness, accuracy, and external and internal modifiers of criticism. Reference AhmadianAhmadian (2020) examined the effect of textual enhancement and teachers’ recast in learners’ roleplays on their production of refusals and judgment of the appropriateness of refusals in conversations. Only one recast per performance and two for each pair of learners were provided. The results revealed that the teaching enhanced the learners’ production and pragmatic awareness of refusals. In addition, the learners’ working memory capacity scores were positively and strongly correlated with their gains.

3.2.4 Output-based Teaching

The output-based teaching approach is built on the Output Hypothesis proposed by Reference Swain, Gass and MaddenSwain (1985), who argues that production (i.e., output) really forces learners to undertake syntactic processing and enhances the L2 development most effectively. Output in second language acquisition (SLA) is not whatever learners produce; it is the language that learners produce to express meanings (Reference VanpattenVanPatten, 2003). This approach emphasizes the critical role for opportunities in which learners are “pushed” to produce improved or modified output in the teaching of an L2 (Reference Swain and HinkelSwain, 2005). Not many studies have investigated output-based teaching in L2 pragmatics. Reference Tajeddin and PezeshkiTajeddin and Pezeshki (2014) examined the teaching effect on learners’ uses of politeness markers using textual enhancement and output tasks that required learners to revise sentences to make them more polite. It was found that the textual enhancement group outperformed the output group in their comprehension and judgment of politeness markers, while the output group improved more than the input group in the production of politeness markers. However, it is worth pointing out that in the output group the learners were also offered explanations about politeness markers in English and examples in their L1, which explicitly provided them with metapragmatic explanation and may have raised their consciousness of the use of politeness markers.

Reference Fakher AjabshirFakher Ajabshir (2022) compared the effects of output- and input-based teaching on learners’ comprehension and production of request modification. After watching short video clips containing target features, the output group learners were required to reconstruct each dialogue so that it was as similar as possible to the video clips. The results indicated that output teaching could yield effects as large as textual enhancement in the learners’ pragmatic appropriateness judgment, and better effects in their production than textual enhancement and input flood.

3.2.5 Task-based Pragmatics Teaching

Task-based teaching in L2 pragmatics has been attracting the attention of a group of researchers over the last decade (see Reference GonzáLez-Lloret and TaguchiGonzález-Lloret, 2019 for a review). Tasks are goal-oriented and meaning-based activities, which simulate authentic communicative contexts. Therefore, tasks can provide opportunities for authentic language practice in the classroom. Reference Gilabert, Barón, Mcdonough and MackeyGilabert and Barón (2013) investigated the extent to which increasing cognitive task demands affected Spanish/Catalan English learners’ pragmatic moves, analyzed by interactions in the use of requests and suggestions. Task complexity was operationalized by [+/- few elements] and [+/- reasoning demands]. The results indicated that the learners produced a larger number of pragmatic moves when performing the complex tasks than the simple tasks. However, task complexity did not influence the learners’ uses of the types of request and suggestion strategies, indicating that greater cognitive task demands may not necessarily push learners to use a wider range of pragmatic strategies. The authors suggested that pedagogy intervention prior to task performance might encourage learners to use unfamiliar and more diverse pragmatic moves.

Reference Kim and TaguchiKim and Taguchi (2015) introduced pragmatic-related episodes (PREs), defined as discussions on, questions about, or corrections of pragmatic production, in analyzing learners’ performance while completing tasks. They examined the impact of tasks across different complexity levels on Korean junior high school students’ request expressions in English. Task complexity was operationalized as different levels of reasoning demands, following Reference RobinsonRobinson’s (2001) Cognition Hypothesis that more complex tasks promote interaction and language development. In the simple tasks group the learners had detailed scenario descriptions and matching pictures, while in the complex group the learners did not have any detailed description of the scenarios. The findings showed that the learners in the complex task group produced more PREs than those in the simple task group. Both task groups outperformed the control group in terms of request production (request strategies, external and internal modifiers) as measured by DCTs. It is worth pointing out that the learners in both groups were provided with explicit metapragmatic explanation of target pragmatic forms, while the control group was not; it is possible that such explicit pre-task training may have also contributed to the differences between the control and experimental groups. The complex task group did not outperform the simple task group in learning the request expressions in the immediate post-test, but one month after the instruction the complex group performed better than the simple group at retaining their knowledge of the target-mitigated preparatory head act forms.

Reference Kim and TaguchiKim and Taguchi (2016) expanded their previous work by adding high vs. low pragmatic situational demands with respect to power, social distance, and imposition of ranking of a request. The results showed that collaborative tasks could benefit the learning of pragmatic features; cognitively more complex tasks elicited more interaction based on the number of turns than the simple tasks, regardless of the level of pragmatic task demands. However, the influence was on the learners’ discussions of sociopragmatic factors rather than pragmalinguistic forms.

Reference Gilabert, Barón, Taguchi and KimGilabert and Barón (2018) carried out one of the few studies to examine the possible effect of task sequence. They operationalized four levels of task complexity based on input frequency, familiarity with the interlocutor, casual vs. intentional reasoning, and number of elements. The tasks were presented across two conditions: a simple to complex condition and a randomized condition. The results showed that the judges’ perception of task difficulty was in accordance with the difficulty levels predicted by the task design. It was likely that the judges interpreted the difficulty and mental effort of tasks similarly, which caused the authors to argue that only one measurement of difficulty or mental effort may be necessary for future research. In addition, the task sequence (simple to complex vs. randomized) did not influence the learners’ pragmatic performance assessed by the judges’ rating.

Reference Gomez-Laich, Taguchi and KimGomez-Laich (2018) expanded the scope of instructional targets from speech acts in oral communication to written genres, examining the effect of the reasoning demands of tasks on learners’ interaction patterns during a collaborative writing task involving persuasion. The simple task group received teaching on rhetoric moves and linguistic resources for persuasive writing, while the complex task group did not receive much assistance. The results showed that the complex task prompted learners to negotiate more over the essay’s structure and their sources of difficulty than the simple task. These findings are rather predictable since the difficulties encountered by the complex task group were taught to the simple task group. What would have been more interesting is whether the reasoning demands affected the learners’ persuasive writing, but unfortunately this was not analyzed in this study.

The genre of email has also been investigated in task-based pragmatics teaching. Reference Alcón Soler, Taguchi and KimAlcón Soler (2018) examined whether interacting with a teacher or another learner would influence learners’ use of request mitigators in email tasks. The results revealed that the tasks improved the learners’ use of request mitigators, and the participatory structure also influenced their learning outcome. The learners who engaged in student–student interaction paid more attention to pragmatics and enhanced their awareness of mitigation in email requests more than those who engaged in student–teacher interaction. Meanwhile, Reference Levkina, Taguchi and KimLevkina (2018) investigated the effect of proficiency and email writing tasks on Spanish EFL learners’ judgments of the appropriateness of apologies, justifications, and thanks in students’ emails. The results revealed that the two proficiency groups improved their performance after the task-based instruction, but this did not correlate with their proficiency.

The effects of task modalities have also been explored. Reference TangTang (2019) examined the effects of computer-mediated communication (CMC) vs. a face-to-face (FTF) condition. The results showed the advantages of FTF on L2 Chinese learners’ use of modal verbs. She hypothesized that the frequent use of fragments and symbols in CMC might have constrained opportunities for learners to use proper linguistic forms. Reference Reagan, Payant, Taguchi and KimReagan and Payant (2018) examined the influence of oral vs. written tasks on L2 Spanish learners’ request strategies and external and internal modification, measured by oral and written DCTs. The results indicated that task-based teaching had positive effects on the learners’ development of indirect request head acts and their external and internal modifications. Task modality did not influence the learners’ pragmatic development. Again, however, it is worth pointing out that both groups of learners were given explicit teaching on request head acts and modification prior to the task activities. As with Reference Kim and TaguchiKim and Taguchi’s (2015) study, this explicit pre-task teaching may have influenced the learners’ performance. In the next subsection, I will examine the effectiveness of explicit teaching on pragmatics.

3.2.6 Explicit Teaching

Second language pragmatics explicit teaching involves metapragmatic explanations of pragmalinguistic uses and/or of the sociopragmatic rules of target pragmatic features, which are often accompanied by various kinds of activities. The benefits of explicit teaching are well documented in the literature (e.g., Reference HalenkoHalenko, 2021; Reference RoseRose, 2005; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2011c). For example, Reference Félix-BrasdeferFélix-Brasdefer (2008b) examined the effects of explicit instruction on L2 Spanish learners’ use of internal modification of refusals. Both the experimental group and the control group carried out a cross-cultural segment comparison activity, but only the experimental group received metapragmatic instruction. The results demonstrated that the experimental group’s pragmatic competence improved significantly, as evidenced by more types and a higher frequency of lexical and syntactic mitigators in their refusal responses. The improvement was retained one month after the instruction.

Existing research often compares the effectiveness of explicit and implicit interventions across a wide range of pragmatic features, and most studies have revealed the advantage of explicit teaching over implicit teaching in improving learners’ pragmatic competence. For example, Reference Takahashi, Rose and KasperTakahashi (2001) compared the improvement of learners’ requests in four conditions: explicit metapragmatic teaching, two methods aiming to raise learners’ pragmatics consciousness by asking them to compare forms between native speakers’ and their own request strategies and to search for native-like request strategies in input, and reading comprehension. The results showed that the three implicit conditions did not yield significant differences, while explicit teaching was the most effective in improving learners’ pragmatic competence.

Similarly, Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler (2005) investigated the effects of explicit instruction versus textual enhancement on Spanish high school EFL learners’ English requests. The explicit teaching group received awareness-raising tasks and written metapragmatic feedback related to the use of appropriate requests. The results revealed that both the explicit and the implicit teaching helped the learners to develop their pragmatic awareness with similar effects. However, the explicit teaching had an advantage over implicit teaching with respect to the learners’ request production. Meanwhile, Reference Ghobadi and FahimGhobadi and Fahim (2009) compared the effects of explicit and implicit teaching on Iranian EFL learners’ pragmatic awareness of thanking. The explicit group were provided with metapragmatic explanations of English thanking strategies and roleplayed conversations in pairs, while the implicit group carried out the roleplays but were not given any explanations. The results showed that the explicit group outperformed the implicit group.

Reference TakimotoTakimoto (2012) investigated the effects of consciousness raising teaching with and without metapragmatic instruction on Japanese learners’ recognition and production of English request downgraders. The results showed that both groups performed significantly better than the control group, and no differences were found between the two groups with respect to pragmatic judgment. However, the explicit teaching group performed better in terms of their production of request downgraders than the implicit teaching group. Finally, in Reference AhmadianAhmadian’s (2020) study, explicit group learners received descriptions and examples of refusal strategies and were provided with corrective feedback on their incorrect use of refusal strategies in roleplays. The results revealed that explicit instruction was more effective than implicit instruction for both the production and comprehension of refusals.

Researchers have sometimes included several activities in their design. For example, in Reference Eslami, Mirzaei and DiniEslami et al. (2015), for the implicit group the teaching activities included textual enhancement, consciousness-raising activities of requests comparison, production activities using DCTs, and reflection based on recasts and implicit feedback; for the explicit group, the teaching activities included metapragmatic explanation, consciousness-raising by asking learners to provide examples of requests in L1 and L2 and identify request strategies and modification, discussion of inappropriate requests and the potential reasons, production activities, and explicit feedback. The findings revealed that the explicit group improved more than the implicit group in terms of requests, measured by DCTs. However, with so many activities in the two teaching groups, it is difficult to tell which activities led to the different improvements in the two groups.

In contrast, not all studies have demonstrated the advantages of explicit teaching over implicit teaching. Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler (2007) compared explicit and implicit teaching in terms of learners’ awareness of suggestions. The explicit group received metapragmatic explanations and were offered awareness-raising tasks and practices, while the implicit group received textual enhancement of pragmalinguistic aspects and sociopragmatic factors in bold and recast during roleplays. The results revealed that the combination of the two implicit teaching techniques was as effective as the explicit teaching in improving the learners’ pragmatic awareness. Reference HernándezHernández (2011) even reported that combining explicit teaching with input flood was not superior to input flood alone in developing learners’ use of Spanish discourse markers.

The effect of explicit teaching has also been investigated in different modalities. For example, Reference FordyceFordyce (2014) investigated the effect of explicit and implicit intervention on learners’ uses of epistemic stance in writing. The explicit intervention included metapragmatic rule explanation combined with various textual enhancement and awareness-raising activities to direct learners’ attention to target forms. The results showed that the explicit intervention clearly had a much greater effect on the learners’ use of epistemic features than the implicit intervention, although the development from the explicit intervention decreased from immediate post-test to delayed post-test five months later. Reference Fakher AjabshirFakher Ajabshir (2019) explored the effect of pragmatic teaching delivered through synchronous and asynchronous CMC versus FTF interaction on learners’ acquisition of requests. The teaching activities were the same for the three groups of learners, and included metapragmatic instruction, watching video clips on requests, and discussion and practice with peers. The findings revealed that the CMC-oriented instruction produced better results than the FTF instruction, but no significant difference was found between the asynchronous and synchronous groups. Reference TakimotoTakimoto (2020) compared the effects of cognitive metaphor with explanation and visualization of the politeness level of requests in terms of spatial concepts versus metapragmatic explanation of politeness level on learners’ production and awareness of requests. The results revealed that the cognitive approach outperformed the non-cognitive approach for both production and awareness tasks.

In terms of explicit metapragmatic instruction, the relative advantage of inductive and deductive teaching has also been investigated. For the teaching of pragmatics, deductive teaching refers to the instruction that begins with the provision of metapragmatic information, followed by examples and practice, while in inductive teaching, learners are first presented with examples and exercises containing the pragmatic aspects to be taught, and then are guided to discover or provided with the metapragmatic rules. Reference GlaserGlaser (2016) compared inductive and deductive approaches to explicit teaching in the context of EFL learners’ use of refusal strategies and adjuncts. The results revealed an advantage of inductive explicit teaching over deductive explicit teaching in teaching refusals in English. The superiority of inductive explicit instruction over deductive explicit instruction is echoed in Reference Qi and LaiQi and Lai’s (2017) study, which examined the teaching of requests to L2 Chinese learners via a self-access website. Likewise, Reference Haghighi, Jafarigohar, Khoshsima and VahdanyHaghighi et al. (2019) found an advantage of metapragmatic explanation with inductive teaching over deductive teaching in Iranian EFL learners’ use of refusals, although they compared flipped classroom and conventional classroom.

Finally, assessment methods may yield different results, even if learners receive the same teaching. For example, Reference TaylorTaylor (2002) offered learners metapragmatic teaching and practice in learning gambits in Spanish, testing their performance by either discussion tasks or roleplay tasks. The study found that the discussion group showed a significant increase in the variety of gambits they used after instruction, whereas the roleplay group did not.

3.3 Summary

The above sections demonstrate that pragmatics teaching contributes to learners’ pragmatic development, whether in more salient aspects such as pragmatic routines and speech act strategies or less salient aspects such as internal modifiers of speech acts. Therefore, the first question about whether pragmatic teaching is effective is often treated as a prerequisite for further investigation in L2 pragmatics teaching. As reviewed in this section, there are now fairly well-established teaching frameworks that have been found to be effective and can be used in bringing pragmatics to the classroom. Researchers now spend their efforts on exploring which approach and methods can bring about better and stronger effects in terms of developing learners’ pragmatic competence.

This line of research reveals a much more complex picture. Existing narrative reviews (Reference Kasper and RoseKasper and Rose, 1999; Reference RoseRose, 2005; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2011c), syntheses (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2015b; Reference Takahashi and TrosborgTakahashi, 2010) and meta-analyses (Reference Jeon, Kaya, Norris and OrtegaJeon and Kaya, 2006; Reference Plonsky, Zhuang and TaguchiPlonsky and Zhuang, 2019) all point to the superiority of explicit teaching interventions over implicit teaching. However, they also acknowledge that factors such as target pragmatic features, learner characteristics, and different degrees of the teaching method, along implicitness–explicitness and length of instruction, all have impacts on the efficacy of L2 pragmatics instruction. In addition, the lack of delayed tests in empirical studies fails to document the sustainability or longevity of instructional effects. Finally, as illustrated by the reviews in this section, many studies combine a wide variety of teaching activities. Quite often the explicit teaching group also receives various implicit teaching activities, particularly aiming to raise learners’ awareness of target features. The increasingly popular task-based pragmatics teaching also often includes metapragmatic explanation prior to task activities. Therefore, the findings regarding the efficacy of teaching approaches are far from conclusive, and more research is warranted to further explore this issue.

4 Second Language Pragmatics Assessment

Pragmatics assessment is a crucial but often neglected branch of research in L2 pragmatics, although it dates back at least as far as seminal works by Reference Hudson, Detmer and BrownHudson et al. (1992, Reference Hudson, Detmer and Brown1995). One reason for this may be because assessing and measuring pragmatic competence is inherently complex since pragmatics in essence emphasizes dynamic meaning making processes in interaction. It is extremely difficult to capture the fluid and ongoing nature of interaction, particularly in formal and large-scale tests. This section will review this growing field, examining what research on pragmatics assessment has explored and achieved.

4.1 Pragmatic Aspects Assessed

To date, few L2 pragmatics studies explicitly state that they are assessing learners’ pragmatic competence. This may be partially due to the fact that assessing learners’ pragmatic competence at a certain point is close to measuring their pragmatic production and/or perception, which is the focus of studies on pragmatic learning (see Section 2). There is considerable overlap between these two fields in this sense. Reference Hudson, Detmer and BrownHudson et al. (1995) is one of the earliest studies to examine L2 English learners’ pragmatic knowledge of apology, request, and refusal using six measures, including open-ended written DCTs, oral DCTs, roleplays, multiple-choice DCTs, and self-assessment rating scales for the DCTs and roleplays. They operationalized the difficulty levels of items according to three sociopragmatic variables from Reference Brown and LevinsonBrown and Levinson’s (1987) politeness theory: social status, social distance, and ranking of imposition. A situation with higher social status difference and social distance between the speaker and hearer and a higher ranking for the imposition of a speech act was considered more difficult than a situation with lower levels of these three factors. This way of distinguishing the difficulty levels of items is very influential and has frequently been used in later studies (e.g., Reference LiuLiu, 2007; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2007b; Reference YounYoun, 2020a).

Compared with speech acts, more studies assess learners’ receptive pragmatic knowledge such as their comprehension of conversation implicature. For example, Reference RoeverRoever (2005) examined learners’ knowledge of implicature and routines using multiple-choice DCTs, with the implicature items adopted from Reference BoutonBouton (1994). Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2009b) illustrated how she designed multiple-choice items to evaluate learners’ comprehension of more and less implicatures in English based on two corpora. She found that comprehension of indirect refusals was easier than comprehension of routines. Questioning a lack of correspondence with empirical conversational data, Reference WaltersWalters (2009) showcased the development of a multiple-choice listening test to assess learners’ pragmatic comprehension in English, employing findings from conversation analysis. He operationalized learners’ pragmatic competence as the ability to understand three types of pragmatic actions: assessment responses, compliment responses, and pre-sequence responses. Walters acknowledged that the three actions did not represent learners’ overall pragmatic competence; the reasons for using these three types of actions were simply because they were well documented in the existing conversation analysis literature.

The above approach explicitly assesses learners’ pragmatic knowledge as an individual trait, but this has been criticized on the grounds that it fails to uncover learners’ pragmatic performance in real interaction. In recent years, many researchers have paid attention to assessing learners’ abilities to use language in interaction, often conceptualized as interactional competence (see a collection of studies in Reference Ross and KasperRoss and Kasper, 2013). Reference YounYoun (2020a) investigated how to analyze learners’ interactional competence quantitatively. Three interactional features were identified—length of interaction, engaging with interaction, and sequential organization—each consisting of various indices. Although the interactional features functioned differently across different roleplays, they were shown to be useful in operationalizing the construct of L2 pragmatic interaction. In another qualitative study, Reference YounYoun (2020b) showcased how learners jointly accomplished proposal sequences in roleplays. It was found that lower-level learners often initiated a proposal sequence without establishing a shared understanding relevant to upcoming actions, while higher-level learners typically employed various shift markers and stepwise transition when initiating and shifting actions. When responding to a proposal the higher-level learners recurrently used coherent topic organizations, but in terms of closing a proposal sequence learners across all proficiency levels were able to perform well.

The associations between high-stakes exams and real communication contexts have also been explored. Reference Seedhouse, Ross and KasperSeedhouse (2013) compared interaction, in terms of repair, sequence organization, and turn taking, in the IELTS Speaking Test (IST) and in L2 classrooms and academic classes at universities. The findings showed that the interaction in the three settings was organized to meet the specific institutional goals. Contrasting with the rigid structure of the IST, the interactional organization of L2 classrooms and university classes varied widely. For example, interviewers in the IST limited repair to clear up test takers’ misunderstanding of interview questions, while teachers and peers frequently provided repair, corrections, and evaluation in L2 classrooms and academic classes. Reference Roever and IkedaRoever and Ikeda (2022) investigated the extent to which the speaking section of the TOEFL iBT could provide information about the test-takers’ interactional competence. They asked two raters to evaluate participants’ roleplays against four criteria: language use for mitigation, social actions, engagement in interaction, and turn organization. The results suggested that the TOEFL iBT speaking section could only predict testees’ ability to use language in interaction to a limited degree. The authors suggested that the absence of recipient design, one of the crucial components of interactional competence, in TOEFL iBT might account for the dissociation between the two measures.

In line with the status quo in L2 pragmatics learning, learners’ interaction has rarely been investigated in the written modality (see Section 2.3). Reference Huth, Betz, Salaberry and KunitzHuth and Betz (2019) set out to assess learners’ interactional knowledge by testing generic practices of interaction in written formats, for instance, using an unscramble-the-sequence task for testing sequence organization in conversation closings, a multiple-choice completion task for projection in interactional quoting, and data analysis/peer discovery of connectors for repair. The authors argued that testing formats for learners’ interactional competence may not necessarily encourage learners to follow prescriptive normativity. Assessing L2 interaction did “not aim for the right answers, but highlight[ed] systematicity, context sensitivity and diversification” (Reference Huth, Betz, Salaberry and KunitzHuth and Betz, 2019: 348).

Dynamic assessment has also been employed to evaluate learners’ pragmatic production and comprehension. For example, combining conversation analysis and dynamic assessment, Reference NicholasNicholas (2020) used dynamically administered strategic interaction scenarios (D-SIS) to assess Japanese EFL learners’ difficulties and development when requesting during interaction. In a D-SIS the researcher performs one of the roles in a roleplay with the learner. Either the learner or the researcher can pause the task whenever they feel there is a challenging issue and begin a mediation procedure, an insertion sequence initiated after pausing the roleplay. Nicholas distinguished two types of learners’ mediation: requiring implicit assistance, indicating that the learner had efficiently oriented to or resolved an object, or extensive explicit mediation, indicating that the object was a particular problem for the learner (Reference Van Compernolle, Ross and Kaspervan Compernolle, 2013). Thus, by analyzing mediation in the D-SIS, researchers can identify learners’ pragmatic difficulties and trace their development in interaction. Reference Qin and Van CompernolleQin and van Compernolle (2021) reported on the design and administration of an online dynamic assessment of implicature comprehension in L2 Chinese; learners were classified into beginner, intermediate, and advanced levels, and the assessment was centered on indirect acceptance, refusals, and opinions. The results showed that while all the learners could improve with mediation, individual variation existed in their responsiveness to assistance. The authors claimed that the learners’ performances with mediation were more informative than independent performance in terms of assessing their pragmatic competence.

4.2 Validity

Validity is an important aspect of assessment, whether the research targets learners’ pragmatic knowledge or interaction. Validation studies aim to systematically investigate empirical evidence that “provides insights into the extent to which a test measures what it is supposed to measure, relative to its purpose and use” (Reference Timpe-Laughlin and ChoiTimpe-Laughlin and Choi, 2017: 21). Reference HaastrupHaastrup (1986) carried out one of the earliest studies to address this, dealing with the validity issues of assessing pragmatics in a structured oral interview in English with Danish children. Haastrup argued that teachers can engage children in a conversation or a simulation (similar to a roleplay) to assess their pragmatic competence, which was operationalized as the ability to carry out a range of speech acts. The validity of roleplays was analyzed in Reference Youn, Taguchi and KimYoun (2018a, Reference Youn2020a) based on conversation analysis, showing that carefully designed roleplays can elicit valid interactional data that reflects learners’ pragmatic competence.

The validity of different measurements has been compared. Reference Brown, Rose and KasperBrown (2001) investigated six types of measures in an EFL setting and a Japanese as L2 setting: a written DCT, a multiple-choice DCT, an oral DCT, a roleplay, a discourse self-assessment task, and a roleplay self-assessment. The results showed that the original English versions of the six pragmatics tests generally did not work as well as the Japanese translation versions, in terms of their reliability and the amounts of variance produced. The EFL tests were subject to stronger method effects than the JSL (Japanese as a Second Language) tests. The author discussed the practical advantages and disadvantages of the six types of tests and ranked the EFL and JSL results separately according to each of the following characteristics: easiness to complete, ease of administration and scoring, validity, reliability, variance, encourages oral language and self-reflection, and suitability for high-stakes decision.

Reference Hudson, Rose and KasperHudson (2001) examined the method effects of written DCTs, oral DCTs, and roleplays on three speech acts: requests, refusals, and apologies. The results showed that oral DCTs were slightly more difficult than roleplays and written DCTs, while roleplays seemed to measure different pragmatic constructs than the two DCTs. This discrepancy between DCTs and more interactive oral measurement techniques was echoed by Reference Walters, Ross and KasperWalters (2013), who suggested that the oral conversation analysis-informed tests and DCTs did not measure the same construct. Reference Brown, Alcón Soler and Martínez-FlorBrown (2008) reported reasonably high reliability estimates for the test instruments used in his earlier study (Reference Brown, Rose and KasperBrown, 2001; Reference Hudson, Detmer and BrownHudson et al., 1995) except for multiple-choice DCTs.

Reference HinkelHinkel (1997) argued that to increase the validity of a multiple-choice task, it must be constructed around actual situations in which the speech act was performed, supplemented by previous relevant research findings on the speech act. In addition, production responses from pilot DCTs should serve as the options for multiple-choice test design and development. These suggestions were adopted by Reference LiuLiu (2007), who developed a multiple-choice DCT to assess Chinese English learners’ apologies in five steps. The first was exemplar generation, in which Chinese students were asked to identify situations where apologies were needed. The second step was to check situation likelihood, which explored how likely it was that the situation would occur in the students’ life. The third step, metapragmatic assessment, asked the students to assess the sociopragmatic variables in each scenario. In the fourth step the scenarios were validated. Finally, multiple-choice options were developed by including alternative responses collected from students’ DCTs. The results of both Rasch analysis and learners’ verbal reports showed that the multiple-choice DCT was reliable and valid.

The validity of DCTs has been examined and challenged for a long time. They have been criticized for having low construct validity, since they cannot collect data representing the oral features of authentic discourse (Reference GolatoGolato, 2003; Reference Johnston, Kasper and RossJohnston et al., 1998; Reference RoseRose, 1994). For example, examining advice among English native speakers and Chinese EFL learners, Reference HinkelHinkel (1997) found that DCTs failed to elicit responses approximating actual speech acts. However, Reference Beebe, Cummings, Gass and NeuBeebe and Cummings (1996) documented that data elicited with DCTs were consistent with naturally occurring data in terms of main patterns and formulas. Reference Economidou-KogetsidisEconomidou-Kogetsidis (2013) compared written DCT and naturally occurring requests, which found that the written DCT requests approximated natural data in terms of directness and lexical modification. In addition, Reference Billmyer and VargheseBillmyer and Varghese (2000) examined the effects of enriching the content of situation prompts in DCTs on L1 and L2 speakers’ output. The results showed that the content-enriched prompts elicited more robust external modification and elaboration than content-poor prompts, “in much the same way as speech in natural spontaneous interaction” (Reference Billmyer and VargheseBillmyer and Varghese, 2000: 533). This is in line with Reference Beebe, Cummings, Gass and NeuBeebe and Cummings (1996). However, enriched DCT prompts are often long, which means that learners may find them cognitively demanding and difficult (Reference Roever, Boxer and CohenRoever, 2004).

Cognitive validity in an L2 pragmatics test is occasionally explored by examining what test takers do when completing the test, and whether the cognitive mechanisms involved in their test performances match the construct. Reference LabbenLabben (2016) examined the cognitive validity of DCTs and argued that written DCTs should be treated as a language test rather than a questionnaire, and should undergo a rigorous developmental process. Reference Youn and BiYoun and Bi (2019) analyzed learners’ reported strategy use in monologic speaking tasks and roleplays to investigate the cognitive validity of L2 pragmatic assessment. They found that higher-level learners were likely to report different types of strategies more frequently than lower-level learners. However, regardless of L2 proficiency levels, learners were aware of the importance of being polite and pragmatically appropriate. Distinct strategies were used depending on the different characteristics of monologic versus dialogic tasks, thus demonstrating that the pragmatic assessment tasks measured the intended constructs and echoing the findings in previous research (e.g., Reference Hudson, Rose and KasperHudson, 2001).

The above studies focus on assessing learners’ production, albeit through measurements that require learners to choose an option for production. Reference Timpe-Laughlin and ChoiTimpe-Laughlin and Choi (2017) aimed to collect evidence of construct validity for a test of receptive pragmatic competence, using a web-based assessment measuring learners’ pragmatic comprehension. The test consisted of tasks targeting learners’ knowledge of the speech acts of requests and offers, routine formulae, and culture-dependent phrases and idioms. The results were quite positive, indicating that the test scores could reliably measure university students’ pragmatic knowledge and provide instructors with useful and meaningful information about the students’ receptive pragmatic competence.

4.3 Rating

It is challenging but crucial to develop criteria that measure learners’ pragmatic competence. Since appropriateness varies according to contexts and potentially in different raters’ reactions, rater disagreement is likely and even expected in pragmatics assessment. Therefore, it is necessary to develop a reliable and valid rating scale to objectively evaluate learners’ L2 pragmatic performance.

Several studies developed their rating scales a priori, sometimes with reference to previous literature or a needs analysis (Reference YounYoun, 2018b). For example, Reference Hudson, Detmer and BrownHudson et al. (1995) used five-point scales on six dimensions, asking participants to evaluate learners’ production collected by DCTs from very unsatisfactory to completely appropriate; the dimensions included the ability to use the correct speech act, typical expressions, amount of speech and information, and levels of formality, directness, and politeness. Reference SasakiSasaki (1998) asked two trained native speakers to evaluate learners’ pragmatic production in DCTs and roleplays in terms of appropriateness and grammar/structure. Although she also used fluency for roleplays, this dimension was not reported. Appropriateness had four levels, ranging from least appropriate/acceptable (scores 1–2), not really appropriate but acceptable (scores 3–5), appropriate to some degree (scores 6–8), and most appropriate for the given situation (scores 9–10) (Reference SasakiSasaki, 1998: 465).

Meanwhile, Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2007b: 121) used a six-point simplified appropriateness rating scale to ask six native speakers to evaluate learners’ performance on a pragmatic speaking task. Specifically, the zero point indicated no performance; a rating of 1 indicated very poor performance, where responses were very difficult to understand and no intended speech acts were performed; a rating of 2 indicated a poor performance, where appropriateness was difficult to judge; a score of 3 indicated a fair performance, where expressions were somewhat appropriate; a score of 4 showed a good performance, where expressions were mostly appropriate; and a score of 5 points indicated an excellent performance, where expressions were fully appropriate. This rating scale has frequently been adopted or adapted in later studies to measure learners’ pragmatic performance (e.g., Reference Levkina, Taguchi and KimLevkina, 2018; Reference LiLi, 2014; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2011b). Reference Kuiken and VedderKuiken and Vedder (2017) developed a six-point rating scale to assess to what extent a text fulfilled the communicative function intended. The rating scale consisted of four dimensions, including content, task requirements, comprehensibility, and coherence and cohesion. Finally, Reference Kley, Salaberry and KunitzKley (2019: 318) proposed a rubric for evaluating learners’ interactional competence using a three-point rating scale with four criteria: initiating new topics, reciprocating the interlocutor’s topic-initiating questions, shifting between topics, and expanding on topics.

Unlike the above ratings analyzing learner responses on different dimensions, Reference WaltersWalters (2007) deliberately designed a four-point holistic rating scale to evaluate learners’ oral production of assessment responses, compliment responses, and pre-sequence responses. The rubric was vague, with a score of 1 indicating no evidence of control of assessment responses, 2 indicating more evidence of no control, 3 indicating more evidence of control, and a score of 4 indicating evidence of control (Reference WaltersWalters, 2007: 168). It is worth noting that Walters stated that the vagueness of the rubric was intentional, given the exploratory nature of applying conversation analysis to rating pragmatic performance.

A couple of studies generated their rating scales based on analyses of learners’ pragmatic performance, thereby increasing the validity of the rating scales. Reference YounYoun (2015) developed five rating categories for interactive open roleplays to be used in classroom assessment in an academic English context, including content delivery, language use, sensitivity to situation, engaging with interaction, and turn organization. Content delivery measures learners’ ability to speak appropriately and fluently in a turn; language use measures a range of pragmalinguistic strategies; sensitivity to situation measures the learners’ sociopragmatic perceptions and awareness; engaging with interaction measures the degree of learners’ engagement and coordination with the interlocutor in roleplays; and turn organization measures learners’ orientation to normative turn-taking conventions. Quantitative analyses confirmed the unique contribution of each of the five categories in measuring learners’ pragmatic competence in interaction.

Reference LiuChen and Liu (2016) developed and validated a rating scale to evaluate the speech act performance of apology and request by intermediate Chinese EFL learners in written DCTs. In the scale development stage, initially native speaker raters without training were asked to judge the content and form of DCT productions by American English native speakers and Chinese EFL learners across five hypothesized ability levels, ranging from very poor, poor, fair, good, to excellent along a 10-point Likert scale with a 2-point margin between each level, and write comments for each response. Next, the raters’ comments were coded and the DCTs were classified into five sub-corpora consisting of scripts banded into the five hypothesized levels. A detailed analyses of the rater comments and the DCT productions resulted in a rating scale consisting of two 10-point subscales (for a detailed description, see Reference LiuChen and Liu, 2016: 239). In the validation stage, seven trained raters were asked to rate another group of Chinese EFL learner’ email productions, and a FACETS analysis was conducted to examine the validity and reliability of the rating scale. The results showed that the rating scale could reliably differentiate the learners’ email production. In terms of validity, the results showed that some levels of the 10-point scale were disordered, and the 2-point interval for each ability level confused the raters because it was difficult for them to tell the difference between the two scores at the same level.

In addition to developing rating scales, researchers have also investigated rater variation in evaluating learners’ pragmatic performance. Reference WaltersWalters (2007) found that a native English rater and a non-native English rater interpreted learners’ performance differently, with the non-native rater paying more attention to fluency and pronunciation than the native speaker rater. Reference Tajeddin and AlemiTajeddin and Alemi (2014) investigated native English teachers’ ratings of EFL learners’ apology DCTs, and whether there was rater bias in their ratings. A content analysis of the raters’ descriptions revealed that they mostly paid attention to expressions of apology, explanations, offers of repair, promises for the future, and politeness. Quantitative analyses via FACETS indicated that the raters showed different degrees of severity and tolerance in their ratings.

The influences of raters’ cultural and linguistic backgrounds on their ratings have also been explored. Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2011b) investigated rater variation in the judgment of speech acts by analyzing judges’ verbal interview data. Four native English speakers from different cultural backgrounds – one African American, one Asian American and two Australians – assessed the appropriateness of requests and opinions produced by Japanese EFL learners. The findings showed that the raters evaluated the appropriateness of speech acts differently; some focused more on pragmalinguistic aspects such as the directness level of head acts and the use of politeness markers, while others paid more attention to non-linguistic aspects such as the content of politeness strategies. Even when raters focused on the same dimensions of pragmatic performance, they varied in their degree of tolerance – for example, in their level of acceptance of length of low-imposition requests.

Reference Sonnenburg-Winkler, Eslami and DerakhshanSonnenburg-Winkler et al. (2020) carried out one of the few studies exploring the relationship between linguistic background and pragmatics ratings. This study investigated variability among raters from different L1s by asking ten participants to evaluate written request DCTs produced by their peers and themselves using the rating scale developed by Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2011b). The raters were also asked to provide an explanation for their rating. The findings showed that the raters considered a variety of dimensions when evaluating the speech acts, with some raters focusing more on the directness levels of expressions, while others based their decisions on non-linguistic aspects such as the content of reasons for requests. This finding echoed the rater variation documented in Reference TaguchiTaguchi (2011b). In addition, raters from the same L1 tended to rate productions produced by speakers of the same L1 similarly.

4.4 Summary

Second language pragmatic assessment is an emerging and crucial field, particularly concerning the validity and reliability in assessing and measuring learners’ pragmatic competence in the learning and teaching studies. Validity is indeed one of the key topics in pragmatic assessing research, and the reliability of rating is also an often-examined issue. However, the L2 pragmatic assessing literature should be expanded to explore more issues that are well examined in language assessment but are much neglected in pragmatic assessment, such as washback, assessment literacy, theoretical models, and computer assisted assessment. In addition, learners’ cognitive processes during completing tasks are key to understanding their test performance and the cognitive validity of assessment tools. In the next section, the role of cognitive process is discussed as related to pragmatic learning, pragmatic teaching, and pragmatic assessing.

5 Cognitive Processes

The previous sections introduced research on the three fields of L2 pragmatics. This section will analyze studies that explore learners’ cognitive processes during pragmatic performance using verbal reports, aiming to identify the major contributions and key methodological concerns related to the use of verbal reports in L2 pragmatics research. Cognitive processes can be investigated alone, or as a part of research to triangulate findings of pragmatic performance. To date, not many studies have examined learners’ cognitive processes during pragmatic performance. The following subsections will analyze those studies in detail.

5.1 Instruments to Measure Cognitive Processes in Pragmatics

Verbal reports as a form of introspection solicit verbalization data about the thought processes of participants during the completion of a task (Reference Cohen, Félix-Brasdefer and KoikeCohen, 2012; Reference Gass and MackeyGass and Mackey, 2016). Verbal reports aim to provide insights into the cognitive processes behind learners’ written or spoken behaviors during linguistic performance, which otherwise would have to be investigated indirectly (Reference CohenCohen, 1998). There are two types of verbal reports: concurrent verbal reports (CVR), also referred to as think aloud protocols, and retrospective verbal reports (RVR), which are implemented subsequent to a task and prompt learners to report on the thoughts they had during task completion. In one of the earliest studies examining learners’ cognitive processes in L2 pragmatics, Reference Robinson and KasperRobinson (1992) showed that when designed and executed with care, particularly in combination with other data collection methods, both CVR and RVR can provide in-depth insights into learners’ pragmatic knowledge and their pragmatic difficulties in the planning and execution of speech acts.

Since then a few studies have used CVR, but the majority of the studies on cognitive process in L2 pragmatics have employed RVR. This is understandable as most L2 pragmatics studies examine learners’ oral performance, which means they cannot perform CVR simultaneously. RVRs have most often been combined with roleplays (e.g., Reference Cohen and OlshtainCohen and Olshtain, 1993; Reference Woodfield, Felix-Brasdefer and KoikeWoodfield, 2012; Reference Ying and RenYing and Ren, 2021), but other data collection methods that have been combined with RVRs include conversation elicitation tasks (Reference NguyenNguyen, 2017), comprehension questionnaires (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2008b), written DCTs (Reference BellaBella, 2014a), and multimedia elicitation tasks (Reference RenRen, 2014, Reference Ren2015). In contrast, CVRs have been combined with various types of questionnaires, including yes/no questions (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2002), appropriateness judgment questionnaires (Reference Timpe-Laughlin, Green and OhTimpe-Laughlin et al., 2021), and multiple-choice questionnaires (Reference Chen and LinChen and Lin, 2021).

Three studies employed both CVR and RVR. Reference Robinson and KasperRobinson (1992) combined both types of verbal report with DCTs to investigate Japanese English learners’ pragmatic knowledge in producing refusals. Reference WoodfieldWoodfield (2010) innovatively explored learners’ cognitive processes when they were engaged in DCTs by asking them to conduct verbal reports in pairs. The results showed that paired verbal reports were able to provide insights into learners’ cognitive processes and pragmatic knowledge. In addition, paired CVRs were easier and more natural for learners than single-subject CVRs. Following Reference WoodfieldWoodfield (2010), Reference ChenChen (2015) also combined the two types of verbal report to explore learners’ cognitive processes while they were completing an email task.

5.2 Pragmatic Aspects Examined with Cognitive Processes

Research has employed RVRs to investigate learners’ cognitive processes during their production of speech acts, among which requests (e.g., Reference ChenChen, 2015; Reference Cohen and OlshtainCohen and Olshtain, 1993; Reference WoodfieldWoodfield, 2010, Reference Woodfield, Felix-Brasdefer and Koike2012) and refusals (e.g., Reference BellaBella, 2014b; Reference Félix-BrasdeferFélix-Brasdefer, 2008a; Reference RenRen, 2014) have been the most frequently examined. Other speech acts that have been examined include apology (Reference BellaBella, 2014a; Reference Cohen and OlshtainCohen and Olshtain, 1993), complaint (Reference Cohen and OlshtainCohen and Olshtain, 1993; Reference Hassall, Alcon Soler and Martinez FlorHassall, 2008), criticism and responses (Reference NguyenNguyen, 2017), feedback (Reference Youn and BiYoun and Bi, 2019), greeting responses (Reference Ying and RenYing and Ren, 2021), and offers (Reference BellaBella, 2016).

A couple of studies have employed CVRs to explore learners’ cognitive processes while they are performing comprehension tasks involving conversational implicature (Reference Chen and LinChen and Lin, 2021; Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2008b). Reference Timpe-Laughlin and ChoTimpe-Laughlin and Cho (2021) used CVRs to investigate the effectiveness of a learning platform in developing EFL learners’ awareness of requests and offers.

5.3 Verbal Report Data Examined

Most L2 pragmatics research on cognitive processes has collected and analyzed data based on Reference Newell and SimonNewell and Simon’s (2019 [Reference Newell and Simon1972]) theory of problem-solving (e.g., Reference RenRen, 2015; Reference WoodfieldWoodfield, 2010) in order to explore the three types of problem-solving processing posited by the model: orient, solve, and evaluation. The first of these, orientation, is often investigated by asking about learners’ cognition or attention through questions such as: What did you notice about the situation? What were you paying attention to? What were you focusing on? (e.g., Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler, 2012; Reference RenRen, 2014; Reference WoodfieldWoodfield, 2010). Second, solution is often investigated by asking about learners’ intentions or planning through questions such as: What did you intend to convey? (Reference ChenChen, 2015) What were you trying to achieve? (Reference BellaBella, 2016) Why did you choose the answer? (Reference TaguchiTaguchi, 2008b) What made you reply in this manner? (Reference RenRen, 2014) To what extent was it important for you to be direct or indirect when you replied in this situation? (Reference RenRen, 2015) Can you explain your decision-making process? (e.g., Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler, 2012; Reference Félix-BrasdeferFélix-Brasdefer, 2008a). The third stage, evaluation, is often investigated by asking learners to evaluate the appropriateness of their own pragmatic performance through questions such as: Were you satisfied with your answer? (Reference Alcón SolerAlcón Soler, 2012; Reference WoodfieldWoodfield, 2010) How would you evaluate your performance in … ? (Reference ChenChen, 2015; Reference Ying and RenYing and Ren, 2021).

Some studies have been interested in learners’ language of thought, asking learners to report on the language they used in planning and executing their performance (e.g., Reference Félix-BrasdeferFélix-Brasdefer, 2008a; Reference RenRen, 2015; Reference WoodfieldWoodfield, 2010). In addition, studies have explored learners’ difficulties and strategy use (e.g., Reference RenRen, 2014; Reference Youn and BiYoun and Bi, 2019) in completing pragmatic tasks based on the collected data.

5.4 Procedures of Verbal Report

When learners are asked to report their cognitive processes, either by CVR or RVR, they must be offered training and allowed to practice. Researchers should ask learners specific questions and avoid asking them cognitively complex questions. For example, questions asking learners to explain complicated issues or to compare abstract concepts such as “Which social parameter carries more weight?” should be avoided.

The time span between RVR and pragmatic tasks, whether they are production or perception tasks, should be kept as short as possible. The majority of L2 pragmatics studies conduct RVR immediately after the pragmatic task, which is highly recommended. In cases where RVR cannot be conducted immediately, it is suggested that it should be carried out within two days (Reference Ericsson and SimonEricsson and Simon, 1993). In addition, learners’ performance in the pragmatic task should be replayed to them to help them recall what they did during the task. Researchers should avoid checking learners’ memories, since questions asking learners to recall what they were thinking during data collection in the past can cause a serious validity threat. If researchers wish to investigate changes in learners’ cognitive processes, RVRs should be conducted after each session, as in Reference RenRen (2014). It is not recommended to conduct RVR only in the last phase of data collection if the intention is to explore pragmatic variation across different phases, as Reference WoodfieldWoodfield (2010) admits in her discussion of limitations.

With respect to the mode of RVR, Reference Hernández and BoeroHernández and Boero (2018) carried out the only study to date that asked learners to write down their thoughts while they were completing pragmatic tasks. As writing is usually much slower than speaking and only offers learners the chance to access their conscious thoughts, it may involve further threats to validity. Future research could investigate the effects of modality on the data collected by verbal report.

5.5 Summary

Verbalization of learners’ cognitive processes can provide insights into the reasoning and thought processes behind their performance (e.g., production of certain pragmatic features, awareness of speech acts, and comprehension of implicature). When carefully designed, verbal report data on learners’ cognitive process can reveal useful information including the pragmatic difficulties they encounter, the source of knowledge they have, the perception of different instruction approaches and measurement tools, and whether their performance resulted from the lack of pragmatic development or their pragmatic agency. Therefore, investigating learners’ cognitive process during pragmatic activities can provide added value for advancing our understanding of L2 pragmatic development. In sum, cognitive process research can integrate studies on learning, teaching, and assessing different aspects of learners’ pragmatic production and perception to provide a comprehensive picture of L2 pragmatic competence.

6 Case Studies

This section presents two projects in progress, both focusing on pragmatic production (Section 2.1). The first case study explores advanced L2 Chinese learners’ self-praise on social media, in which, in addition to linguistic resource, learners’ semiotic and multimodal resources are also included in coding self-praise strategies. The second case study investigates ethnic minority children’s pragmatic competence, which challenges the concept of monolingual native speakers in L2 pragmatics and provides more empirical data to research on children’s pragmatic development. In showcasing these two studies, we call for more research on L2 pragmatics on social media and on lingua franca pragmatics among children.

6.1 Case Study 1: Second Language Self-praise on Social Media

As discussed in Section 2.3, L2 pragmatics has mainly focused on oral communication, with communication in other modalities being examined less frequently. The field needs to expand its research scope to include more pragmatic features, particularly concerning semiotic and multimodal resources. It is important to examine whether L2 learners can successfully achieve meaning making on social media with the increasingly universal digital affordance. Against this background, I designed a project to investigate instances of self-praise posted on social media by advanced learners of Chinese. Self-praise is a speech act through which interlocutors present positive content of themselves such as appearance, possession, and skills (Reference Ren and GuoRen and Guo, 2020). The reason why self-praise was chosen as the research target is that the phenomenon is pervasive in individuals’ daily lives as well as on social media, but it has not been examined from an L2 pragmatics perspective.

Participants and Data

Data were collected from twenty advanced learners of Chinese, who were asked to share self-praise they posted on WeChat Friend, a popular social media platform in China. The learners were aged from twenty to twenty-six years old (M = 21.42, SD = 1.29), of whom nine were from Thailand, seven from Indonesia, and two each from Mongolia and Laos. They had studied in China for more than three and a half years at the time of data collection and were enrolled in advanced Chinese classes at a university in South China. They contributed different numbers of self-praise instances, ranging from one to five. Altogether sixty-one posts containing instances of self-praise in L2 Chinese were collected from the learners.

To provide a comparison dataset, I also collected 200 self-praise posts from WeChat Friend from 46 Chinese native speaker volunteers aged from nineteen to twenty-five (M = 21.8, SD = 1.622) who were studying in various universities across China.

Data Analysis

The self-praise posts were analyzed in terms of different pragmatic strategies based on the coding scheme developed by Reference Ren and GuoRen and Guo (2020), with considerations of linguistic, semiotic, and multimodal resources used in the posts. The self-praise posts were read through in several rounds and coded into three major categories: explicit self-praise without modification, modified explicit self-praise, and implicit self-praise. Instances of modified explicit self-praise were further coded into change of praise focus, collectivism, comparison of oneself between one aspect and another, comparison of oneself between past and present, comparison between oneself and others, disclaimer, praise from a third party, and reference to hard work; implicit self-praise instances were further coded into self-praise as a complaint, self-praise as a narration, and self-praise as sharing (see Reference Ren and GuoRen and Guo, 2020 for definitions and examples of each coding).

During the data coding process it was found that some posts contained a mixture of strategies. In such cases, each independent self-praise strategy received a separate coding. Consequently, the 61 self-praise posts shared by the advanced L2 Chinese learners yielded a total of 64 self-praise strategies, and the 200 self-praise posts shared by the Chinese native speakers yielded 205 self-praise strategies.

Two researchers coded the self-praise posts to establish the interrater reliability of the coding. The agreement percentage was 98.36 percent with a kappa coefficient of 0.943, indicating a high interrater reliability.

Results

Due to space limitations, the results can only be introduced very briefly, without delving into statistical analyses and qualitative examinations related to each self-praise strategy. Table 1 presents the frequency and percentage of all the self-praise strategies across the two groups.

Table 1 Frequency and percentage of self-praise strategies

Self-praise strategiesL2 users (Total post = 61)Native speakers (Total post = 200)
FrequencyPercentageFrequencyPercentage
Explicit self-praise without modification3148.446230.24
Modified self-praise1929.694923.90
Change of praise focus11.56125.85
Collectivism11.5631.46
Comparison between aspects of oneself812.5000
Comparison between past and present11.5652.44
Comparison between oneself and others23.1300
Disclaimer0010.49
Praise from a third party69.38157.32
Reference to hard work00136.34
Implicit self-praise1421.879445.86
Self-praise as complaint23.1320.98
Self-praise as narration1117.1841.95
Self-praise as sharing11.568842.93
Total64100205100

As shown in Table 1, on WeChat Friend, which is accessible to the user’s real-life WeChat friends, the L2 learners preferred praising themselves explicitly without modification (48.44%) (e.g., “I feel like a star.”), followed by modified self-praise (29.69%) (e.g., “A lot of people say that I’m very smart.”) and implicit self-praise (21.87%) (e.g., “How annoying! Why do others only see my lovely face but do not know that I also have a smart brain behind it?). In contrast, the Chinese native speakers tended to use implicit self-praise much more frequently (45.86%), followed by explicit self-praise without modification (30.24%) and with modification (23.90%).

It is possible that the learners had not fully developed their pragmatic competence, which led to the divergence of the two groups’ self-praise. However, it is also possible that the learners would like to perform their agency in the way of positively presenting themselves. Future studies need to include retrospective verbal reports (see Section 5) or interviews to explore whether the (non-)employment of certain strategies resulted from learners’ agency or lack of sociopragmatic and pragmalinguistic knowledge. In this case study, the learners had all been studying abroad in China for a few years. As Chinese universities provide dormitories specifically for international students (Reference Ren, Sanz and Morales-FrontRen, 2018a), it is likely that the learners had forged a group of WeChat friends of whom the majority were international students. They might have formed a local group ritual of posting for clarity (Reference RenRen, 2014) and explicitly sharing happiness, ease, and positive aspects of life.

This case study exemplifies the benefits of investigating social media communication. Researchers have highlighted the importance of exploring authentic data with real-life consequences (Reference Bardovi-Harlig, Hartford, Bardovi-Harlig and HartfordBardovi-Harlig and Hartford, 2005; Reference Cohen, Félix-Brasdefer and KoikeFélix-Brasdefer and Cohen, 2012), which cannot be achieved using DCTs and roleplays. Social media presents researchers with an ideal platform in this sense. In addition, more research on pragmatic aspects of social media communication, which have previously been investigated in oral communication, can shed light on the similarities and differences between face-to-face and social media communication in terms of learner pragmatic competence.

6.2 Case Study 2: Lingua Franca Pragmatics

Lingua franca pragmatics refers to the pragmatic practice of ethnic minority people in their national lingua franca. It is different from pragmatics in English as a lingua franca (e.g., Reference RenRen, 2018b). As pointed out earlier, L2 pragmatics has predominantly examined adult learners. Children’s pragmatic competence, particularly that of ethnic minority children in a multilingual country or area, has rarely been explored. Therefore, I developed a project to investigate Chinese ethnic minority children’s pragmatic competence in Mandarin, the national lingua franca in China. I collected data from local children in primary schools, junior high schools, and senior high schools in ethnic minority areas in China including Xinjiang, Tibet, and Inner Mongolia. Due to space limitations, here I only report on one study examining Uyghur-Mandarin children’s pragmatic competence when giving advice in written communication, to provide more data to the under-researched written pragmatic competence (see Section 2.3).

Participants and Data

A total of 200 children participated in this case study: 100 bilingual Uyghur-Mandarin children, and 100 monolingual Han-Mandarin children, equally distributed according to gender. The Uyghur-Mandarin children were aged from ten to thirteen years (M = 11.64, SD = 1.14), and all were learning Mandarin as the national lingua franca in their formal schooling context. The Han children were also aged between ten and thirteen years (M = 11.46, SD = 1.08), but they had learned Mandarin as their L1. The 200 participants were all sixth-grade students in elementary schools in the Xinjiang Uygur Autonomous Region of China at the time of data collection.

The study was purposefully designed to investigate the children’s ability to write a note. Therefore, a written DCT was used to elicit the speech act of advice. To ensure that the children could understand the task, the scenario was designed to be familiar within the context of the children’s life; it presented a situation where a child’s mother was trying to lose weight by eating vegetables alone every day. The children were required to write a note to their mother and advise her to change her unhealthy diet.

Data Analysis

The children’s responses were coded according to content-based categories, with reference to previous literature (Reference Decapua and DunhamDeCapua and Dunham, 2007; Reference HampelHampel, 2015). After several rounds of careful reading and coding, the children’s responses were coded into advice strategies and supportive moves. The advice strategies consisted of admonition and solution (further divided into single choice and alternatives), while the supportive moves were coded into four types including assessment, compromise, rationale, and affective expression, with the last one being further divided into pleading, warning, complimentary, and encouraging.

Two researchers coded the children’s responses to ensure the reliability of the data. The agreement percentage was 96.42 percent with a kappa coefficient of 0.829, which indicated a high interrater reliability.

Results

Table 2 shows the frequency and percentage of each advice strategy and supportive move used by the Uyghur-Mandarin children and the Han children.

Table 2 Frequency and percentage of advice strategies and supportive moves

UyghurHan
FrequencyPercentageFrequencyPercentage
Advice Strategies13449.0812145.15
Admonition5219.053814.18
Solution8230.048330.97
Single choice5118.684416.42
Alternatives3111.363914.55
Supportive Moves13950.9214754.85
Assessment4416.123914.55
Compromise134.76176.34
Rationale6222.716825.37
Affective expression207.33238.58
Pleading20.7300
Warning134.76197.09
Compliment51.8300
Encouraging0041.49
Total273100268100

As shown in Table 2, the Uyghur-Mandarin children and the Han children revealed a similar profile with respect to the advice strategies and supportive moves they used while writing a note to their mother in this hypothetical situation. The Uyghur-Mandarin used slightly more advice strategies and fewer supportive moves than the Han children (134 vs. 121 and 139 vs. 147, respectively). In terms of specific advice strategies, the Uyghur-Mandarin children used more admonition strategies than the Han children, but the difference was not significant (p = 0.59). The two groups of children used similar numbers of solution strategies, although they showed different preferences in offering single choices or alternatives. The Uyghur-Mandarin children used single choices more frequently and alternatives less frequently than the Han children (51 vs. 44 and 31 vs. 39, respectively), but the differences were not significant.

With respect to supportive moves, it was interesting to note that, when showing affection toward their mother, the Uyghur-Mandarin children used pleading and compliments, but the Han children did not use these two types of supportive moves; on the other hand, the Han children used more warnings and encouragement than the Uyghur-Mandarin children. These findings indicate that the Uyghur-Mandarin preferred to show more involvement politeness to their mothers while writing the advice note than the Han children, who paid more attention to their mothers’ independence politeness (Reference Ren and WoodfieldRen and Woodfield, 2016; Reference Scollon and ScollonScollon and Scollon, 2001). However, this finding warrants more exploration, as the dataset was rather small and the findings were far from conclusive.

Overall, the results revealed more similarities than differences between the ethnic minority children and the monolingual native speaker children. The bilingual ethnic minority children had much more in common with the native speaker children than the advanced L2 learners documented in Case Study 1 and in the previous literature. Case Study 2 exemplifies the need to investigate lingua franca pragmatics, which has been neglected in L2 pragmatics. Lingua franca pragmatics is different from foreign/second language pragmatics, heritage pragmatics, and pragmatics in English as a lingua franca. Ethnic minority children learn a nationally dominant language not only as their L2 but also as the national lingua franca, which is of significant importance if they wish to communicate with the majority community, and crucially it is also owned by them. Therefore, they are highly motivated to learn the language and invest their own agency in using it to perform their identities. Investigating lingua franca pragmatics will provide a novel and comprehensive understanding of L2 pragmatic competence and development. This case study asked the children to write in Mandarin Chinese, their lingual franca; future studies may explore the children’s entire linguistic repertoires and their translanguaging competence in pragmatic practice. Indeed, pragmatics of young language learners is a particularly under-studied area within L2 pragmatics (but see Reference CekaiteCekaite, 2017; Reference LeeLee, 2010; Reference RoseRose, 2000; Reference Savić, Economidou-Kogetsidis and MyrsetSavić et al., 2021; Reference SchauerSchauer, 2019 for some exceptions), which deserves more attention in the field.

7 Advancing Second Language Pragmatics

As the above sections have shown, L2 pragmatics covers many topics in learning, teaching, and assessing, and investigates a range of pragmatic aspects and possible influential factors. This section will only point out a few potential areas to advance L2 pragmatics research.

Second language pragmatics research tends to investigate specific areas of pragmatic competence (see Section 2) among different learners, which makes the generalization of different studies difficult or even impossible since learners differ across many variables. Although questions concerning commonality versus particularity of pragmatic development and the relation between productive and receptive pragmatic competences have been raised for more than two decades (Reference Kasper and SchmidtKasper and Schmidt, 1996), not many studies have been designed to address such issues. Future research should investigate the interactions among learners’ different pragmatic sub-competences (production, perception, and cognition), and explore their pragmatic competence across different layers at micro (immediate interactional contexts), meso (local communicative norms or rituals), and macro (sociocultural systems) levels (Reference RenRen, 2019a), as illustrated in Figure 2.

Figure 2 Layers of L2 pragmatics research

Some particular topics awaiting further exploration include: whether changes in cognitive processes correspond to development in pragmatic performance; whether learners’ pragmatic development can be sustained after intervention, whether through learning environment (e.g., study abroad) or pedagogical instruction, and the reasons underlying such results; the usefulness of interventions for learners’ pragmatic development and how the development can be facilitated and assessed, particularly if L2 culture contradicts learners’ identities or if other factors inhibit their pragmatic learning; and finally, how to link findings in L2 pragmatics learning, teaching, and assessing. To date, L2 pragmatics has mostly examined university students. Future research should investigate more types of learners, including but not restricted to younger learners, and learners at beginner and advanced levels.

The field will benefit from further well-designed studies from a wider range of L1 background and L2 target languages to explore learners’ individual differences (see Reference Takahashi and TaguchiTakahashi, 2019 for a review), investigating not only their difficulties in using an L2 to achieve pragmatic goals, but also how they achieve their desires by using the pragmatic resources available to them (refer to the three components of pragmatic competence in Section 1). In addition, future research should shift from examining one factor at a time to the simultaneous investigation of multiple dynamic variables, to investigate possible interactions between different variables. Sophisticated statistical methods such as multivariable analysis and Structural Equation Modeling will be useful here. Figure 3 illustrates the focus shift for future research.

Figure 3 Shift of focus for L2 pragmatics research

Another important area is learners’ own understanding of their pragmatics performance and development. Assessment-based pragmatic performance alone is not sufficient. Methods such as verbal reports should be included to triangulate not only what learners can really achieve, but also what motives and meaning are assigned to their performance. In addition, social network analysis may provide illuminating insights into factors influencing learners’ pragmatic learning and behaviors (e.g., Reference Li, Li and RenLi et al., 2021).

With respect to pragmatics teaching, as reviewed in Section 3, future research should focus on how to teach pragmatics more effectively and efficiently, rather than on whether pragmatics could or should be taught. Length of instruction, instructional delivery, as well as multiple delayed test stages to determine at what point the benefits of instruction start to wane are underexplored and warrant more specific attention. The majority of L2 pragmatics teaching studies have been conducted in face-to-face contexts. It is vital to investigate how to develop learners’ pragmatic competence via technology (see Reference González-LloretGonzález-Lloret, 2021 for a recent review), particularly in the context of the COVID-19 crisis. It is encouraging that some studies have already explored this issue (e.g., Reference Blyth and SykesBlyth and Sykes, 2020; Reference Taguchi and SykesTaguchi and Sykes, 2013), but it is worth further exploring how to design tasks using technology to assist learners developing pragmatic competence out of class. Also, Reference Bardovi-Harlig, Mossman and SuBardovi-Harlig et al. (2017) showed the positive effect of using a corpus on learners’ oral production and comprehension of pragmatic routines. They highlighted the importance of providing guidance and consciousness-raising activities for learners. However, corpora with annotations for pragmatic categories are still rare. It will be illuminating to explore how teachers can train learners to use the various corpora that are available to improve their pragmatic competence, and how to assess their learning and improvement during this process.

A final aspect is related to offering legitimacy for learners to employ all their available multilingual, multi-semiotic, and multimodal resources in their translanguaging practice (Reference LiGarcía and Li, 2014). As being multilingual is increasingly accepted as the norm rather than an exception, multilingual learners should be treated as a whole person (Reference García, Flores and SeltzerGarcía et al., 2021). Therefore, researchers should revisit the methodological assumptions of the conventional research methods in L2 pragmatics that analyze pragmatic strategies separately in quantifiable codes and compare learners’ performance against the monolingual native speaker norm, particularly in fields such as lingua franca pragmatics (see Case Study 2). Rather, L2 pragmatics will develop further by moving forward to investigate learners’ pragmatic competence in a dynamic, complex, and interactive perspective to advance our understanding of the principles and mechanisms of pragmatic development. In addition, more empirical studies are warranted to explore how to codify learners’ translanguaging strategies in interaction (see Case Study 1). It remains to be seen how such practices will come to be acknowledged in foreign language classrooms and large-scale high-stakes exams. A comprehensive coding scheme of learners’ translanguaging strategies will indeed help in this aspect. To conclude, the definition and construct of pragmatic competence consisting of linguistic, semiotic and multimodal resources and knowledge can help to push the boundaries of L2 pragmatics.

Applied Linguistics

  • Li Wei

  • University College London

  • Li Wei is Chair of Applied Linguistics at the UCL Institute of Education, University College London (UCL), and Fellow of Academy of Social Sciences, UK. His research covers different aspects of bilingualism and multilingualism. He was the founding editor of the following journals: International Journal of Bilingualism (Sage), Applied Linguistics Review (De Gruyter), Language, Culture and Society (Benjamins), Chinese Language and Discourse (Benjamins) and Global Chinese (De Gruyter), and is currently Editor of the International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism (Taylor and Francis). His books include the Blackwell Guide to Research Methods in Bilingualism and Multilingualism (with Melissa Moyer) and Translanguaging: Language, Bilingualism and Education (with Ofelia Garcia) which won the British Association of Applied Linguistics Book Prize.

  • Zhu Hua

  • University College London

  • Zhu Hua is Professor of Language Learning and Intercultural Communication at the UCL Institute of Education, University College London (UCL) and is a Fellow of Academy of Social Sciences, UK. Her research is centred around multilingual and intercultural communication. She has also studied child language development and language learning. She is book series co-editor for Routledge Studies in Language and Intercultural Communication and Cambridge Key Topics in Applied Linguistics, and Forum and Book Reviews Editor of Applied Linguistics (Oxford University Press).

About the Series

  • Mirroring the Cambridge Key Topics in Applied Linguistics, this Elements series focuses on the key topics, concepts and methods in Applied Linguistics today. It revisits core conceptual and methodological issues in different subareas of Applied Linguistics. It also explores new emerging themes and topics. All topics are examined in connection with real-world issues and the broader political, economic and ideological contexts.

Applied Linguistics

References

Achiba, M. 2003. Learning to request in a second language: A study of child interlanguage pragmatics. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ahmadian, M. J. 2020. Explicit and implicit instruction of refusal strategies: Does working memory capacity play a role? Language Teaching Research, 24, 163188.Google Scholar
Alcón Soler, E. 2005. Does instruction work for learning pragmatics in the EFL context? System, 33, 417435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcón Soler, E. 2007. Developing pragmatic awareness of suggestions in the EFL classroom: A focus on instructional effects. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 10, 4776.Google Scholar
Alcón Soler, E. 2012. Teachability and bilingualism effects on third language learners’ pragmatic knowledge. Intercultural Pragmatics, 9, 511541.Google Scholar
Alcón Soler, E. 2018. Effects of task supported language teaching on learners’ use and knowledge of email request mitigators. In Taguchi, N. and Kim, Y. (eds.), Task-based approaches to teaching and assessing pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 5582.Google Scholar
Alcón Soler, E. and Sánchez-Hernández, A. 2017. Learning pragmatic routines during study abroad: A focus on proficiency and type of routine. Atlantis, 39, 191210.Google Scholar
Al-Ali, M. N. and Alawneh, R. 2010. Linguistic mitigating devices in American and Jordanian students’ requests. Intercultural Pragmatics, 7, 311339.Google Scholar
Allami, H. and Naeimi, A. 2011. A cross-linguistic study of refusals: An analysis of pragmatic competence development in Iranian EFL learners. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 385406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alsuhaibani, Z. 2020. Developing EFL students’ pragmatic competence: The case of compliment responses. Language Teaching Research, 120.Google Scholar
Badjadi, N. E. I. 2016. A meta-analysis of the effects of instructional tasks on L2 pragmatics comprehension and production. In Tang, S. F. and Logonnathan, L. (eds.), Assessment for learning within and beyond the classroom. Singapore: Springer, 241268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2005. Contextualizing interlanguage pragmatics. In Tyler, A., Takada, M., Kim, Y. and Marinova, D. (eds.), Language in use: Cognitive and discourse perspectives on language and language learning. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, 6584.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2008. Recognition and production of formulas in L2 pragmatics. In Han, Z. (ed.), Understanding second language process. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, 205222.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2009. Conventional expressions as a pragmalinguistic resource: Recognition and production of conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. Language Learning, 59, 755795.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2012. Formulas, routines, and conventional expressions in pragmatics research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 32, 206227.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2014. Awareness of meaning of conventional expressions in second-language pragmatics. Language Awareness, 23, 4156.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2018. Matching modality in L2 pragmatics research design. System, 75, 1322.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Bastos, M.-T. 2011. Proficiency, length of stay, and intensity of interaction, and the acquisition of conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. Intercultural Pragmatics, 8, 347384.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Dörnyei, Z. 1998. Do language learners recognize pragmatic violations? Pragmatic versus grammatical awareness in instructed L2 learning. TESOL Quarterly, 32, 233263.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Griffin, R. 2005. L2 pragmatic awareness: Evidence from the ESL classroom. System, 33, 401415.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Hartford, B. 1993. Learning the rules of academic talk: A longitudinal study of pragmatic development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 279304.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Hartford, B. 2005. Institutional discourse and interlanguage pragmatics research. In Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Hartford, B. (eds.), Interlanguage pragmatics: Exploring institutional talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 736.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K., Mossman, S., and Su, Y. 2017. The effect of corpus-based instruction on pragmatic routines. Language Learning & Technology, 21, 76103.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Su, Y. 2018. The acquisition of conventional expressions as a pragmalinguistic resource in Chinese as a foreign language. The Modern Language Journal, 102, 732757.Google Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and Vellenga, H. E. 2012. The effect of instruction on conventional expressions in L2 pragmatics. System, 40, 7789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barón, J. and Ortega, M. 2018. Investigating age differences in e-mail pragmatic performance. System, 78, 148158.Google Scholar
Barón Parés, J. L. 2012. “Please, please, please”: Trying to be polite in an EFL context. In Amaya, L. F., López, M. D. L. O. H., Morón, R. G., et al. (eds.), New perspectives on (im)politeness and interpersonal communication. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 174197.Google Scholar
Barón Parés, J. L. 2015. “Can I make a party, mum?” The development of requests from childhood to adolescence. Atlantis, 37, 179198.Google Scholar
Barron, A. 2003. Acquisition in interlanguage pragmatics: Learning how to do things with words in a study abroad context. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Barron, A. 2019. Using corpus-linguistic methods to track longitudinal development: Routine apologies in the study abroad context. Journal of Pragmatics, 146, 87105.Google Scholar
Barron, A. and Black, E. 2015. Constructing small talk in learner-native speaker voice-based telecollaboration: A focus on topic management and back-channeling. System, 48, 112128.Google Scholar
Béal, C. 1994. Keeping the peace: A cross-cultural comparison of questions and requests in Australian English and French. Multilingua, 13, 3558.Google Scholar
Beebe, L. M. and Cummings, M. 1996. Natural speech data versus written questionnaire data: How data collection method affects speech act performance. In Gass, S. and Neu, J. (eds.), Speech acts across cultures: Challenges to communication in a second language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 6586.Google Scholar
Beebe, L. M., Takahashi, T., and Uliss-Weltz, R. 1990. Pragmatic transfer in ESL refusals. In Scarcella, R. C., Anderson, E. and Krashen, S. D. (eds.), Developing communicative competence in a second language. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, 5573.Google Scholar
Bell, N., Shardakova, M., and Shively, R. L. 2021. The DCT as a data collection method for L2 humor production. In Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. and Shively, R. L. (eds.), New directions in second language pragmatics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 156178.Google Scholar
Bella, S. 2011. Mitigation and politeness in Greek invitation refusals: Effects of length of residence in the target community and intensity of interaction on non-native speakers’ performance. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 17181740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bella, S. 2012a. Pragmatic awareness in a second language setting: The case of L2 learners of Greek. Multilingua, 31, 133.Google Scholar
Bella, S. 2012b. Pragmatic development in a foreign language: A study of Greek FL requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 19171947.Google Scholar
Bella, S. 2014a. A contrastive study of apologies performed by Greek native speakers and English learners of Greek as a foreign language. Pragmatics, 24, 679713.Google Scholar
Bella, S. 2014b. Developing the ability to refuse: A cross-sectional study of Greek FL refusals. Journal of Pragmatics, 61, 3562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bella, S. 2016. Offers by Greek FL learners: A cross-sectional developmental study. Pragmatics, 26, 531562.Google Scholar
Belz, J. A. and Kinginger, C. 2002. The cross-linguistic development of address form use in telecollaborative language learning: Two case studies. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 59, 189214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billmyer, K. and Varghese, M. 2000. Investigating instrument-based pragmatic variability: Effects of enhancing discourse completion tests. Applied Linguistics, 21, 517552.Google Scholar
Blattner, G. and Fiori, M. 2012. Virtual social network communities: An investigation of language learners’ development of sociopragmatic awareness and multiliteracy skills. CALICO Journal, 29, 2443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blood, R. 2018. “When you speak to a police officer and (call them) du”: Examining the impact of short-term study abroad on Australian students’ awareness of address forms in German. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education, 3, 117143.Google Scholar
Blum-Kulka, S., House, J., and Kasper, G. (eds.) 1989. Cross-cultural pragmatics: Requests and apologies. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Blyth, C. and Sykes, J. M. 2020. Technology-enhanced L2 instructional pragmatics. Language Learning & Technology, 24, 17.Google Scholar
Bouton, L. F. 1994. Conversational implicature in the second language: Learned slowly when not deliberately taught. Journal of Pragmatics, 22, 157167.Google Scholar
Brown, J. D. 2001. Pragmatics tests: Different purposes, different tests. In Rose, K. R. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Pragmatics in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 301325.Google Scholar
Brown, J. D. 2008. Raters, functions, item types and the dependability of L2 pragmatics tests. In Alcón Soler, E. and Martínez-Flor, A. (eds.), Second language acquisition: Investigating pragmatics in foreign language learning, teaching and testing. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 224248.Google Scholar
Brown, L. 2013. Identity and honorifics use in Korean study abroad. In Kinginger, C. (ed.), Social and cultural aspects of language learning in study abroad. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 269298.Google Scholar
Brown, P. and Levinson, S. C. 1987. Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Buysse, L. 2012. So as a multifunctional discourse marker in native and learner speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 17641782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buysse, L. 2015. “Well it’s not very ideal … ” the pragmatic marker well in learner English. Intercultural Pragmatics, 12, 5989.Google Scholar
Cekaite, A. 2017. What makes a child a good language learner? Interactional competence, identity, and immersion in a Swedish classroom. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 37, 4561.Google Scholar
Chang, Y.-F. 2010. “I no say you say is boring”: The development of pragmatic competence in L2 apology. Language Sciences, 32, 408424.Google Scholar
Chang, Y.-F. 2011. Refusing in a foreign language: An investigation of problems encountered by Chinese learners of English. Multilingua, 30, 7198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, R. 2020. Single author self-reference: Identity construction and pragmatic competence. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 45, 114.Google Scholar
Chen, X. and Dewaele, J.-M. 2019. The relationship between English proficiency and humour appreciation among English L1 users and Chinese L2 users of English. Applied Linguistics Review, 10, 653676.Google Scholar
Chen, Y.-S. 2015. Chinese learners’ cognitive processes in writing email requests to faculty. System, 52, 5162.Google Scholar
Chen, Y.-S. and Lin, M.-F. 2021. Effects of peer collaboration on EFL learners’ comprehension of conversational implicatures. System, 97, 112.Google Scholar
Chen, Y.-S. and Liu, J. 2016. Constructing a scale to assess L2 written speech act performance: WDCT and e-mail tasks. Language Assessment Quarterly, 13, 231250.Google Scholar
Chen, Y.-S., Rau, D.-H. V., and Rau, G. (eds.) 2016. Email discourse among Chinese using English as a lingua franca. Singapore: Springer.Google Scholar
Cho, C. M. and Dewaele, J.-M. 2021. A crosslinguistic study of the perception of emotional intonation: Influence of the pitch modulations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 43, 870895.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. D. 1998. Strategies in learning and using a second language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. D. 2012. Research methods for describing variation in intercultural pragmatics for cultures in contact and conflict. In Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. and Koike, D. A. (eds.), Pragmatic variation in first and second language contexts: Methodological issues. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 271294.Google Scholar
Cohen, A. D. and Olshtain, E. 1993. The production of speech acts by EFL learners. TESOL Quarterly, 27, 3356.Google Scholar
Cook, M. and Liddicoat, A. J. 2002. The development of comprehension in interlanguage pragmatics: The case of request strategies in English. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 1939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D. 1997. The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cunningham, D. J. 2017. Methodological innovation for the study of request production in telecollaboration. Language learning & Technology, 21, 7699.Google Scholar
Czerwionka, L. and Cuza, A. 2017. A pragmatic analysis of L2 Spanish requests: Acquisition in three situational contexts during short-term study abroad. Intercultural Pragmatics, 14, 391419.Google Scholar
De Cristofaro, E. and Badan, L. 2021. The acquisition of Italian discourse markers as a function of studying abroad. Corpus Pragmatics, 5, 95120.Google Scholar
Decapua, A. and Dunham, J. F. 2007. The pragmatics of advice giving: Cross-cultural perspectives. Intercultural Pragmatics, 4, 319342.Google Scholar
Diao, W. 2016. Peer socialization into gendered L2 Mandarin practices in a study abroad context: Talk in the dorm. Applied Linguistics, 37, 599620.Google Scholar
Diao, W. and Chen, C. 2021. L2 use of pragmatic markers in peer talk: Mandarin utterance-final particles. IRAL.Google Scholar
Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. 2013. Strategies, modification and perspective in native speakers’ requests: A comparison of WDCT and naturally occurring requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 53, 2138.Google Scholar
Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. 2016. Variation in evaluations of (im)politeness of emails from L2 learners and perceptions of the personality of their senders. Journal of Pragmatics, 106, 119.Google Scholar
Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. 2018. “Mr paul, please inform me accordingly”: Address forms, directness and degree of imposition in L2 emails. Pragmatics, 28, 489515.Google Scholar
Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. and Halenko, N. 2022. Developing spoken requests during UK study abroad: A longitudinal look at Japanese learners of English. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education, 7, 2454.Google Scholar
Economidou-Kogetsidis, M., Savic, M., and Halenko, N. 2021. Email pragmatics and second language learners. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. and Woodfield, H. (eds.) 2012. Interlanguage request modification. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Edmonds, A. 2014. Conventional expressions: Investigating pragmatics and processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36, 6999.Google Scholar
El-Dakhs, D. a. S. 2018. Investigating the apology strategies of Saudi learners of English: Foreign language learning in focus. Pragmatics and Society, 9, 598625.Google Scholar
Ellis, R. 1992. Learning to communicate in the classroom: A study of two language learners’ requests. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14, 123.Google Scholar
Ellis, R., Zhu, Y., Shintani, N., and Roever, C. 2021. A study of Chinese learners’ ability to comprehend irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 172, 720.Google Scholar
Ericsson, A. and Simon, H. 1993. Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eslami, Z. R., Mirzaei, A., and Dini, S. 2015. The role of asynchronous computer mediated communication in the instruction and development of EFL learners’ pragmatic competence. System, 48, 99111.Google Scholar
Fakher Ajabshir, Z. 2019. The effect of synchronous and asynchronous computer-mediated communication (CMC) on EFL learners’ pragmatic competence. Computers in Human Behavior, 9, 169177.Google Scholar
Fakher Ajabshir, Z. 2022. The relative efficacy of input enhancement, input flooding, and output-based instructional approaches in the acquisition of L2 request modifiers. Language Teaching Research, 26(3), 411433.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. 2003. Declining an invitation: A cross-cultural study of pragmatic strategies in American English and Latin American Spanish. Multilingua, 22, 225255.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. 2004. Interlanguage refusals: Linguistic politeness and length of residence in the target community. Language Learning, 54, 587653.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. 2008a. Perceptions of refusals to invitations: Exploring the minds of foreign language learners. Language Awareness, 17, 195211.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. 2008b. Teaching pragmatics in the classroom: Instruction of mitigation in Spanish as a foreign language. Hispania, 91, 479494.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. 2013. Refusing in L2 Spanish: The effects of the context of learning during a short-term study abroad program. In Martí-Arnándiz, O. and Salazar-Campillo, P. (eds.), Refusals in instructed contexts and beyond. Amsterdam: Rodopi, 147173.Google Scholar
Félix-Brasdefer, J. C. and Cohen, A. D. 2012. Teaching pragmatics in the foreign language classroom: Grammar as a communicative resource. Hispania, 95, 650669.Google Scholar
Fernández-Polo, F. J. 2014. The role of I mean in conference presentation by ELF speakers. English for Specific Purposes, 34, 5867.Google Scholar
Fordyce, K. 2014. The differential effects of explicit and implicit instruction on EFL learners’ use of epistemic stance. Applied Linguistics, 35, 628.Google Scholar
Fukuya, Y. J. and Martínez-Flor, A. 2008. The interactive effects of pragmatic-eliciting tasks and pragmatic instruction. Foreign Language Annals, 41, 478500.Google Scholar
Furniss, E. A. 2016. Teaching the pragmatics of Russian conversation using a corpus referred website. Language Learning & Technology, 20, 3860.Google Scholar
Gablasova, D., Brezina, V., Mcenery, T., and Boyd, E. 2017. Epistemic stance in spoken L2 English: The effect of task and speaker style. Applied Linguistics, 38, 613637.Google Scholar
Al-Gahtani, S. and Roever, C. 2012. Proficiency and sequential organization of L2 requests. Applied Linguistics, 33, 4265.Google Scholar
Al-Gahtani, S. and Roever, C. 2014. Preference structure in L2 Arabic requests. Intercultural Pragmatics, 11, 619643.Google Scholar
Al-Gahtani, S. and Roever, C. 2018. Proficiency and preference organization in second language refusals. Journal of Pragmatics, 129, 140153.Google Scholar
García García, M. 2021. Turn-initial discourse markers in L2 Spanish conversations: Insights from conversation analysis. Corpus Pragmatics, 5, 3761.Google Scholar
García, O., Flores, N., Seltzer, K., et al. 2021. Rejecting abyssal thinking in the language and education of racialized bilinguals: A manifesto. Critical Inquiry in Language Studies, 126.Google Scholar
García, O. and Li, W. 2014. Translanguaging: Language, bilingualism and education. Basingstoke: Palgrave Pivot.Google Scholar
Garcia, P. 2004. Developmental differences in speech act recognition: A pragmatic awareness study. Language Awareness, 13, 96115.Google Scholar
García-Gómez, A. 2020. Learning through WhatsApp: Students’ beliefs, L2 pragmatic development and interpersonal relationships. Computer Assisted Language Learning.Google Scholar
García-Pastor, M. D. 2020. Researching identity and L2 pragmatics in digital stories: A relational account. CALICO Journal, 37, 4665.Google Scholar
Gass, S. and Houck, N. 1999. Interlanguage refusals: A cross-cultural study of Japanese-English. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Gass, S. and Mackey, A. 2016. Stimulated recall methodology in applied linguistics and L2 research. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Geyer, N. 2007. Self-qualification in L2 Japanese: An interface of pragmatic, grammatical, and discourse competences. Language Learning, 57, 337367.Google Scholar
Ghavamnia, M., Tavakoli, M., and Rezazadeh, M. 2012. A comparative study of requests among L2 English, L1 Persian, and L1 English speakers. Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics, 11, 105123.Google Scholar
Ghobadi, A. and Fahim, M. 2009. The effect of explicit teaching of English “thanking formulas” on Iranian EFL intermediate level students at English language institutes. System, 37, 526537.Google Scholar
Gilabert, R. and Barón, J. 2013. The impact of increasing task complexity on L2 pragmatic moves. In Mcdonough, K. and Mackey, A. (eds.) Second language interaction in diverse education contexts. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 4570.Google Scholar
Gilabert, R. and Barón, J. 2018. Independently measuring cognitive complexity in task design for interlanguage pragmatics development. In Taguchi, N. and Kim, Y. (eds.), Task-based approaches to teaching and assessing pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 159190.Google Scholar
Glaser, K. 2016. News from the pragmatics classroom: Contrasting the inductive and the deductive approach in the teaching of pragmatic competence. Intercultural Pragmatics, 13, 529561.Google Scholar
Golato, A. 2003. Studying compliment responses: A comparison of DCTs and recordings of naturally occurring talk. Applied Linguistics, 24, 90121.Google Scholar
Gomez-Laich, M. P. 2018. Task complexity effects on interaction during a collaborative persuasive writing task: A conversation analytic perspective. In Taguchi, N. and Kim, Y. (eds.), Task-based approaches to teaching and assessing pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 83112.Google Scholar
González-Lloret, M. 2020. Pragmatic development in L2: An overview. In Schneider, K. P. and Ifantidou, E. (eds.) Developmental and clinical pragmatics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 237267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
González-Lloret, M. 2021. L2 pragmatics and CALL. Language Learning & Technology, 25, 90105.Google Scholar
GonzáLez-Lloret, M. 2019. Task-based language teaching and L2 pragmatics. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and pragmatics. New York: Routledge, 338352.Google Scholar
Grice, P. 1989. Studies in the ways of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guillot, M.-N. 2009. Interruption in advanced learner French: Issues of pragmatic discrimination. Languages in Contrast, 9, 98123.Google Scholar
Guillot, M.-N. 2012. Conversational management and pragmatic discrimination in foreign talk: Overlap in advanced L2 French. Intercultural Pragmatics, 9, 307333.Google Scholar
Haastrup, K. 1986. Pragmatic and strategic competence in the assessment of oral proficiency. System, 14, 7179.Google Scholar
Habib, R. 2008. Humor and disagreement: Identity construction and cross-cultural enrichment. Journal of Pragmatics, 40, 11171145.Google Scholar
Haghighi, H., Jafarigohar, M., Khoshsima, H., and Vahdany, F. 2019. Impact of flipped classroom on EFL learners’ appropriate use of refusal: Achievement, participation, perception. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 32, 261293.Google Scholar
Halenko, N. 2021. Teaching pragmatics and instructed second language learning: Study abroad and technology-enhanced teaching. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Hampel, E. 2015. “Mama zimbi, pls help me!”: Gender differences in (im)politeness in Ghanaian English advice-giving on Facebook. Journal of Politeness Research, 11, 99130.Google Scholar
Haselow, A. 2021. The acquisition of pragmatic markers in the foreign language classroom: An experimental study on the effects of implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Pragmatics, 186, 7386.Google Scholar
Hassall, T. 2003. Requests by Australian learners of Indonesian. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 19031928.Google Scholar
Hassall, T. 2008. Pragmatic performance: What are learners thinking? In Alcon Soler, E. and Martinez Flor, A. (eds.), Investigating pragmatics in foreign language learning, teaching and testing. Bristol: Multilingual Matters, 7293.Google Scholar
Hassall, T. 2012. Request modification by Australian learners of Indonesian. In Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. and Woodfield, H. (eds.), Interlanguage request modification. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 203242.Google Scholar
Hassall, T. 2013. Pragmatic development during short-term study abroad: The case of address terms in Indonesian. Journal of Pragmatics, 55, 117.Google Scholar
Hernández, T. A. 2011. Re-examining the role of explicit instruction and input flood on the acquisition of Spanish discourse markers. Language Teaching Research, 15, 159182.Google Scholar
Hernández, T. A. and Boero, P. 2018. Explicit interventions for Spanish pragmatic development during short-term study abroad: An examination of learner request production and cognition. Foreign Language Annals, 51, 389410.Google Scholar
Hinkel, E. 1997. Appropriateness of advice: DCT and multiple choice data. Applied Linguistics, 18, 126.Google Scholar
Holtgraves, T. 2007. Second language learners and speech act comprehension. Language Learning, 57, 595610.Google Scholar
Hong, W. 2011. Refusals in Chinese: How do L1 and L2 differ? Foreign Language Annals, 44, 122136.Google Scholar
Hosoda, Y. and Aline, D. 2022. Deployment of I don’t know and wakannai in second language classroom peer discussions. Text & Talk, 42, 2749.Google Scholar
Hudson, T. 2001. Indicators for pragmatic instruction: Some quantitative measures. In Rose, K. R. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Pragmatics in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 283300.Google Scholar
Hudson, T., Detmer, E., and Brown, J. D. 1992. A framework for testing cross-cultural pragmatics. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai’i, Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.Google Scholar
Hudson, T., Detmer, E., and Brown, J. D. 1995. Developing prototypic measures of cross-cultural pragmatics. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.Google Scholar
Huth, T. 2006. Negotiating structure and culture: L2 learners’ realization of L2 compliment-response sequences in talk-in-interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 20252050.Google Scholar
Huth, T. and Betz, E. 2019. Testing interactional competence in second language classrooms: Goals, formats and caveats. In Salaberry, M. R. and Kunitz, S. (eds.), Teaching and testing L2 interactional competence: Bridging theory and practice. New York: Routledge, 322356.Google Scholar
Hyland, K. 2019. Metadiscourse: Exploring interaction in writing. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Ishida, H. 2006. Learners’ perception and interpretation of contextualization cues in spontaneous Japanese conversation: Back-channel cue Uun. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 19431981.Google Scholar
Ishihara, N. 2019. Identity and agency in L2 pragmatics. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and pragmatics. New York: Routledge, 161175.Google Scholar
Itakura, H. 2002. Gender and pragmatic transfer in topic development. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 15, 161183.Google Scholar
Jaworski, A. 1994. Pragmatic failure in second language: Greeting responses in English by Polish students. IRAL, 32, 4155.Google Scholar
Jeon, H. E. and Kaya, T. 2006. Effects of L2 instruction on interlanguage pragmatic development. In Norris, J. M. and Ortega, L. (eds.), Synthesizing research on language learning and teaching. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 165211.Google Scholar
Johnston, B., Kasper, G., and Ross, S. 1998. Effect of rejoinders in production questionnaires. Applied Linguistics, 19, 157182.Google Scholar
Kang, O. and Kermad, A. 2019. Prosody in L2 pragmatics research. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and pragmatics. New York: Routledge, 7892.Google Scholar
Kasper, G. 1997. Can pragmatic competence be taught?. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i, Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center. http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/NetWorks/NW06/Google Scholar
Kasper, G. 2001. Classroom research on interlanguage pragmatics. In Rose, K. R. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Pragmatics in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 3360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasper, G. 2006. Speech acts in interaction: Towards discursive pragmatics. In Bardovi-Harlig, K., Felix-Brasdefer, J. C., and Omar, A. (eds.), Pragmatics and language learning, Vol. 11. Honolulu, HI: National Foreign Language Resource Center, University of Hawaii at Manoa, 281314.Google Scholar
Kasper, G. and Dahl, M. 1991. Research methods in interlanguage pragmatics. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 13, 215247.Google Scholar
Kasper, G. and Rose, K. R. 1999. Pragmatics and SLA. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 19, 81104.Google Scholar
Kasper, G. and Rose, K. R. 2002. Pragmatic development in a second language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kasper, G. and Ross, S. 2013. Assessing second language pragmatics: An overview and introductions. In Ross, S. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Assessing second language pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave, 140.Google Scholar
Kasper, G. and Schmidt, R. 1996. Developmental issues in interlanguage pragmatics. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 149169.Google Scholar
Kecskes, I. 2003. Situation-bound utterances in L1 and L2. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kim, E. Y. A. and Brown, L. 2014. Negotiating pragmatic competence in computer mediated communication. CALICO Journal, 31, 264284.Google Scholar
Kim, J. 2014. How Korean EFL learners understand sarcasm in L2 English. Journal of Pragmatics, 60, 193206.Google Scholar
Kim, Y. and Taguchi, N. 2015. Promoting task-based pragmatics instruction in EFL classroom contexts: The role of task complexity. The Modern Language Journal, 99, 656677.Google Scholar
Kim, Y. and Taguchi, N. 2016. Learner-learner interaction during collaborative pragmatic tasks: The role of cognitive and pragmatic task demands. Foreign Language Annals, 49, 4257.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. and Belz, J. A. 2005. Socio-cultural perspective on pragmatic development in foreign language learning: Microgenetic case studies from telecollaboration and residence abroad. Intercultural Pragmatics, 2, 369421.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. and Blattner, G. 2008. Histories of engagement and sociolinguistic awareness in study abroad. In Ortega, L. and Byrnes, H. (eds.), The longitudinal study of advanced L2 capacities. New York: Routledge, 223246.Google Scholar
Kinginger, C. and Farrell, K. 2004. Assessing development of meta-pragmatic awareness in study abroad. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Study Abroad, 10, 1942.Google Scholar
Kley, K. 2019. What counts as evidence for interactional competence? Developing rating criteria for a German classroom-based paired speaking test. In Salaberry, M. R. and Kunitz, S. (eds.), Teaching and testing L2 interactional competence: Bridging theory and practice. New York: Routledge, 291321.Google Scholar
Koike, D. A. 1996. Transfer of pragmatic competence and suggestions in Spanish foreign language learning. In Gass, S. and Neu, J. (eds.), Speech acts across cultures. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 257281.Google Scholar
Köylü, Y. 2018. Comprehension of conversational implicatures in L2 English. Intercultural Pragmatics, 15, 373408.Google Scholar
Kuiken, F. and Vedder, I. 2017. Functional adequacy in L2 writing: Towards a new rating scale. Language Testing, 34, 321336.Google Scholar
Kuriscak, L. M. 2015. Examination of learner and situation level variables: Choice of speech act and request strategy by Spanish L2 learner. Hispania, 98, 300318.Google Scholar
Labben, A. 2016. Reconsidering the development of the discourse completion test in interlanguage pragmatics. Pragmatics, 26, 6991.Google Scholar
Lee, C. 2010. An exploratory study of the interlanguage pragmatic comprehension of young learners of English. Pragmatics, 20, 343373.Google Scholar
Lee, C. 2016. Understanding refusal style and pragmatic competence of teenage Cantonese English learners in refusals: An exploratory study. Intercultural Pragmatics, 13, 257282.Google Scholar
Levkina, M. 2018. Developing pragmatic competence through tasks in EFL contexts: Does proficiency play a role? In Taguchi, N. and Kim, Y. (eds.) Task-based approaches to teaching and assessing pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 137158.Google Scholar
Li, C., Li, W., and Ren, W. 2021. Tracking the trajectories of international students’ pragmatic choices in studying abroad in China: A social network perspective. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 34, 398416.Google Scholar
Li, D. 2000. The pragmatics of making requests in the L2 workplace: A case study of language socialization. Canadian Modern Language Review 57, 5887.Google Scholar
Li, E. S. H. 2010. Making suggestions: A contrastive study of young Hong Kong and Australian students. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 598616.Google Scholar
Li, S. 2014. The effects of different levels of linguistic proficiency on the development of L2 Chinese request production during study abroad. System, 45, 103116.Google Scholar
Liu, B. 2016. Effect of L2 exposure: From a perspective of discourse markers. Applied Linguistics Review, 7, 7398.Google Scholar
Liu, J. 2007. Developing a pragmatics test for Chinese EFL learners. Language Testing, 24, 391415.Google Scholar
Long, M. H. 1996. The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. C. and Bhatia, T. K. (eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 413468.Google Scholar
Lundell, F. F. and Erman, B. 2012. High-level requests: A study of long residency L2 users of English and French and native speakers. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 756775.Google Scholar
Lv, X., Ren, W., and Li, L. 2021. Pragmatic competence and willingness to communicate among L2 learners of Chinese. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 19.Google Scholar
Magliacane, A. and Howard, M. 2019. The role of learner status in the acquisition of pragmatic markers during study abroad: The use of “like” in L2 English. Journal of Pragmatics, 146, 7286.Google Scholar
Martínez-Flor, A. 2012. Examining EFL learners’ long-term instructional effects when mitigating requests. In Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. and Woodfield, H. (eds.), Interlanguage request modification. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 243274.Google Scholar
Al Masaeed, K. 2017. Interlanguage pragmatic development: Internal and external modification in L2 Arabic requests. Foreign Language Annals, 50, 808820.Google Scholar
Al Masaeed, K., Waugh, L. R., and Burns, K. E. 2018. The development of interlanguage pragmatics in L2 Arabic: The production of apology strategies. System, 74, 98108.Google Scholar
Matsumura, S. 2001. Learning the rules for offering advice: A quantitative approach to second language socialization. Language Learning, 51, 635679.Google Scholar
Matsumura, S. 2003. Modelling the relationships among interlanguage pragmatic development, L2 proficiency, and exposure to L2. Applied Linguistics, 24, 465491.Google Scholar
Mazzaggio, G., Panizza, D., and Surian, L. 2021. On the interpretation of scalar implicatures in first and second language. Journal of Pragmatics, 171, 6275.Google Scholar
Meiners, J. G. 2017. Cross-cultural and interlanguage perspectives on the emotional and pragmatic expression of sympathy in Spanish and English. In Parvaresh, V. and Capone, A. (eds.), The pragmeme of accommodation: The case of interaction around the event of death. Cham, Switzerland: Spinger Cham, 319348.Google Scholar
Miller, D., Giancaspro, D., Iverson, M., et al. 2016. Not just algunos, but indeed unos L2ers can acquire scalar implicatures in L2 Spanish. In Fuente, A. a. D. L., Valenzuela, E., and Sanz, C. (eds.), Language acquisition beyond parameters. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 125145.Google Scholar
Morkus, N. 2021. Negative pragmatic transfer and language proficiency: American learners of Arabic. The Language Learning Journal, 49, 4165.Google Scholar
Nakamura, C., Arai, M., Hirose, Y., and Flynn, S. 2020. An extra cue is beneficial for native speakers but can be disruptive for second language learners: Integration of prosody and visual context in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–14.Google Scholar
Narita, R. 2012. The effects of pragmatic consciousness-raising activity on the development of pragmatic awareness and use of hearsay evidential markers for learners of Japanese as a foreign language. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 129.Google Scholar
Newell, A. and Simon, H. 2019 [1972]. Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Echo Point Books and Media.Google Scholar
Nguyen, M. T. T., Pham, H. T., and Pham, T. M. 2017. The effects of input enhancement and recasts on the development of second language pragmatic competence. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching, 11, 4567.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. T. M. 2008. Criticizing in an L2: Pragmatic strategies used by Vietnamese EFL learners. Intercultural Pragmatics, 5, 4166.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. T. M. 2017. Using conversation tasks and retrospective methodology to investigate L2 pragmatics development: The case of EFL criticisms and responses to criticisms. The Language Learning Journal, 45, 399417.Google Scholar
Nguyen, T. T. M. and Pham, T. T. T. 2021. L2 emails of complaints: Strategy use by low and high proficiency learners of English as a foreign language. In Economidou-Kogetsidis, M., Savić, M., and Halenko, N. (eds.), Email pragmatics and second language learners. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 4170.Google Scholar
Nicholas, A. 2020. Dynamic assessment and requesting: Assessing the development of Japanese EFL learners’ oral requesting performance interactively. Intercultural Pragmatics, 17, 545575.Google Scholar
Niezgoda, K. and Roever, C. 2001. Pragmatic and grammatical awareness: A function of learning environment? In Rose, K. R. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Pragmatics in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 6379.Google Scholar
Olshtain, E. 1989. Apologies across languages. In Blum-Kulka, S., House, J., and Kasper, G. (eds.), Cross-cultural pragmatics: Requests and apologies. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 155173.Google Scholar
Pan, P. C. 2012. Interlanguage requests in institutional e-mail discourse. In Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. and Woodfield, H. (eds.), Interlanguage request modification. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 119161.Google Scholar
Pérez-Llantada, C. 2014. Formulaic language in L1 and L2 expert academic writing: Convergent and divergent usage. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 14, 8494.Google Scholar
Plonsky, L. and Zhuang, J. 2019. A meta-analysis of L2 pragmatics instruction. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and pragmatics. New York: Routledge, 287307.Google Scholar
Qi, X. and Lai, C. 2017. The effects of deductive instruction and inductive instruction on learners’ development of pragmatic competence in the teaching of Chinese as a second language. System, 70, 2637.Google Scholar
Qin, T. and Van Compernolle, R. A. 2021. Computerized dynamic assessment of implicature comprehension in L2 Chinese. Language Learning & Technology, 25, 5574.Google Scholar
Qin, W. and Uccelli, P. 2019. Metadiscourse: Variation across communicative contexts. Journal of Pragmatics, 139, 2239.Google Scholar
Reagan, D. and Payant, C. 2018. Task modality effects on Spanish learners’ interlanguage pragmatic development. In Taguchi, N. and Kim, Y. (eds.), Task-based approaches to teaching and assessing pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 113136.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2013. The effect of study abroad on the pragmatic development of the internal modification of refusals. Pragmatics, 23, 715741.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2014. A longitudinal investigation into L2 learners’ cognitive processes during study abroad. Applied Linguistics, 35, 575594.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2015. L2 pragmatic development in study abroad contexts. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2018a. Developing L2 pragmatic competence in study abroad contexts. In Sanz, C. and Morales-Front, A. (eds.), The Routledge handbook of study abroad research and practice. New York: Routledge, 119133.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2018b. Pragmatic strategies to solve and preempt understanding problems in Chinese professionals’ emails when using English as lingua franca communication. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21, 968981.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2019a. Emancipating (im)politeness research and increasing its impact. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 66, 289298.Google Scholar
Ren, W. 2019b. Pragmatic development of Chinese during study abroad: A cross-sectional study of learner requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 146, 137149.Google Scholar
Ren, W. and Guo, Y. 2020. Self-praise on Chinese social networking sites. Journal of Pragmatics, 169, 179189.Google Scholar
Ren, W. and Han, Z. 2016. The representation of pragmatic knowledge in recent ELT textbooks. ELT Journal, 70, 424434.Google Scholar
Ren, W. and Liu, W. 2021. Phatic communion in Chinese students’ gratitude emails in English: Production and perception. In Economidou-Kogetsidis, M., Savic, M., and Halenko, N. (eds.), Email pragmatics and second language learners. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 129150.Google Scholar
Ren, W. and Woodfield, H. 2016. Chinese females’ date refusals in reality TV shows: Expressing involvement or independence? Discourse, Context and Media, 13, 8997.Google Scholar
Reynolds-Case, A. 2013. The value of short-term study abroad: An increase in students’ cultural and pragmatic competency. Foreign Language Annals, 46, 311322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, M. A. 1992. Introspective methodology in interlanguage pragmatics research. In Kasper, G. (ed.) Pragmatics of Japanese as native and target language. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press, 2781.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. 2001. Task complexity, task difficulty, and task production: Exploring interactions in a componential framework. Applied Linguistics, 22, 2757.Google Scholar
Rodriguez, S. 2001. The perception of requests in Spanish by instructed learners of Spanish in the second- and foreign-language contexts: A longitudinal study of acquisition patterns. PhD Dissertation, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Roever, C. 2004. Difficulty and practicality in tests of interlanguage pragmatics. In Boxer, D. and Cohen, A. D. (eds.), Studying speaking to inform second language learning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, 283301.Google Scholar
Roever, C. 2005. Testing ESL pragmatics: Development and validation of a web-based assessment battery. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Roever, C. and Ikeda, N. 2022. What scores from monologic speaking tests can(not) tell us about interactional competence. Language Testing, 39, 729.Google Scholar
Romero-Trillo, J. 2019. Prosodic pragmatics and feedback in intercultural communication. Journal of Pragmatics, 151, 91102.Google Scholar
Rose, K. R. 1994. On validity of discourse completion tests in non-Western contexts. Applied Linguistics, 15, 114.Google Scholar
Rose, K. R. 2000. An exploratory cross-sectional study of interlanguage pragmatic development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 2767.Google Scholar
Rose, K. R. 2005. On the effects of instruction in second language pragmatics. System, 33, 385399.Google Scholar
Rose, K. R. 2009. Interlanguage pragmatic development in Hong Kong, phase 2. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 23452364.Google Scholar
Ross, S. and Kasper, G. 2013. Assessing second language pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sánchez-Hernández, A. and Alcón Soler, E. 2019. Pragmatic gains in the study abroad context: Learners’ experiences and recognition of pragmatic routines. Journal of Pragmatics, 146, 5471.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. 1998. Investigating EFL students’ production of speech acts: A comparison of production questionnaires and role plays. Journal of Pragmatics, 30, 457484.Google Scholar
Savic, M. 2014. Politeness through the prism of requests, apologies and refusals: A case of advanced Serbian EFL learners, Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambrridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Savić, M. and Đorđević, M. 2021. “You are the best!” relational practices in emails in English at a Norwegian university. In Economidou-Kogetsidis, M., Savić, M., and Halenko, N. (eds.), Email pragmatics and second language learners. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 227254.Google Scholar
Savić, M., Economidou-Kogetsidis, M. and Myrset, A. 2021. Young Greek Cypriot and Norwegian EFL learners: Pragmalinguistic development in request production. Journal of Pragmatics, 180, 1534.Google Scholar
Schauer, G. A. 2009. Interlanguage pragmatic development: The study abroad context. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Schauer, G. A. 2019. Teaching and learning English in the primary school: Interlanguage pragmatics in the EFL context. Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. 2007. Sequence organization in interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. 1983. Interaction, acculturation, and the acquisition of communicative competence: A case study of an adult. In Wolfson, N. and Judd, E. (eds.), Sociolinguistics and language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House Publishers, 137174.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. 1993. Consciousness, learning, and interlanguage pragmatics. In Kasper, G. and Blum-Kulka, S. (eds.), Interlanguage pragmatics. New York: Oxford University Press, 2142.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. 2001. Attention. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and second language instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 332.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. and Scollon, S. W. 2001. Intercultural communication: A discourse approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Seedhouse, P. 2013. Oral proficiency interviews as varieties of interaction. In Ross, S. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Assessing second language pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, 199219.Google Scholar
Shardakova, M. 2005. Intercultural pragmatics in the speech of American L2 learners of Russian: Apologies offered by Americans in Russian. Intercultural Pragmatics, 2, 423451.Google Scholar
Sharwood-Smith, M. 1993. Input enhancement in instructed SLA: Theoretical bases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 165179.Google Scholar
Shimizu, T. 2009. Influence of learning context on L2 pragmatic realization: A comparison between JSL and JFL learners’ compliment responses. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), Pragmatic competence. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 167198.Google Scholar
Shishavan, H. B. and Sharifian, F. 2013. Refusal strategies in L1 and L2: A study of Persian-speaking learners of English. Multilingua, 6, 801836.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. 2011. L2 pragmatic development in study abroad: A longitudinal study of Spanish service encounters. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 18181835.Google Scholar
Shively, R. L. 2015. Developing interactional competence during study abroad: Listener responses in L2 Spanish. System, 48, 8698.Google Scholar
Sidnell, J. 2010. Conversation analysis: An introduction. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. 2010. Scalar implicatures in second language acquisition. Lingua, 120, 24442462.Google Scholar
Snape, N. and Hosoi, H. 2018. Acquisition of scalar implicatures evidence from adult Japanese L2 learners of English. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8, 163192.Google Scholar
Sonnenburg-Winkler, S. L., Eslami, Z. R., and Derakhshan, A. 2020. Rater variation in pragmatic assessment: The impact of the linguistic background on peer-assessment and self-assessment. Lodz Papers in Pragmatics, 16, 6785.Google Scholar
Stavans, A. and Webman Shafran, R. 2018. The pragmatics of requests and refusals in multilingual settings. International Journal of Multilingualism, 15, 149168.Google Scholar
Su, I.-R. 2010. Transfer of pragmatic competences: A bi-directional perspective. The Modern Language Journal, 94, 87102.Google Scholar
Su, Y. and Ren, W. 2017. Developing L2 pragmatic competence in Mandarin Chinese: Sequential realization of requests. Foreign Language Annals, 50, 433457.Google Scholar
Swain, M. 1985. Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In Gass, S. and Madden, C. (eds.), Input in second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury House, 235253.Google Scholar
Swain, M. 2005. The output hypothesis: Theory and research. In Hinkel, E. (ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 471484.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2002. An application of relevance theory to the analysis of L2 interpretation processes: The comprehension of indirect replies. IRAL, 40, 151176.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2005. Comprehending implied meaning in English as a foreign language. The Modern Language Journal, 89, 543562.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2007a. Development of speed and accuracy in pragmatic comprehension in English as a foreign language. TESOL Quarterly, 41, 313338.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2007b. Task difficulty in oral speech act production. Applied Linguistics, 28, 113135.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2008a. Cognition, language contact, and the development of pragmatic comprehension in a study-abroad context. Language Learning, 58, 3371.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2008b. Pragmatic comprehension in Japanese as a foreign language. The Modern Language Journal, 92, 558576.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2008c. The role of learning environment in the development of pragmatic comprehension: A comparison of gains between EFL and ESL learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 423452.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2009a. Comprehension of indirect opinions and refusals in L2 Japanese. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), Pragmatic competence. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 249273.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2009b. Corpus-informed assessment of comprehension of conversational implicatures in L2 English. TESOL Quarterly, 43, 738749.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2011a. The effect of L2 proficiency and study-abroad experience on pragmatic comprehension. Language Learning, 61, 904939.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2011b. Rater variation in the assessment of speech acts. Pragmatics, 21, 453471.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2011c. Teaching pragmatics: Trends and issues. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 31, 289310.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2013a. Production of routines in L2 English: Effect of proficiency and study-abroad experience. System, 41, 109121.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2013b. Refusals in L2 English: Proficiency effects on appropriateness and fluency. In Martí-Arnándiz, O. and Salazar-Campillo, P. (eds.), Refusals in instructed contexts and beyond. Amsterdam: Rodopi, 101119.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2015a. Developing interactional competence in a Japanese study abroad context. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2015b. Instructed pragmatics at a glance: Where instructional studies were, are, and should be going. Language Teaching, 48, 150.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2017. Interlanguage pragmatics: A historical sketch and future directions. In Barron, A., Gu, Y., and Steen, G. (eds.), The Routledge handbook of pragmatics. London: Routledge, 153167.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. 2019. Second language acquisition and pragmatics: An overview. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and pragmatics. New York: Routledge, 114.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N., Fernández, L., and Jiang, Y. 2021. Systemic functional linguistics applied to analyze L2 speech acts: Analysis of advice-giving in a written text. New directions in second language pragmatics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 2757.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N., Gomez-Laich, M. P., and Arrufat-Marques, M.-J. 2016a. Comprehension of indirect meaning in Spanish as a foreign language. Foreign Language Annals, 49, 677698.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. and Li, S. 2019. Replication research in contextual and individual influences in pragmatic competence: Taguchi, Xiao & Li (2016) and Bardvo-Harlig & Bastos (2011). Language Teaching, 52, 128140.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N., Li, S., and Xiao, F. 2013. Production of formulaic expressions in L2 Chinese: A development investigation in a study abroad context. Chinese as a second language research, 2, 2358.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. and Roever, C. 2017. Second language pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N. and Sykes, J. M. 2013. Technology in interlanguage pragmatics research and teaching. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Taguchi, N., Xiao, F., and Li, S. 2016b. Effects of intercultural competence and social contact on speech act production in a Chinese study abroad context. The Modern Language Journal, 100, 775796.Google Scholar
Tajeddin, Z. and Alemi, M. 2014. Criteria and bias in native English teachers’ assessment of L2 pragmatic appropriacy: Content and facets analyses. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 23, 425434.Google Scholar
Tajeddin, Z. and Pezeshki, M. 2014. Acquisition of politeness markers in an EFL context: Impact of input enhancement and output tasks. RELC Journal, 45, 269286.Google Scholar
Takahashi, S. 1996. Pragmatic transferability. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 189223.Google Scholar
Takahashi, S. 2001. The role of input enhancedment in developing pragmatic competence. In Rose, K. R. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Pragmatics in language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 171199.Google Scholar
Takahashi, S. 2005. Pragmalinguistic awareness: Is it related to motivation and proficiency? Applied Linguistics, 26, 90120.Google Scholar
Takahashi, S. 2010. Assessing learnability in second language pragmatics. In Trosborg, A. (ed.), Pragmatics across languages and cultures. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 391421.Google Scholar
Takahashi, S. 2019. Individual learner considerations in SLA and L2 pragmatics. In Taguchi, N. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and pragmatics. New York: Routledge, 429443.Google Scholar
Takahashi, T. and Beebe, L. M. 1987. The development of pragmatic competence by Japanese learners of English. JALT Journal, 18, 131155.Google Scholar
Takimoto, M. 2008. The effects of deductive and inductive instruction on the development of language learners’ pragmatic competence. The Modern Language Journal, 92, 369386.Google Scholar
Takimoto, M. 2009. The effects of input-based tasks on the development of learners’ pragmatic proficiency. Applied Linguistics, 30, 125.Google Scholar
Takimoto, M. 2012. Metapragmatic discussion in interlanguage pragmatics. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 12401253.Google Scholar
Takimoto, M. 2020. Investigating the effects of cognitive linguistic approach in developing EFL learners’ pragmatic proficiency. System, 89, 114.Google Scholar
Taleghani-Nikazm, C. and Huth, T. 2010. L2 requests: Preference structure in talk-in-interaction. Multilingua, 29, 185202.Google Scholar
Tang, X. 2019. The effects of task modality on L2 Chinese learners’ pragmatic development: Computer-mediated written chat vs. face-to-face oral chat. System, 80, 4859.Google Scholar
Tang, X. and Taguchi, N. 2021. Digital game-based learning of formulaic expression in second language Chinese. The Modern Language Journal, 105, 740–459.Google Scholar
Tatsuki, D. H. 2000. If my complaints could passions move: An interlanguage study of aggression. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 10031017.Google Scholar
Taylor, G. 2002. Teaching gambits: The effect of instruction and task variation on the use of conversation strategies by intermediate Spanish students. Foreign Language Annals, 35, 171189.Google Scholar
Thomas, J. 1983. Cross-cultural pragmatic failure. Applied Linguistics, 4, 91112.Google Scholar
Timpe-Laughlin, V. and Cho, Y. 2021. Reflecting on assessing young foreign language learners. Language Testing, 38, 343355.Google Scholar
Timpe-Laughlin, V. and Choi, I. 2017. Exploring the validity of a second language intercultural pragmatics assessment tool. Language Assessment Quarterly, 14, 1935.Google Scholar
Timpe-Laughlin, V., Green, A. and Oh, S. 2021. Raising pragmatic awareness: A think-aloud study. System, 98, 115.Google Scholar
Van Compernolle, R. A. 2013. Interactional competence and the dynamic assessment of L2 pragmatic abilities. In Ross, S. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Assessing second language pragmatics. London: Palgrave MacMillan, 327353.Google Scholar
Vanpatten, B. 2003. From input to output: A teacher’s guide to second language acquisition. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Vassilaki, E. and Selimis, S. 2020. Children’s requestive behavior in L2 Greek: The core request. Journal of Pragmatics, 170, 271283.Google Scholar
Verdugo, D. R. and Romero-Trillo, J. 2005. The pragmatic function of intonation in L2 discourse: English tag questions used by Spanish speakers. Intercultural Pragmatics, 2, 151168.Google Scholar
Villarreal, D. 2014. Connecting production to judgments: T/V address forms and the L2 identities of intermediate Spanish learners. Journal of Pragmatics, 66, 114.Google Scholar
Walters, F. S. 2007. A conversation-analytic hermeneutic rating protocol to assess L2 oral pragmatic competence. Language Testing, 24, 155183.Google Scholar
Walters, F. S. 2009. A conversation analysis-informed test of L2 aural pragmatic comprehension. TESOL Quarterly, 43, 2954.Google Scholar
Walters, F. S. 2013. Interfaces between a discourse completion test and a conversation analysis-informed test of L2 pragmatic competence. In Ross, S. and Kasper, G. (eds.), Assessing second language pragmatics. Basingstoke: Palgrave MacMillan, 172195.Google Scholar
Wannaruk, A. 2008. Pragmatic transfer in Thai EFL refusals. RELC Journal, 39, 318337.Google Scholar
Warga, M. and Schölmberger, U. 2007. The acquisition of French apologetic behavior in a study abroad context. Intercultural Pragmatics, 4, 221251.Google Scholar
Wei, M. 2011. Investigating the oral proficiency of English learners in China: A comparative study of the use of pragmatic markers. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 34553472.Google Scholar
Wijayanto, A. 2016. Variability of refusal in L2: Evidence of L1 pragmalinguistic transfer and learner’s idiosyncratic usage. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 26, 99119.Google Scholar
Winans, M. D. 2020. Email requests: Politeness evaluations by instructors from diverse language backgrounds. Language Learning & Technology, 24, 104118.Google Scholar
Wong, W. 2005. Input enhancement: From theory and research to the classroom. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Woodfield, H. 2010. What lies beneath? Verbal report in interlanguage requests in English. Multilingua, 29, 127.Google Scholar
Woodfield, H. 2012. Pragmatic variation in learner perception: The role of retrospective verbal report in L2 speech act research. In Felix-Brasdefer, C. and Koike, D. A. (eds.), Pragmatic variation in first and second language contexts: Methodological issues. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 209223.Google Scholar
Xu, W., Case, R. E., and Wang, Y. 2009. Pragmatic and grammatical competence, length of residence, and overall L2 proficiency. System, 37, 205216.Google Scholar
Yang, H. and Ren, W. 2019. Pragmatic awareness and second language learning motivation: A mixed-methods investigation. Pragmatics & Cognition, 26, 447473.Google Scholar
Yang, L. 2016. Learning to express gratitude in Mandarin Chinese through web-based instruction. Language Learning & Technology, 20, 191208.Google Scholar
Yang, L. 2019. Development of pragmatic and grammatical awareness in L2 Chinese classrooms. In Yuan, F. and Li, S. (eds.), Classroom research on Chinese as a second language. New York: Routledge, 190210.Google Scholar
Yeh, E. and Swinehart, N. 2020. Social media literacy in L2 environments: Navigating anonymous user-generated content. Computer Assisted Language Learning.Google Scholar
Ying, J. and Ren, W. 2021. Advanced learners’ responses to Chinese greetings in study abroad. IRAL.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. 2014. Measuring syntactic complexity in L2 pragmatic production: Investigating relationships among pragmatics, grammar, and proficiency. System, 42, 270287.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. 2015. Validity argument for assessing L2 pragmatics in interaction using mixed methods. Language Testing, 32, 199225.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. 2018a. Task design and validity evidence for assessment of L2 pragmatics in interaction. In Taguchi, N. and Kim, Y. (eds.), Task-based approaches to teaching and assessing pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 217246.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. 2018b. Task-based needs analysis of L2 pragmatics in an EAP context. Journal of English for Academic Purposes, 36, 8698.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. 2020a. Interactional features of L2 pragmatic interaction in role-play speaking assessment. TESOL Quarterly, 54, 201233.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. 2020b. Managing proposal sequences in role-play assessment: Validity evidence of interactional competence across levels. Language Testing, 37, 76106.Google Scholar
Youn, S. J. and Bi, N. Z. 2019. Investigating test-takers’ strategy use in task-based L2 pragmatic speaking assessment. Intercultural Pragmatics, 16, 185218.Google Scholar
Yousefi, M. and Nassaji, H. 2019. A meta-analysis of the effects of instruction and corrective feedback on L2 pragmatics and the role of moderator variables face-to-face vs. computer-mediated instruction. ITL – International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 170, 278309.Google Scholar
Yuan, Z. and Zhang, R. 2018. Investigating longitudinal pragmatic development of complaints made by Chinese EFL learners. Applied Linguistics Review, 9, 6387.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Branches of L2 pragmatics

Figure 1

Table 1 Frequency and percentage of self-praise strategies

Figure 2

Table 2 Frequency and percentage of advice strategies and supportive moves

Figure 3

Figure 2 Layers of L2 pragmatics research

Figure 4

Figure 3 Shift of focus for L2 pragmatics research

Accessibility standard: Unknown

Accessibility compliance for the HTML of this Element is currently unknown and may be updated in the future.

Save element to Kindle

To save this element to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Second Language Pragmatics
  • Wei Ren, Beihang University, China
  • Online ISBN: 9781009082709
Available formats
×

Save element to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Second Language Pragmatics
  • Wei Ren, Beihang University, China
  • Online ISBN: 9781009082709
Available formats
×

Save element to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Second Language Pragmatics
  • Wei Ren, Beihang University, China
  • Online ISBN: 9781009082709
Available formats
×