Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-42vt5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-12T05:35:06.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Archaeology of Forgetting, the Dorset, and Arctic Antiquity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 August 2025

Donald H. Holly Jr*
Affiliation:
Department of Sociology, Anthropology, and Criminology, Eastern Illinois University, 600 Lincoln Avenue, Charleston, IL 61920-3011, USA
T. Max Friesen
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Toronto, 19 Ursula Franklin St, Toronto, ON, M5S 2S2, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Donald H. Holly, Jr; Email: dhholly@eiu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Forgetting, an attendant to culture change, is the stuff of history. When cultural innovations, exchange and adoption occur, previous customs, knowledge, technology and other dimensions of culture are often lost—they are forgotten. This paper considers the phenomenon of forgetting and its permutations—the passive forgetting that is more or less an accepted outcome of change, the unintentional forgetting that is accidental and undesired, and the intentional forgetting of wilful erasure—as a way of contemplating agency and culture loss/change among the Dorset Paleo-Inuit peoples of the central and eastern North American Arctic, and more broadly, in Arctic archaeology.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research

Forgetting

Forgetting offers something of an interpretative conundrum to archaeologists. It would seem at once to be the common stuff of history—that when culture changes, dimensions of it are lost and forgotten. Elizabeth Marshall Thomas (Reference Thomas2006, 250–51) makes the observation in her book The Old Way that the ice-age hunters of Europe must have had an intimate understanding of mammoths. They would been familiar with their sounds, smells, temperament and behaviour, and how these things varied with the age and sex of the animal, composition of the herd, and season. And, they would have taught and shown their children and grandchildren these things—yet all this knowledge is effectively lost now. Far closer to the present, readers of a certain age might remember what it was like to be unreachable, and bored, and present, in a way that young and future generations of smart-phone users may never completely understand.

At the same time, memory can seem eternal. The past—in the form of artifacts, ruins, ritual traces and landscapes of familiarity and habit—constitutes a vast mnemonic reservoir from which societies can remember or re-remember (Bender Reference Bender2002; Borić Reference Borić and Borić2010a,b; Gosden & Lock Reference Gosden and Lock1998; Hendon Reference Hendon2010; Kahn Reference Kahn1990; Van Dyke Reference Van Dyke2019). The debris of the past can also inspire the constitution of new memories or histories (Iverson Reference Iverson2017; Van Dyke Reference Van Dyke2019; Van Dyke & Alcock Reference Van Dyke and Alcock2003); when this happens, we could say that the past has not been so much ‘lost’ as only misunderstood by those rewriting or reimagining it. And even when the past is truly obliterated—in the form of the purposeful discarding or destruction of things—the memory of the act can preserve the memory of the thing itself (Küchler Reference Küchler1997; Reference Küchler, Forty and Küchler1999). Some go so far as to suggest that forgetting is only a modern, western phenomenon (Connerton Reference Connerton2009; Nora Reference Nora1989).

Culture is often broadly conceptualized as enduring, accumulative and progressive. If anything is truly forgotten it is because it was beyond our control (mammoths go extinct) or something better (mobile phones) took its place. Powell (Reference Powell1888, 99) asserted long ago that cultures generally learn and improve their lot and that ‘retrogression in culture proper is rarely, perhaps never, exhibited on any large scale’. Wissler (Reference Wissler1923, 40) likewise alleged that culture grows by accumulation and that ‘little of importance is ever lost’. Tylor (Reference Tylor1865, 235–6) employed a similar logic to buttress his scepticism of claims that the Tasmanians did not know how to make fire. His position was that once something so important and critical as fire-making was learned, humans would never forget it. Taylor (Reference Taylor2008) has recently revisited the apparent absence of fire-making among the Tasmanians and concludes that while it is not absolutely certain they made fire, it is ‘far more logical and probable’ that they did (see also Gott Reference Gott2002). That the Tasmanians, more likely than not, knew how to make fire intuitively makes sense to many archaeologists; we are apt to be highly sceptical of claims for the loss of useful technologies and knowledge such as fire-making for the good reason that the ability to make fire would seem essential.

On fire-making, however, various claims have been made to the absence of the art among the Siriono (Holmberg [1950] Reference Holmberg1969, 17), Yuquí (Stearman Reference Stearman1984, 643) and Guajá (Cormier Reference Cormier2003, 53) of the Amazon (Balée Reference Balée2013, 106, 133), the Andaman Islanders (Radcliffe-Brown [Reference Radcliffe–Brown1922] 1964, 472) and Mbuti (Duffy Reference Duffy1984, viii), with some reasoning that even Neanderthals living in Ice Age Europe could have managed without it (Dibble et al. Reference Dibble, Abodolahzadeh, Aldeins, Goldberg, McPherron and Sandgathe2017; Macdonald Reference Macdonald2018). Our point is not to question fire-making by Tasmanians, Neanderthals, or anyone else, but rather to point out that claims for loss of ‘useful arts’ are apt to be met with significant scepticism, or even dismissed outright, by drawing on an underlying premise of progress or functional primacy/adaptation—the former inherited from the nineteenth-century cultural evolutionists and the latter from the processualists. Furthermore, the sense that fire-making and other useful technologies are achievements can make claims to the loss or absence of such technologies/knowledge seem like insults. Interpreting forgetting in the archaeological record is a fraught exercise.

Here we consider forgetting in several forms—passive, unintentional and intentional—as a way of thinking about the disappearance of technologies, practices and knowledge-systems in the archaeological past. We focus our attention on Arctic antiquity, and especially the archaeological record of the Dorset Paleo-Inuit culture of the Eastern Arctic of North America. The Dorset are famous for many things, including figurative art, exquisite small tools, snow-knives and longhouses, but also for what disappeared when they emerged on the archaeological scene—dogs, bows and arrows and drills. We consider these losses through the interpretive lens of various forms of forgetting, with attention to how human agency is in play in each instance. By doing so, we hope to clear a humanistic space for understanding forgetting in the Dorset context, and more broadly, in Arctic antiquity.

Passive forgetting

Perhaps the most common form of forgetting is the kind that happens without much care. It is forgetting how to surprise a mammoth, or use an atlatl, or read a map, or write cursive. In archaeological theory, such forgettings are often perceived to be the result of progress or adaptation—the offloading of ballast to make room for something better, or more useful, or relevant. We use the term ‘perceived’ here, because a rear-view-mirror value assessment seems inherent to making sense of what was lost. If it was merely the knowledge of how to use a dial telephone, we are apt to attribute it to the forgetting of the passive and inconsequential sort; but if it is how to make fire, it must be something else. Accordingly, interpreting forgetting in the archaeological record is difficult without a value assessment, and often that value assessment is reasoned in terms of progress or adaptation.

Arguments for progress are typically appeals to improvement devoid of context. Agriculture, for instance, was long believed to be so superior to hunting and gathering that no real interpretation was necessary to explain its development, people started farming because it was simply better (Cohen Reference Cohen, Ember, Ember and Peregrine2007). An adaptationist perspective on agriculture, by contrast, is apt to situate the transition in terms of population pressure, environmental change, or social—perhaps fitness-enhancing—aspirations. Similar thinking can also account for the loss of ‘superior’ technology, like pottery (Kirch Reference Kirch2000, 220–22) or microliths (Costa et al. Reference Costa, Sternke and Woodman2005; Potter Reference Potter2008), or ‘advanced’ knowledge, such as plant terminology (Witkowski & Brown Reference Witkowski and Brown1978; Witkowski & Burris Reference Witkowski and Burris1981). It can also explain their re-invention and re-discovery when conditions change.

Archaeologists who rely on an underlying assumption of progress can be befuddled by evidence that deviates from an expected improvement trajectory. A century ago, W.H.R. Rivers (Reference Rivers and Smith1926) noted how his (evolutionary) contemporaries struggled to understand how people could have settled Tasmania and other islands in the unseaworthy watercraft they possessed in historic times, with some going so far as to reason that that these islands must have been reached on foot, or alternatively, had been always occupied by people. Their underlying logic here parallels Tylor’s (Reference Tylor1865, 235) on fire-making—the art of superior watercraft, once learned, would never be forgotten. Even today the loss of sophisticated seafaring technology and complexity in the region offers something of a puzzle to some archaeologists. O’Connell, Allen and Hawkes (Reference O’Connell, Allen, Hawkes, Anderson, Barrett and Boyle2010) respond to them by noting how well suited the simpler watercraft that Australian First Nations employed at contact were for the broad bays and estuaries in which they were used. Thus, the shift from sturdy vessels used for open-water navigation and deep-sea fishing in the late Pleistocene to the simpler rafts and canoes of the historic period is best understood as an adaptative pivot that required less energy and lower investment costs (O’Connell et al. Reference O’Connell, Allen, Hawkes, Anderson, Barrett and Boyle2010).

Similar explanations have been proffered to explain why the Tasmanians apparently stopped eating fish around 3500 bp. Rhys Jones (Reference Jones and Gould1978) interpreted the dietary shift as part and parcel of a broader de-evolution and regression of Tasmanian culture. Critics quickly countered that the shift in diet was likely adaptive and related to processes such as modifications to settlement patterns, economic re-organization, or environmental changes (Allen Reference Allen1979; Bowdler Reference Bowdler, Bailey and Parkington1988; Horton Reference Horton1979; White & O’Connell Reference White and O’Connell1982, 170). Others question the source material—maybe the Tasmanians never stopped eating fish (Taylor Reference Taylor2007). The assumption of the adaptationist position is laid bare by Bowdler (Reference Bowdler1980, 339), who, in grappling with the Tasmanian aversion to fish, presumes that if ‘culture is a form of adaptation’ and fish-eating was prohibited, then ‘a fundamentally economic reason’ (for doing so) must have motivated it. Such scepticism (fish-eating) and denial (fire-making) is apt to occur when the evidence is not easily explained in terms of adaptation. It seems especially common in hunter-gatherer studies, where cultural ecology and related theoretical positions dominate; to wit, we struggle to make sense of why the Tasmanians may have ceased eating fish in a way that seems less theoretically taxing when accounting for why Hindus may have stopped consuming cows, or Muslims and Jews, pigs (cf. Holly Reference Holly2019).

Unintentional forgetting

Unintentional forgetting can be said to be forgetting of the undesired and accidental sort. It is apt to occur when knowledges (and associated technologies, practices and rituals) that are highly specialized or secretive are lost when the small subgroups of people that know them pass on, and what they knew is not easily re-discovered or re-created. Rivers (Reference Rivers and Smith1926), for instance, cites the disappearance of certain kinds of canoes in Melanesia and well-made adzes in Indonesia when the small group of craftspeople that made them died. Afterwards, people just managed without them.

Demography has been controversially invoked as a variable in explanations for both cultural loss and innovation. On loss, Rhys Jones (Reference Jones and Wright1977; Reference Jones and Gould1978) infamously suggested that the ‘disappearance’ of Tasmanian cultural traits—the aforementioned cessation of fish-eating, but also fire-making, bone-tool production and the ‘deterioration’ and simplification of technology—was an effect of the island’s long isolation from Australia following sea-level rise at the end of the Pleistocene. Jones thought that isolation not only stifled innovation on Tasmania, but that it caused cultural regression and ‘deterioration’—a slow cultural dumbing-down of the maladaptive sort. His thesis was immediately challenged by reinterpretations that framed these losses and simplifications as adaptations (Allen Reference Allen1979; Bowdler Reference Bowdler, Bailey and Parkington1988; Horton Reference Horton1979; Vanderwal Reference Vanderwal1978; White & O’Connell Reference White and O’Connell1982, 170).

More recently, Henrich (Reference Henrich2004) has resurrected aspects of Jones’ thesis by suggesting that low population numbers and isolation could indeed have worked to create a smaller and less-active pool of social learners and innovators, and led to cultural loss, simplification, stagnation and even maladaptation (see also Diamond Reference Diamond1997, 312-19; Flannery Reference Flannery1994, 264–70; cf. Vaesen et al. Reference Vaesen, Collard, Cosgrove and Roebroeks2016). Similar demographic arguments have been made for the Yamana of Tierra del Fuego. Garvey (Reference Garvey and Lemke2018), for instance, posits that peoples moving southward through the Americas would have lost the art of how to make well-tailored cold-weather clothing as they passed through the tropics, and that on the other side, the small population size of the Yamana, together with their apparent isolation, worked against them reinventing it. As a result, they ended up poorly-equipped for the Subantarctic climate and paid for it with poor health and fertility (Garvey Reference Garvey and Lemke2018). Inversely, some archaeologists have suggested that high population densities and frequent interactions stimulate innovations and could have generated a cultural florescence in the Upper Palaeolithic (Bar-Yosef Reference Bar-Yosef2002; Gilman Reference Gilman and Spriggs1984; Powell et al. Reference Powell, Shennan and Thomas2009).

Even small groups of foragers, however, interact with many hundreds of people in the course of their lifetime (Hill et al. Reference Hill, Wood, Baggio, Hurtado and Boyd2014). Mobile hunter-gatherers can be rather cosmopolitan, covering vast areas and interacting with far-flung neighbours in a way that more numerous but rooted farmers do not (see Holly Reference Holly2013, 122–3). There is also little evidence to suggest that hunter-gatherer technological complexity, skills and knowledge are narrowly dependent on population size—witness the intricate sealing harpoon and other tool complexes of the Inuit (Oswalt Reference Oswalt1987; Read Reference Read2008). Indeed, rather than a liability, the small size of hunter-gatherer groups may allow them to pivot to new technologies and practices faster than larger (Bettinger et al. Reference Bettinger, Barton and Morgan2010, 12) and less egalitarian groups, whose size and social structures can act as a force of inertia (see Hegmon et al. Reference Hegmon, Peeples, Kinzig, Kulow, Meegan and Nelson2008). Accordingly, we are suspicious of broad characterizations of hunter-gatherers as isolated, innovation-resistant and at risk of cultural regression due to low population densities. That said, it is not difficult to envision how cultural loss could occur in very small populations (Riede et al. Reference Riede, Edinborough, Thomas, Crombé, Van Strydonck, Sergant, Boudin and Bats2009), especially if the knowledge it was based on was highly specialized, secretive and/or limited to a subgroup of individuals. In theory, such cultural drift scenarios could also occur in very large but specialized populations—the death of Aztec priests with the fall of Tenochtitlán (Arbagi Reference Arbagi2011, 114) or the hypothetical plane load of astrovolcanologists that crashes on the way to a conference.

Unintentional forgetting may be indicated when the loss is sudden and great—as when a bundle of traits and technologies vanish simultaneously. In the Amazon, the absence of fire-making and musical instruments, relatively simple cosmology and rituals, and the abandonment of agriculture have been attributed to population collapse from disease, violence and displacement (Balée Reference Balée2013; Cormier Reference Cormier2003; Isaac Reference Isaac1977; Stearman Reference Stearman1984). Something similar may have occurred among the Inughuit (Polar Inuit), an isolated Inuit group in northwestern Greenland. In the early 1800s, the Inughuit had lost several important categories of technology, including kayaks, the bow and arrow and some kinds of fishing gear. This has been attributed by some to population loss/knowledge loss caused by disease (unintentional forgetting); others connect their disappearance to the declining availability of wood (passive forgetting) during the Little Ice Age (see LeMoine & Darwent Reference LeMoine, Darwent, Friesen and Mason2016). The case for disease among the Inughuit, however, is supported by the fact that epidemics, famines, natural catastrophes and violence are known to have greatly reduced other small Arctic communities in a short time (Burch Reference Burch1998, 320–23; Corbett Reference Corbett, Corbett, West and Lefèvre2010, 41–2; Darwent Reference Darwent, Mondini, Muñoz and Wickler2004; McGhee Reference McGhee1994), and by the fact that declining wood availability alone would not have prevented the Polar Inughuit from crafting bows and arrows out of different raw materials (LeMoine & Darwent Reference LeMoine, Darwent, Friesen and Mason2016; Walls et al. Reference Walls, Knudsen and Larsen2015).

Unintentional loss need not involve the forgetting of essential technologies and skills. It could be forgetting how to perform certain rituals, recall genealogies, or read an indigenous script. These things might be critical to the culture, even if they do not jeopardize survival in the way that forgetting how to make fire would. How might we interpret their disappearance? In Newfoundland and Labrador, an evident demographic collapse around 3500 bp resulted in the abandonment of a vast territory (including the entire island of Newfoundland) and a hard pivot that fundamentally transformed technological traditions, foodways, social organization, settlement patterns and exchange relations, with associated losses in technologies and practices (Holly et al. Reference Holly, Wolff, Hull, Holyoke and Hrynick2022). In the rear-view mirror, we may deem these radical realignments to be of the adaptive, resilient and passive sort, although it is doubtful that the people who lived through them would have thought about it this way.

Intentional forgetting

People can also actively work to forget what they or others know. One way to achieve this is through the deliberate targeting and killing of knowledge-keepers, as in the aforementioned slaughter of Aztec priests by the Spanish (Arbagi Reference Arbagi2011, 114), or the Khmer Rouge’s mass killing of Cambodia’s intelligentsia (Clayton Reference Clayton1998). In such cases, murder—or genocide—becomes a weapon of intentional forgetting. Languages, likewise, have been killed by the wilful erasure efforts of governments, missionaries and social engineering programmes (see Hinton Reference Hinton1996); they have also died the slower death of the wilful absence of practice at home.

Knowledge and memories of habit can be lost with religious conversion too. The Ayoreo of northern Paraguay wilfully ceased traditional chanting and self-consciously abandoned many other practices in the aim of assimilation and conversion (Bessire Reference Bessire2014). Indonesians stopped rearing pigs and consuming pork when they converted to Islam (Lape Reference Lape2005). Religiously motivated forgetting can take more forceful forms (Connerton Reference Connerton1989, 14-15; González-Ruibal Reference González–Ruibal, Starzmann and Roby2016; Schwartz Reference Schwartz2013), as when the Spanish destroyed religious paraphernalia, burned books and erected churches on the ruins of Indigenous sacred spaces (Arbagi Reference Arbagi2011) or when Pueblo peoples returned the favour following the revolt of 1680 (Liebmann Reference Liebmann2008). Protestants removed, destroyed and white-washed Catholic imagery and assaulted the pagan stones of Avebury during the Reformation (Bender Reference Bender and Bender1993). The Taliban obliterated the great Buddha statues of Afghanistan (Meskell Reference Meskell2002). Savonarola’s devotees set fire to Florence’s vanities (Strathern Reference Strathern2017, 224–5). Religious conversion, accordingly, can be a powerful catalyst for forgetting of the wilful sort. Political ‘conversion’ can be, too.

Intentional forgetting is difficult to infer from the archaeological record, but one circumstance where it has been interpreted convincingly is in the sphere of politics. Political successors or revolutionaries appear to have mutilated, defaced and buried the colossal stone heads of the Olmec which depicted previous leaders (Pool & Loughlin Reference Pool and Loughlin2017). The Maya at La Sierra, Honduras, similarly vandalized and buried symbols of prior rulership (Schortman & Urban Reference Schortman and Urban2011). In a hunter-gatherer context, a related phenomenon is seen with the expansion of Numic peoples in the American Great Basin, which saw them intentionally deface or obscure the rock art of their predecessors (Bettinger & Baumhoff Reference Bettinger and Baumhoff1982; Quinlan & Woody Reference Quinlan and Woody2003).

The Dorset

One particularly noteworthy instance of ‘forgetting’ in the archaeological record occurs with the emergence of the Dorset culture in the eastern North American Arctic (consisting of the Canadian Arctic and Greenland). This region’s culture history consists of two quite different traditions (Table 1). The more recent is known as Inuit (or Neo-Inuit/Neo-Eskimo), and spans from c. 1250 ce to the present. The earlier tradition, Paleo-Inuit (also known as the Paleo-Eskimo or Arctic Small Tool tradition), extends from c. 3000 bce to 1350 ce (Friesen Reference Friesen, Mason and Friesen2017). Peoples of these two traditions are only distantly related, with the earliest Inuit, known as Thule, displacing the final Paleo-Inuit during the thirteenth and fourteenth centuries ce.

Table 1. General chronology for Eastern Arctic archaeology.

Our emphasis in this paper is on technologies and other archaeologically visible phenomena that were lost, or ‘forgotten’, midway through the Paleo-Inuit period during the ‘Pre-Dorset–Dorset transition’. The earliest Paleo-Inuit are known in most of the Canadian Arctic as Pre-Dorset, in the High Arctic as Independence I and in most of Greenland as Saqqaq. Pre-Dorset arrived from Siberia by way of Alaska, and throughout most of the region are characterized by relatively small sites which were occupied only briefly, as indicated by sparse artifacts and faunal remains. However, based on pan-regional syntheses, as well as comparison with a particularly rich Saqqaq assemblage from West Greenland (Grønnow Reference Grønnow2017), it is clear that they carried with them an elaborate technology allowing the production of warm clothing, small watercraft and a range of bone, antler and driftwood implements. Their hunting technologies were particularly elaborate and included the bow and arrow for terrestrial mammal hunting and harpoons and lances for hunting seals. Pre-Dorset demography shifted substantially over time, with regional surveys indicating periods of population peaks (as measured by numbers of dwellings) alternating with periods marked by population crashes or even abandonment of some regions (Dyke & Savelle Reference Dyke and Savelle2009).

During the first millennium bce, Pre-Dorset society developed into Dorset. While there is some uncertainty about the nature of this transition, Dorset is highly recognizable in the archaeological record due to such newly introduced elements as semi-subterranean houses, new harpoon head forms and new stone tool types. The poorly understood transitional/Early Dorset period begins between 800 and 500 bce (Friesen Reference Friesen, Friesen and Mason2016; Houmard Reference Houmard2018; Maxwell Reference Maxwell, Gilberg and Gulløv1997; Schledermann Reference Schledermann1990, 325–7; cf. Ramsden and Tuck Reference Ramsden and Tuck2001; Ryan Reference Ryan, Friesen and Mason2016). By around 100 bce, a widespread and distinctive horizon known as Middle Dorset had spread across most of the southern Canadian Arctic and deep into the subarctic regions of southern Labrador and Newfoundland. Finally, around 500 ce Late Dorset peoples, with distinctive new tool types and large ‘longhouse’ aggregations, reoccupied large areas including the High Arctic and northwest Greenland (Friesen Reference Friesen, Mason and Friesen2017) (Fig. 1).

Figure 1. Map of the North American Arctic, showing maximum extent of Dorset settlement. This map includes regions occupied by Middle Dorset and Late Dorset; other related cultural phases are not included because their relationship with Dorset is less clear. (Map drafted by Susannah Clinker and Max Friesen.)

The end of the Dorset period is poorly understood, but it appears to have varied across different sub-regions. It is becoming increasingly clear, for instance, that it persisted in some places into the thirteenth or fourteenth century ce (Appelt & Gulløv Reference Appelt, Gulløv, Maschner, Mason and McGhee2009; Friesen Reference Friesen2020a; Labrèche Reference Labrèche2015; Rowley Reference Rowley, Morrison and Pilon1994). Much debate focuses on the dating of the Dorset’s demise, and whether they were still around when the Thule (direct ancestors of modern Inuit) first set foot in the Eastern Arctic, or if they had already disappeared by then (Appelt & Gulløv Reference Appelt, Gulløv, Maschner, Mason and McGhee2009; Appelt et al. Reference Appelt, Damkjar, Friesen, Friesen and Mason2016; Friesen Reference Friesen2020a; McGhee Reference McGhee, Gilberg and Gulløv1997; Park 1993; Reference Park, Friesen and Mason2016; Pinard & Gendron Reference Pinard, Gendron, Maschner, Mason and McGhee2009). Thule Inuit migrated east from Alaska in the thirteenth century ce, and recent studies indicate that Dorset and Inuit are not closely related (Raghavan et al. Reference Raghavan, Degiorgio and Albrechtsen2014). Modern Inuit tell stories of a race of giants, or alternatively dwarfs—the Tuniit—that occupied the country when they arrived (Bennett & Rowley Reference Bennett and Rowley2004). These accounts likely reference the Dorset directly, though they may have also been inspired in some regions by encounters with earlier Dorset or Thule ruins or tools (Mathiassen Reference Mathiassen1927).

The Dorset are famous in Arctic archaeology for vanishing. They are also famous for what they gained, and lost, in the transition from the preceding Pre-Dorset period. In terms of gains, among many categories of new technologies, the Dorset saw the introduction or greatly increased frequency of semi-subterranean dwellings, snow knives (used to construct snow houses), ice creepers (attached to kamiks (boots) to increase grip on ice), ground slate tools, figurative art and implements associated with shamanism. However, it is the losses, or ‘forgettings’, that concern us here. There is no good evidence that Dorset had dogs; at best they were extremely rare (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Darwent and Sacks2013; Morey & Aaris-Sørensen Reference Morey and Aaris–Sørensen2002). Dorset sleds were likely pulled by humans (Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985). Their Pre-Dorset predecessors did have dogs, though it should be noted that evidence for dogs is variable and relatively low throughout the Paleo-Inuit period (Darwent Reference Darwent, Mondini, Muñoz and Wickler2004, 65; Meldgaard Reference Meldgaard2004; Morey & Aaris-Sørensen Reference Morey and Aaris–Sørensen2002). The Dorset also did not have the bow and arrow, but their predecessors did (Desrosiers & Sørensen Reference Desrosiers, Sørensen, Friesen and Mason2016, 166–7; Grønnow Reference Grønnow2017, 47, 52–8; Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985, 88, 138; Meldgaard Reference Meldgaard and Campbell1962). Curiously, the Dorset also lacked drills, and so they instead arduously gouged holes into bone and antler (Fig. 2). The Pre-Dorset, however, had them (Grønnow Reference Grønnow2017, 157–8; Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985, 86). Diamond Jenness (Reference Jenness1925, 435), who first described the Dorset culture, reasoned that it was old in part by the logic that gouging had to have pre-dated drilling, echoing the common progressivist refrain that ‘no tribe that had once known the bow drill would have forgotten its use’ (see also Rowley Reference Rowley1940, 496). Despite these losses, it is important to note that Dorset society can be seen as relatively successful in comparison with its Pre-Dorset predecessor, as indicated by periodic expansions to new areas during both the Middle and Late Dorset periods and the occupation of large and relatively permanent settlements.

Figure 2. Three Dorset artifacts from Victoria Island, Nunavut, showing the Dorset method of gouging linear holes rather than drilling round ones. Left to right: harpoon foreshaft (Middle Dorset); needle (Middle Dorset); plaque/box part with incised decoration (Late Dorset). (Photograph: Max Friesen.)

Dorset forgetting

Cultural hiatuses and territorial contractions are common in Arctic antiquity and likely mark periods when food resources were poor and environmental conditions marginal (see Darwent Reference Darwent, Mondini, Muñoz and Wickler2004; Desjardins & Jordan Reference Desjardins and Jordan2019; Schledermann Reference Schledermann1990). Not a few of these appear to dovetail with population decline (Holly Reference Holly, Sassaman and Holly2011; Holly et al. Reference Holly, Wolff, Hull, Holyoke and Hrynick2022; Jorgensen & Riede Reference Jørgensen and Riede2019; Maschner Reference Maschner, Friesen and Mason2016; Savelle & Dyke Reference Savelle and Dyke2009; Tallavaara & Pesonen, Reference Tallavaara and Pesonen2020). There is, for instance, evidence of a significant Pre-Dorset population crash around 2000 bce in the central Canadian Arctic (Dyke & Savelle Reference Dyke and Savelle2009; Savelle & Dyke 2002; Reference Savelle and Dyke2009), at about the same time that large swathes of the High Arctic were abandoned (Grønnow Reference Grønnow, Friesen and Mason2016; Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985, 110; McGhee Reference McGhee1996, 111; Schledermann Reference Schledermann1990, 314). Another population crash and abandonment event occurred around 600 bce in many areas of the Eastern Arctic (Dyke & Savelle Reference Dyke and Savelle2009; Schledermann Reference Schledermann1990, 314), at about the time of the Pre-Dorset/Dorset transition. Archaeologists have frequently noted that this period is associated with a cooling climate, with colder temperatures seen as driving change in sea ice conditions and terrestrial ecosystems with direct implications for human societies (Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985; McGhee Reference McGhee1996). Recent Palaeoclimatic research continues to support generally colder conditions during the first millennium bce, but they also indicate significant regional variability in the timing and severity of climate change (e.g. Briner et al. Reference Briner, McKay and Axford2016; Finkelstein Reference Finkelstein, Friesen and Mason2016).

Assessing continuity between Pre-Dorset and Dorset is greatly complicated by matters of geographic scale, fieldwork and survey coverage, and temporal resolution—to say nothing of a dynamic resource environment which could also account for changes in the material record. Cultural discontinuities across the Pre-Dorset/Dorset transition have been identified in parts of Arctic Canada, Greenland, Labrador and Newfoundland, but not in others, and there are keen disagreements on dating and interpretation, as well as on the identity and origins of the direct predecessors of the ‘Dorset’ (Friesen Reference Friesen, Mason and Friesen2017; Grønnow Reference Grønnow2017, 390–91; Houmard Reference Houmard2018; Maxwell Reference Maxwell, Gilberg and Gulløv1997; Meldgaard Reference Meldgaard and Campbell1962; Milne et al. Reference Milne, Park, Stenton and Preston2013; Nagy Reference Nagy, Morrison and Pilon1994; Odess Reference Odess, Fitzhugh, Loring and Odess2002; Ramsden & Tuck Reference Ramsden and Tuck2001; Ryan Reference Ryan, Friesen and Mason2016; Savelle & Dyke 2009; Reference Savelle and Dyke2014). Environmental change and significant temporal and geographic gaps also occur during the subsequent Dorset sequence (Grønnow & Sørensen Reference Grønnow, Sørensen, Arneborg and Grønnow2006; Holly Reference Holly, Sassaman and Holly2011; Jensen Reference Jensen and Sutherland2005; Savelle & Dyke Reference Savelle and Dyke2014). In short, with geographic and temporal gaps, cultural discontinuities and a dynamic Arctic environment, one can find in the Paleo-Inuit sequence ample opportunities for forgetting in one form or another.

Passive forgetting can be expected with a quick pivot to new environmental landscapes or conditions and resources. Indeed, some posit that changing climatic conditions can account for most of the archaeological changes witnessed across the Pre-Dorset/Dorset transition (Fitzhugh Reference Fitzhugh1976; Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985, 107–10). People may have responded to colder temperatures and the expansion of sea ice, for instance, by living more frequently in snow houses directly on the sea ice and closer to their prey, as indicated by the increased presence of snow knives and proliferation of stone lamps in the early Dorset period. In this context, snow houses and portable light and heat are envisioned as innovations and adaptations to a changing environment. Yet Pre-Dorset peoples appear to have managed without elaborate snow houses—even in the bitterly cold High Arctic—for centuries, and portable lamps did not reach the Canadian Arctic until just prior to the Dorset period (Grønnow et al. Reference Grønnow, Appelt, Odgaard and Gulløv2014). The Dorset also retained these technologies when warmer weather set in during later Dorset times and when they occupied subarctic environments (Damkjar Reference Damkjar and Sutherland2005, 162–3; see Holly Reference Holly, Sassaman and Holly2011). Snow knives and lamps continued to be used on the island of Newfoundland, for instance, where both light and wood was plentiful (Erwin Reference Erwin2016). Accordingly, while it is hard to deny the utility of snow houses and portable light and heat, their absence, addition and retention in conditions where they were not narrowly necessary for survival suggests culturally informed agency too.

Paleo-Inuit peoples also ceased, or at least reduced, open-water hunting and stopped using the bow and arrow at the time of the Pre-Dorset/Dorset transition. This has also been explained as part of an adaptative shift from an emphasis on interior terrestrial resources, such as caribou, to coastal, sea-ice resources with colder temperatures (Maschner & Mason Reference Maschner and Mason2013, 135–6; Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985, 110). Hunting of terrestrial resources did continue in many regions without the bow and arrow (Friesen Reference Friesen, Krupnik and Crowell2020b; Mary-Rousselière Reference Mary-Rousselière1976; Milne et al. Reference Milne, Park, Stenton and Preston2013), but the loss of the bow and arrow arguably would have narrowed subsistence options and strategies and put small bands and families at greater risk of starvation (Howse Reference Howse2019). In other places, the Dorset may have ceased hunting caribou for social rather than economic reasons (Holly Reference Holly2019, 1443–5). That decision, together with the shedding of the bow and arrow, may likewise have narrowed their ability to adapt to changes in the resource environment and to access resources in areas occupied by neighbouring peoples (Erwin et al. Reference Erwin, Holly, Hull and Rast2005; Holly Reference Holly, Sassaman and Holly2011).

Sharp changes in environmental and resource conditions could also have resulted in depopulation events, and unintentional forgetting of the undesired sort. Some have posited broad and sweeping human extinction events in Paleo-Inuit prehistory (Tuck Reference Tuck1976), but even the untimely demise of a small number of prime-aged hunters, knowledge-bearers, or craftspeople could have led to cultural and technological losses (see McGhee Reference McGhee1994). The disappearance of kayaks and essential hunting technology among the aforementioned Polar Inughuit, for instance, may have occurred when just a subset of the community perished (see LeMoine & Darwent Reference LeMoine, Darwent, Friesen and Mason2016).

McGhee (Reference McGhee1996, 146) takes the absence of dogs as an indication that the Early Dorset had difficulty feeding themselves, following the logic that dogs would have been the first to starve during times of food shortage; thus, the Early Dorset could not afford the luxury of dogs because they lived precariously at the edge of hunger. From an adaptative perspective, dogs would seem useful to the Dorset economy given that it was focused on sea-ice hunting and that they would have been immensely valuable in helping hunters locate seal breathing holes (Park Reference Park and Pauketat2012, 121–2; cf. Morey & Aaris-Sørensen Reference Morey and Aaris–Sørensen2002). Without dogs, the Dorset may have been compelled to focus more on ice-edge hunting, to their detriment (Cox & Spiess Reference Cox and Spiess1980). Does the absence of dogs then point to some calamity? Following McGhee’s (Reference McGhee1996, 146) starvation logic, if there were a human bottleneck demographic event, the associated dog population presumably would have been hit hardest—maybe to the point of vanishing. Indeed, there is no good evidence for dogs during the Dorset period. Even the discarded bones of animals do not bear gnaw marks from them at this time (Howse & Friesen Reference Howse and Friesen2016, 4). Dogs are present (albeit rare) in Pre-Dorset contexts, however. Perhaps tellingly, some show signs of having been butchered for food (Morey & Aaris-Sørensen Reference Morey and Aaris–Sørensen2002).

It is also plausible that some Dorset losses were the result of intentional forgetting. Cataclysmic events and dire circumstances are known to inspire people radically to reimagine their cultures. Anthropologists have long noted the connection between cultural revitalization movements and episodes of social stress (e.g. Linton Reference Linton1943; Wallace Reference Wallace1956). With this in mind, it is possible the Early Dorset sought transformative ideological and social solutions to their troubled times. McGhee (Reference McGhee1996, 142–4) wonders if the Dorset drew a connection between a spinning drill, used to bore holes, and the winds that produced storms, and abandoned the former in hopes of preventing the latter. He also thinks it is possible that the Dorset blamed the bow and arrow for causing starvation because it was no longer proving as effective as other hunting technologies, and stopped using it. We note that McGhee’s wind-storm theory could account for both the rejection of the bow and arrow and the drill, especially if the Pre-Dorset were employing a bow-drill and not spinning a drill between their palms (see Grønnow Reference Grønnow2017, 158). Putting aside the fact that storms would have always occurred in the Arctic past (although their frequency and intensity might have varied), and the highly conjectural nature of McGhee’s thesis, the case for the conscious rejection of technology as a mechanism of forgetting might better account for some Dorset losses than cataclysmic demographic events, given how relatively widespread the Pre-Dorset population was, at least during some periods (Maxwell Reference Maxwell1985; Savelle & Dyke Reference Savelle and Dyke2009). The Dorset were not the Polar Inughuit. If the Pre-Dorset/Dorset transition occurred among a geographically more expansive and populous community, it would seem easier to infer the spread of an idea than it would be to account for the elimination of everyone who possessed the knowledge of what was lost.

That the Dorset may have engaged in selective, intentional forgetting as a response to stress also finds support in what was added to their culture. In addition to technologies such as ‘ice creepers’ for traction on ice and snow knives for construction of snow houses, there is an explosion of figurative art and shamanism-related objects, and the eventual appearance of longhouse structures during the Dorset period. These developments have been explained by archaeologists as magical and cultural efforts to respond to various social and environmental stresses—the arrival of the Thule, threats to an egalitarian ethic, a dramatically changing resource environment, the need to foster interactions over greater distances, and a dwindling population (Appelt et al. Reference Appelt, Damkjar, Friesen, Friesen and Mason2016; Friesen Reference Friesen2007; McGhee Reference McGhee1981, 51; Schledermann Reference Schledermann1990, 332; Taçon Reference Taçon1983). We can think of them as idea-driven solutions to stress too, but in the additive rather than subtractive sense.

Forgetting in Arctic antiquity, and forgetting

Two theoretical positions have long dominated Eastern Arctic archaeology—diffusionism and processualism (Holly 2002; Reference Holly2013; Hood Reference Hood1998). Each has its own way of understanding forgetting. In the former, ideas constitute culture; they can be lost when people die, gain traction and circulate with social interaction and stagnate with isolation. It was once thought, for instance, that snowshoes were invented in the Lake Baikal region of Siberia and then the idea/technology spread out from there, transforming cultures along the way. The (presumed) geographically isolated Kivallirmiut (Caribou Inuit) were believed to be late adopters, and thus to have retained aspects of a pre-snowshoe culture into recent times (Birket-Smith Reference Birket-Smith1930). In the latter, ideas are not especially unique or culturally and historically contingent. The processualist expectation is that people generate similar ideas under similar conditions. Thus, the snowshoe would appear where it was needed and vanish where it was not; to wit, it was lost when the first indigenous Americans crossed through the tropics, and it was rediscovered when conditions once again called for it in the snowy interior of Tierra del Fuego (see Cooper Reference Cooper1945).

Passive forgetting of the adaptive sort had to have been a common occurrence in Arctic antiquity. One example is flaked burins. Burins are steep-angled stone tools used for carving implements out of bone, antler and wood. In the early Paleo-Inuit record, flaked burins are common, but over time they were gradually replaced by ‘burin-like tools’ on which the working facet is ground, not flaked. By the end of the Dorset period, many aspects of the manufacture of flaked burins had seemingly been forgotten, due to the adoption of the new, burin-like tools (Desrosiers & Sørensen Reference Desrosiers, Sørensen, Friesen and Mason2016). Another example is the widespread loss of ceramic technology among Inuit groups (Pre-Dorset and Dorset peoples in the Eastern Arctic never made ceramics). Ceramic pots and lamps are relatively common in the earliest Thule Inuit sites, but by the nineteenth century they had become rare and geographically restricted (Savelle Reference Savelle1986). Their diminished popularity is often interpreted as the result of both the more difficult conditions in the east to make fired ceramics (colder temperatures, lack of driftwood) and the more frequent use of soapstone (steatite) suitable for making pots and lamps (Arnold & Stimmell Reference Arnold and Stimmell1983).

Unintentional forgetting may not have been uncommon either. As a consequence of any variety of calamities, the constitution of small Arctic communities can change dramatically in a short period of time (Burch Reference Burch1998, 320–23; Corbett Reference Corbett, Corbett, West and Lefèvre2010, 42; McGhee Reference McGhee1994). By chance alone, analogous to genetic drift (see Koerper & Stickel Reference Koerper and Stickel1980), tragic events could quickly eliminate an entire set of knowledge-keepers in a small community (Jordan Reference Jordan2015, 26). If that knowledge was highly specialized or secret, loss would be even more likely. Consider, for instance, how few Aleut possessed the esoteric knowledge and skills of their whale-hunting shamans (Corbett & Hanson Reference Corbett and Hanson2023, 216–18). The demarcation of knowledge, technology, and practice by gender (Jordan Reference Jordan2015, 347, 364), might also have worked to magnify losses in small populations. On Little Diomede Island, women sewed but never carved, and men alone hunted marine mammals (Jolles Reference Jolles, Jarvenpa and Brumbach2006; Phillips-Chan Reference Phillips-Chan2021, 26). Indeed, in many hunter-gatherer societies, men actively restricted women’s access to hunting knowledge and technology (Brightman Reference Brightman1996)—practices that could prove catastrophic when small communities suffered the sudden loss of prime-aged hunters. We might inversely imagine how the loss of women could affect a community, if, for instance, they were the primary navigators (Chapman Reference Chapman, McEwan, Borrero and Prieto1997, 82–3), clothiers (LeMoine Reference LeMoine2003), or fire keepers (see Hrynick & Betts Reference Hrynick, Betts, Holyoke and Hrynick2022). We may also consider how the vertical transmission of information—from elder to child—in small hunter-gatherer societies (Prentiss Reference Prentiss, Sassaman and Holly2011, 18; cf. Hewlett et al. Reference Hewlett, Fouts, Boyette and Hewlett2011; Jordan Reference Jordan2015, 111, 124–5, 202–3) could also compartmentalize knowledge. That said, people are apt to be familiar with practices even if they do not engage in them regularly and could probably get by in a pinch if they needed to. If, however, a society could manage without something, that ‘something’ could, feasibly, truly disappear if the people that ‘knew it’ did so.

Given the broad networks and high mobility of Eastern Arctic peoples, as evident in the wide distribution of cultural traits—for example, with the Dorset—it is perhaps more likely that forgetting was often intentional rather than accidental and unintentional. Here, as in the drift scenario above, sharp pivots in cultural practices are magnified by the small size of communities, but the process is different. In such cases, a small coalition of influential actors, or a charismatic individual (e.g. a shaman), or an affected and vocal community, successfully advocates for the discarding of a technology or practice so that in a short time a lightly populated but highly connected constellation of communities across a vast region wilfully abandons, for instance, their bows and arrows and bow-drills. Or, by a similar process, a contagion-like spread of ideas quickly adds traits to a cultural tradition—for example, portable art and amulets. For how this might actually work, we might consider how legends, rumours, and contrived evidence have conspired in ways that had people in the United States in the 1980s cancelling Halloween for fear of drug-laced candy, and on guard for satanic cults (see Ellis Reference Ellis1993; Reference Ellis and Santino1994). Could an analogous panic have put a wilful end to wind-causing Dorset drills and weapons?

Support for intentional forgetting on the part of the Dorset may be inferred from the shallow nature of the Arctic archaeological record itself. If we accept that artifacts and ruins can inspire new memories and histories (Borić Reference Borić and Borić2010b; Hendon Reference Hendon2010; Iverson Reference Iverson2017; Van Dyke & Alcock Reference Van Dyke and Alcock2003)—including even a later mythologizing of the Dorset by the Inuit (Mathiassen Reference Mathiassen1927; Park Reference Park1993, 220)—it stands to reason that a landscape littered on the surface with Pre-Dorset tools would have provided opportunities for the Dorset to rediscover some ‘lost arts’ that had disappeared previously if death alone (unintentional forgetting) had placed them there.

Forgetting

As the reader may have surmised by now, we are reluctant to assert a one-size-fits-all explanation for Dorset forgetting—let alone for Arctic or archaeological forgetting in general. All kinds of forgetting are apt to have been in play in Arctic history—and more broadly in human history writ large. We would, however, go so far as to suggest that human agency was reasonably present almost everywhere forgetting occurred. Agents of passive forgetting of the adaptive or progressive sort were certainly mindful of what was happening and what would be lost. Such forward-thinking nostalgia could sometimes have inspired people to resist it. People were aware of bow-and-arrow technology in the interior of the western Subarctic for millennia before they adopted it (Kristensen et al. Reference Kristensen, Hare and Gotthardt2019, 14), and when they did, they surely knew what was being lost. Teachers that formerly taught spelling and cursive—and that still teach AI unassisted writing—do too. Archaeologists, in reading the material remains of culture change, should consider the weight of such decisions—even if they make ‘adaptive’ sense or are ‘for the best’—and look for signs of future-facing nostalgia in the form of the lingering retention of technologies and practices. Examples of this may include the Newfoundland Dorset’s seeming reluctance to abandon traditional seal-hunting places and to pivot to new subsistence strategies when warming temperatures affected resource availability, or the Dorset’s retention of snow houses and lamps (Erwin Reference Erwin2016; Holly Reference Holly, Sassaman and Holly2011; Reference Holly2013, 103–8).

By definition, intentional forgetting is a self-conscious, agent-driven form of memory erasure, but its realization is apt to vary by society and conditions. Societies can be wilfully progressive, or isolationist, or conservative (see Finkel & Barkai Reference Finkel and Barkai2018; Hegmon et al. Reference Hegmon, Peeples, Kinzig, Kulow, Meegan and Nelson2008; Holly Reference Holly, Sassaman and Holly2011; Kristensen et al. Reference Kristensen, Hare and Gotthardt2019), with implications for how, and how often, forgetting occurs. Social conditions too may affect its prevalence. As already mentioned, periods of social stress are known to engender revitalization movements that often include efforts of intentional erasure as part of a broad effort at remaking society. The weaponization of intentional forgetting, likewise, may be more common with fragile political systems, such as chiefdoms, or at moments of political succession or conquest. And, as contemplated with the Dorset, efforts at intentional forgetting may be more common in small, tight-knit communities where messaging from charismatic individuals (e.g. shamans) or an unusual collective experience (e.g. a tragedy) could spark contagious messaging/panics and quick pivots in social or technological practices.

Finally, in a roundabout way, the ghost of human agency may even be present in some forms of unintentional forgetting. Unintentional forgetting that results from the sudden loss of knowledge keepers, for instance, may be traced to cultural practices that intentionally aimed to restrict information to specific subgroups. We are reminded of Hegmon and colleagues’ (Reference Hegmon, Peeples, Kinzig, Kulow, Meegan and Nelson2008) brilliant evocation of a rigidity trap, in which a society’s procrastination (rigidity) in addressing some small problem eventually leads to a critical inflection point (trap) in which a very hard cultural adjustment is required, if it is even still possible. The classic example is when failure to conduct regular controlled burns in a forest results in the accumulation of deadwood and underbrush that later fuels a conflagration. Humans may be faultless for the lightning strike, but not for the scale of devastation wrought by it. Likewise, if a society deems only men, and only certain men, worthy of particular knowledge, the loss that comes from their sudden demise may be unintentional, but it is not without human responsibility—agency.

We recognize the conjectural nature of these scenarios, but offer them as examples of how to contemplate the process of forgetting. The Dorset case study is particularly useful in this instance since a series of archaeologically recognizable phenomena were demonstrably ‘forgotten’, but as with so many other archaeological instances, we lack the resolution to make fine-grained inferences about the precise social and environmental contexts that informed these losses. For archaeology more broadly, we advocate more attention being paid to understanding elements of past societies that have been forgotten. By considering the conditions that led to their forgetting, and the degree to which they were driven by passive, unintentional, or intentional processes, such work has the potential to bolster our understanding of deep underlying currents of change in past societies. It could also help clear a humanistic and human-willed space for agency and history in the arena of forgetting.

References

Allen, H., 1979. Left out in the cold: Why the Tasmanians stopped eating fish. Artefact 4, 110.Google Scholar
Appelt, M., Damkjar, E. & Friesen, T.M., 2016. Late Dorset, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 783806.Google Scholar
Appelt, M. & Gulløv, H.C., 2009. Tunit, Norsemen, and Inuit in thirteenth-century northwest Greenland – Dorset between the Devil and the Deep Sea, in The Northern World AD 900–1400, eds Maschner, H., Mason, O. & McGhee, R.. Salt Lake City (UT): University of Utah Press, 300320.Google Scholar
Arbagi, M., 2011. The Catholic Church and the preservation of Mesoamerican archives: an assessment. Archival Issues 33, 112–20.10.31274/archivalissues.10979CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, C.D. & Stimmell, C., 1983. An analysis of Thule pottery. Canadian Journal of Archaeology 7, 121.Google Scholar
Balée, W., 2013. The Cultural Forests of the Amazon. Tuscaloosa (AL): University of Alabama Press.10.2307/jj.30347164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Yosef, O., 2002. The Upper Paleolithic Revolution. Annual Review of Anthropology 31, 363–93.10.1146/annurev.anthro.31.040402.085416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, B., 1993. Stonehenge – contested landscapes (Medieval to present day), in Landscape: Politics and Perspective, ed. Bender, B.. Providence (RI): Berg, 245–79.Google Scholar
Bender, B., 2002. Time and landscape. Current Anthropology 43(S), 103–12.Google Scholar
Bennett, J. & Rowley, S., 2004. Uqalurait: An oral history of Nunavut. Montreal: McGill-Queen’s University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bessire, L., 2014. Behold the Black Caiman: A chronicle of Ayoreo life. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226175607.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bettinger, R.L., Barton, L. & Morgan, C., 2010. The origins of food production in northern China: a different kind of agricultural revolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 19, 921.Google Scholar
Bettinger, R.L. & Baumhoff, M.A., 1982. The Numic spread: Great Basin cultures in competition. American Antiquity 47, 485503.10.2307/280231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birket-Smith, K., 1930. Folk wanderings and culture drifts in northern North America. Journal de la Société des Américanistes 22, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borić, D., 2010a. Introduction: memory, archaeology and the historical condition, in Archaeology and Memory, ed. Borić, D.. Oxford: Oxbow, 134.Google Scholar
Borić, D., 2010b. Happy forgetting? Remembering and dismembering dead bodies at Vlasac, in Archaeology and Memory, ed. Borić, D.. Oxford: Oxbow, 4867.Google Scholar
Bowdler, S., 1980. Fish and culture: a Tasmanian polemic. Mankind 12, 334–40.Google Scholar
Bowdler, S., 1988. Tasmanian Aborigines in the Hunter Islands in the Holocene, in The Archaeology of Prehistoric Coastlines, eds Bailey, G. & Parkington, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 4252.Google Scholar
Brightman, R., 1996. The social division of foraging labor: biology, taboo, and gender politics. Comparative Studies in Society and History 38, 687729.Google Scholar
Briner, J.P., McKay, N.P., Axford, Y., et al., 2016. Holocene climate change in Arctic Canada and Greenland. Quaternary Science Reviews 147, 340–64.Google Scholar
Brown, S.K., Darwent, C.M. & Sacks, B.N., 2013. Ancient DNA evidence for genetic continuity in Arctic dogs. Journal of Archaeological Science 40, 1279–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burch, E.S. Jr, 1998. The Iñupiaq Eskimo Nations of Northwest Alaska. Fairbanks (AK): University of Alaska Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, A., 1997. The great ceremonies of the Selk’nam and the Yámana, in Patagonia, eds McEwan, C., Borrero, L.A. & Prieto, A.. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press, 82109.Google Scholar
Clayton, T., 1998. Building the new Cambodia: educational destruction and construction under the Khmer Rouge, 1975–1979. History of Education Quarterly 38(1), 116.Google Scholar
Cohen, M.N., 2007. Were early agriculturalists less healthy than food-collectors?, in Discovering Anthropology, eds Ember, C.R., Ember, M. & Peregrine, P.. Upper Saddle River (NJ): Pearson, 8995.Google Scholar
Connerton, P., 1989. How Societies Remember. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511628061CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connerton, P., 2009. How Modernity Forgets. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511627187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, J.M., 1945. Aboriginal South American snowshoes. Primitive Man 18(3/4), 63–9.10.2307/3316332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corbett, D., 2010. Ethnographic background, in The People at the End of the World: The Western Aleutians Project and the archaeology of Shemya Island, eds Corbett, D., West, D. & Lefèvre, C.. (AAA Monograph Series 8.) Anchorage (AK): Alaska Anthropological Association, 2548.Google Scholar
Corbett, D. & Hanson, D., 2023. Culture and Archaeology of the Ancestral Unangax/Aleut of the Aleutian Islands, Alaska. Cham: Springer.Google Scholar
Cormier, L.A., 2003. Kinship with Monkeys: The Guajá foragers of eastern Amazonia. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, L.J., Sternke, F. & Woodman, P.C., 2005. Microlith to macrolith: the reasons behind the transformation of production in the Irish Mesolithic. Antiquity 79, 1933.Google Scholar
Cox, S.L. & Spiess, A., 1980. Dorset settlement and subsistence in northern Labrador. Arctic 33, 659–69.10.14430/arctic2589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damkjar, E., 2005. Late Dorset longhouses: a look inside, in Contributions to the Study of the Dorset Palaeo Eskimos, ed Sutherland, P.D.. Gatineau (QC): Canadian Museum of Civilization, 147–65.10.2307/j.ctv16p21.17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwent, C.M., 2004. The highs and lows of High Arctic mammals: temporal change and regional variability in Paleoeskimo subsistence, in Colonisation, Migration and Marginal Areas, eds Mondini, M., Muñoz, S. & Wickler, S.. Oxford: Oxbow, 6273.Google Scholar
Desjardins, S.P.A. & Jordan, P.D., 2019. Arctic archaeology and climate change. Annual Review of Anthropology 48, 279–96.Google Scholar
Desrosiers, P.M. & Sørensen, M., 2016. Paleoeskimo lithic technology, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 153–74.Google Scholar
Diamond, J., 1997. Guns, Germs, and Steel: The fates of human societies. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Dibble, H.L., Abodolahzadeh, A., Aldeins, V., Goldberg, P., McPherron, S.P. & Sandgathe, D.M., 2017. How did hominins adapt to Ice Age Europe without fire? Current Anthropology 58(S), 267–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, K., 1984. Children of the Forest: Africa’s Mbuti Pygmies. Prospect Heights (IL): Waveland Press.Google Scholar
Dyke, A.S. & Savelle, J.M., 2009. Paleoeskimo demography and sea–level history, Kent Peninsula and King William Island, Central Northwest Passage, Arctic Canada. Arctic 62, 371–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B., 1993. The Highgate Cemetery vampire hunt: the Anglo-American connection in satanic cult lore. Folklore 104, 1339.Google Scholar
Ellis, B., 1994. ‘Safe’ spooks: new Halloween Ttraditions in response to sadism legends, in Halloween and Other Festivals of Death and Life, ed. Santino, J.. Knoxville (TN): University of Tennessee Press, 2444.Google Scholar
Erwin, J.C., 2016. A large–scale systematic study of Dorset and Groswater soapstone vessel fragments from Newfoundland and Labrador. Arctic 69(S1), 18 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erwin, J.C., Holly, D.H. Jr, Hull, S.H. & Rast, T.L., 2005. Form and function of projectile points and the trajectory of Newfoundland prehistory. Canadian Journal of Archaeology 29, 4667.Google Scholar
Finkel, M. & Barkai, R., 2018. The Acheulean handaxe technological persistence: a case of preferred cultural conservatism. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 84, 119.10.1017/ppr.2018.2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkelstein, S.A., 2016. Reconstructing Middle and Late Holocene Paleoclimates of the eastern Arctic and Greenland, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 653–72.Google Scholar
Fitzhugh, W., 1976. Environmental factors in the evolution of Dorset Culture: a marginal proposal for Hudson Bay, in ‘Eastern Arctic Prehistory: Paleoeskimo problems’, ed. M.S. Maxwell, Memoirs of the Society for American Archaeology 31, 139–44.10.1017/S0081130000000824CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannery, T., 1994. The Future Eaters: An ecological history of the Australasian lands and people. New York: Grove Press.Google Scholar
Friesen, T.M., 2007. Hearthrows, hierarchies and Arctic hunter–gatherers: the construction of equality in the Late Dorset period. World Archaeology 39, 194214.10.1080/00438240701249686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, T.M., 2016. Pan–Arctic population movements: the early Paleo–Inuit and Thule Inuit migrations, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 673–92.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199766956.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, T.M., 2017. Archaeology of the eastern Arctic, in Out of the Cold: Archaeology on the Arctic rim of North America, eds Mason, O.K. & Friesen, T.M.. Washington (DC): Society for American Archaeology Press, 133206.Google Scholar
Friesen, T.M., 2020a. Radiocarbon evidence for fourteenth–century Dorset occupation in the eastern North American Arctic. American Antiquity 85, 222–40.10.1017/aaq.2019.88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friesen, T.M., 2020b. Cyclical crashes or continuous abundance? Using archaeological data to infer long-term caribou population dynamics on Victoria Island, Nunavut, in Arctic Crashes: People and animals in the changing North, eds Krupnik, I. & Crowell, A.L.. Washington (DC): Smithsonian Scholarly Press, 6178.Google Scholar
Garvey, R., 2018. Cultural transmission and sources of diversity: a comparison of temperate maritime foragers of the northern and southern hemispheres, in Foraging in the Past: Archaeological Studies of Hunter–Gatherer Diversity, ed. Lemke, A.K.. Louisville (CO): University Press of Colorado, 1948.Google Scholar
Gilman, A., 1984. Explaining the Upper Paleolithic Revolution, in Marxist Perspectives in Archaeology, ed Spriggs, M.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 115–26.Google Scholar
Gosden, C. & Lock, G., 1998. Prehistoric histories. World Archaeology 30, 212.10.1080/00438243.1998.9980393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
González–Ruibal, A., 2016. Land of amnesia: power, predation, and heritage in central Africa, in Excavating Memory: Sites of remembering and forgetting, eds Starzmann, M.T. & Roby, J.R.. Gainesville (FL): University Press of Florida, 131–52.Google Scholar
Gott, B., 2002. Fire-making in Tasmania: absence of evidence is not evidence of absence. Current Anthropology 43, 650–56.Google Scholar
Grønnow, B., 2016. Independence I and Saqqaq: the first Greenlanders, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 713–35.Google Scholar
Grønnow, B., 2017. The Frozen Saqqaq Sites of Disko Bay, West Greenland: Qeqertasussuk and Qajaa (2400–900 BC). Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press.Google Scholar
Grønnow, B., Appelt, M. & Odgaard, U., 2014. In the light of blubber: the earliest stone lamps in Greenland and beyond, in Northern Worlds – Landscapes, interactions and dynamics, ed. Gulløv, H.C.. Copenhagen: National Museum of Denmark, 403–22.Google Scholar
Grønnow, B. & Sørensen, M., 2006. Palaeo–Eskimo migrations into Greenland: the Canadian connection, in Dynamics of Northern Societies, eds Arneborg, J. & Grønnow, B.. Copenhagen: Greenland Research Centre at the National Museum of Denmark, 5974.Google Scholar
Hegmon, M., Peeples, M.A., Kinzig, A.P., Kulow, S., Meegan, C.M. & Nelson, M.C., 2008. Social transformation and its human costs in the Prehispanic U.S. Southwest. American Anthropologist 110, 313–24.10.1111/j.1548-1433.2008.00041.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendon, J.A., 2010. Houses in a Landscape: Memory and everyday life in Mesoamerica. Durham (NC): Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., 2004. Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—the Tasmanian case. Antiquity 69, 197214.10.2307/4128416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B.S., Fouts, H.N., Boyette, A.H. & Hewlett, B.L., 2011. Social learning among the Congo Basin hunter–gatherers. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences 366, 1168–78.10.1098/rstb.2010.0373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, K., Wood, B.M., Baggio, J., Hurtado, A.M. & Boyd, R.T., 2014. Hunter-gatherer inter-band interaction rates: implications for cumulative culture. PLoS One 9(e102806), 19.10.1371/journal.pone.0102806CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton, L., 1996. Flutes of Fire: Essays on California Indian languages. Berkeley (CA): Heyday Books.Google Scholar
Holly, D.H. Jr, 2002. Subarctic ‘prehistory’ in the anthropological imagination. Arctic Anthropology 39, 1026.Google Scholar
Holly, D.H. Jr, 2011. When foragers fail: in the eastern Subarctic, for example, in Hunter–Gatherer Archaeology as Historical Process, eds Sassaman, K.E. & Holly, D.H. Jr. Tucson (AZ): University of Arizona Press, 7992.Google Scholar
Holly, D.H. Jr, 2013. History in the Making: The archaeology of the eastern subArctic. Lanham (MD): AltaMira.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holly, D.H. Jr, 2019. Toward a social archaeology of food for hunters and gatherers in marginal environments. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 26, 1439–69.10.1007/s10816-019-09415-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holly, D.H. Jr, Wolff, C.B. & Hull, S.H., 2022. The struggle was real: on the end of the Archaic on the island of Newfoundland and Labrador, in The Far Northeast: 3000 BP to contact, eds Holyoke, K.R. & Hrynick, M.G.. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press, 2347.Google Scholar
Holmberg, A.R., [1950] 1969. Nomads of the Long Bow: The Siriono of eastern Bolivia. Garden City (NY): Natural History Press.Google Scholar
Hood, B.C., 1998. Theory on ice: the discourse of eastern Canadian Arctic Paleo-Eskimo archaeology. Acta Borealia 15, 358.10.1080/08003839808580478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horton, D.R., 1979. Tasmanian adaptation. Mankind 12, 2834.Google Scholar
Houmard, C., 2018. Cultural continuity from Pre–Dorset to Dorset in the eastern Canadian Arctic highlighted by bone technology and typology. Arctic Anthropology 55, 2447.10.3368/aa.55.1.24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howse, L., 2019. Hunting technologies and archaeofaunas: societal differences between hunter-gatherers of the eastern Arctic. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 26, 88111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howse, L. & Friesen, T.M., 2016. Technology, taphonomy, and seasonality: understanding differences between Dorset and Thule subsistence strategies at Iqaluktuvq, Victoria Island. Arctic 69, 115.10.14430/arctic4640CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrynick, M.G. & Betts, M.W., 2022. ‘And we showered with a thousand praises the women who had been the fire’s guardian’: ancestral Wabanaki gender and place–making in the Woodland Period, in The Far Northeast: 3000 BP to Contact, eds Holyoke, K.R. & Hrynick, M.G.. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press, 259–83.Google Scholar
Isaac, B.L., 1977. The Siriono of eastern Bolivia: a reexamination. Human Ecology 5, 137–54.10.1007/BF00889540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, S.D., 2017. The enduring Toltecs: history and truth during the Aztec-to-colonial transition at Tula, Hidalgo. Journal of Archaeological Methods and Theory 24, 90116.Google Scholar
Jenness, D., 1925. A new Eskimo culture in Hudson Bay. Geographical Review 15, 428–37.Google Scholar
Jensen, J.F., 2005. Palaeo–Eskimo continuity and discontinuity in west Greenland, in Contributions to the Study of the Dorset Palaeo–Eskimos, ed. Sutherland, P.D.. Gatineau (QC): Canadian Museum of Civilization, 93103.Google Scholar
Jolles, C.Z., 2006. Iñupiaq society and gender relations, in Circumpolar Lives and Livelihoods, eds Jarvenpa, R. & Brumbach, H.J.. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 238–62.Google Scholar
Jones, R., 1977. The Tasmanian paradox, in Stone Tools as Cultural Markers: Change, evolution and complexity, ed Wright, R.V.S.. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies, 189204.Google Scholar
Jones, R., 1978. Why did the Tasmanians stop eating fish?, in Explorations in Ethno–archaeology, ed. Gould, R.A.. Albuquerque (NM): University of New Mexico Press, 1147.Google Scholar
Jordan, P., 2015. Technology as Human Social Tradition: Cultural transmission among hunter–gatherers. Oakland (CA): University of California Press.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, E.K. & Riede, F., 2019. Convergent catastrophes and the termination of the Arctic Norwegian Stone Age: a multi–proxy assessment of the demographic adaptive responses of mid–Holocene collectors to biophysical forcing. Holocene 29, 17821800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn, M., 1990. Stone-faced ancestors: the spatial anchoring of myth in Wamira, Papua New Guinea. Ethnology 29, 5166.Google Scholar
Kirch, P.V., 2000. On the Road of the Winds: An archaeological history of the Pacific Islands before European contact. Berkeley (CA): University of California Press.Google Scholar
Koerper, H. & Stickel, E.G., 1980. Cultural drift: a primary process of culture change. Journal of Anthropological Research 36, 463–9.Google Scholar
Kristensen, T.J., Hare, P.G., Gotthardt, R.M., et al., 2019. The movement of obsidian in Subarctic Canada: Holocene social relationships and human responses to a large–scale volcanic eruption. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 56(101114), 118.10.1016/j.jaa.2019.101114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küchler, S., 1997. Sacrificial economy and its objects: rethinking colonial collecting in Oceania. Journal of Material Culture 2, 3960.10.1177/135918359700200102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küchler, S., 1999. The place of memory, in The Art of Forgetting, eds Forty, A. & Küchler, S.. Oxford: Berg, 5372.Google Scholar
Labrèche, Y., 2015. Relecture critique des interprétations relatives aux interactions entre Thuléens et Dorsétiens au Nunavik et au Nunatsiavut. Études/Inuit/Studies 39, 205–31.Google Scholar
Lape, P., 2005. Archaeological approaches to the study of Islam in Island Southeast Asia. Antiquity 79, 829–36.Google Scholar
LeMoine, G., 2003. Woman of the house: gender, architecture, and ideology in Dorset prehistory. Arctic Anthropology 40, 121–38.Google ScholarPubMed
LeMoine, G.E. & Darwent, C.M., 2016. Development of Polar Inughuit culture in the Smith Sound region, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 873–96.Google Scholar
Liebmann, M., 2008. The innovative materiality of revitalization movements: lessons from the Pueblo Revolt of 1680. American Anthropologist 110, 360–72.10.1111/j.1548-1433.2008.00045.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linton, R., 1943. Nativistic movements. American Anthropologist 45, 230–40.10.1525/aa.1943.45.2.02a00070CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macdonald, K., 2018. Fire-free hominin strategies for coping with cool winter temperatures in north-western Europe from before 800,00 to circa 400,00 years ago. PaleoAnthropology 2018, 726.Google Scholar
Mary-Rousselière, G., 1976. The Paleoeskimo in northern Baffinland, in ‘Eastern Arctic Prehistory: Paleoeskimo problems’, ed. M.S. Maxwell, Memoirs of the Society for American Archaeology 31, 4057.10.1017/S0081130000000769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maschner, H.D.G., 2016. Archaeology of the eastern Aleut region, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 323–48.Google Scholar
Maschner, H.D.G. & Mason, O.K., 2013. The bow and arrow in northern North America. Evolutionary Anthropology 22, 133–8.10.1002/evan.21357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathiassen, T., 1927. Archeology of the Central Eskimos. Report of the Fifth Thule Expedition, 1921–1924, vol. 4. Copenhagen: Gyldendal.Google Scholar
Maxwell, M.S., 1985. Prehistory of the Eastern Arctic. Orlando (FL): Academic Press.Google Scholar
Maxwell, M.S., 1997. The Canadian Arctic in transition: Pre–Dorset to Dorset, in Fifty Years of Arctic Research: Anthropological studies from Greenland to Siberia, eds Gilberg, R. & Gulløv, H.C.. Copenhagen: Department of Ethnography, National Museum of Denmark, 205–8.Google Scholar
McGhee, R., 1981. A tale of two cultures: a prehistoric village in the Canadian Arctic. Archaeology 34, 4451.Google Scholar
McGhee, R., 1994. Disease and the development of Inuit culture. Current Anthropology 35, 565–94.10.1086/204318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGhee, R., 1996. Ancient People of the Arctic. Vancouver: University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
McGhee, R., 1997. Meetings between Dorset Culture Palaeo–Eskimos and Thule Culture Inuit: evidence from Brooman Point, in Fifty Years of Arctic Research: Anthropological studies from Greenland to Siberia, eds Gilberg, R. & Gulløv, H.C.. Copenhagen: Department of Ethnography, National Museum of Denmark, 209–13.Google Scholar
Meldgaard, J., 1962. On the Formative Period of the Dorset Culture, in Prehistoric Cultural Relations Between the Arctic and Temperate Zones of North America, ed. Campbell, J.. Montreal: Arctic Institute of North America, 92–5.Google Scholar
Meldgaard, M., 2004. Ancient Harp Seal Hunters of Disko Bay. Subsistence and settlement at Saqqaq Culture site Qeqertasussuk (2400–1400 BC), West Greenland. (Meddelelser om Grønland: Man and Society 30.) Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meskell, L., 2002. Negative heritage and past mastering in archaeology. Anthropological Quarterly 75, 557–74.10.1353/anq.2002.0050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, S.B., Park, R.W. & Stenton, D.R., 2013. For caribou, chert, and company: assessing mobility as evidence for cultural continuity among the Palaeo-Eskimos of Baffin Island, Arctic Canada, in Mobility, Transition and Change in Prehistory and Classical Antiquity, ed. Preston, P.R.. Oxford: Archeopress, 4961.Google Scholar
Morey, D.F. & Aaris–Sørensen, K., 2002. Paleoeskimo dogs of the eastern Arctic. Arctic 55, 4456.Google Scholar
Nagy, M., 1994. A critical review of the Pre–Dorset/Dorset transition, in Threads of Arctic Prehistory: Papers in honour of William E. Taylor Jr, eds Morrison, D. & Pilon, J.-L.. Hull (QC): Canadian Museum of Civilization, 114.Google Scholar
Nora, P., 1989. Between memory and history: les lieux de mémoire . Representations 26, 724.10.2307/2928520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Connell, J.F., Allen, J. & Hawkes, K., 2010. Pleistocene Sahul and the origins of seafaring, in The Global Origins and Development of Seafaring, eds Anderson, A., Barrett, J.H. & Boyle, K.V.. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 5768.Google Scholar
Odess, D., 2002. Demography and interaction: an appraisal of the core area concept in PaleoEskimo studies, in Honoring our Elders: A History of eastern Arctic archaeology, eds Fitzhugh, W.W., Loring, S. & Odess, D.. Washington (DC): Arctic Studies Center, National Museum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution, 113–19.Google Scholar
Oswalt, W.H., 1987. Technological complexity: the Polar Eskimos and the Tareumiut. Arctic Anthropology 24, 8298.Google Scholar
Park, R.W., 1993. The Dorset–Thule succession in Arctic North America: assessing claims for culture contact. American Antiquity 58, 203–34.10.2307/281966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, R.W., 2012. Adapting to a frozen coastal environment, in The Oxford Handbook of North American Archaeology, ed. Pauketat, T.. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 113–23.Google Scholar
Park, R.W., 2016. The Dorset–Thule transition, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 807–26.Google Scholar
Phillips-Chan, A., 2021. Our Stories Etched in Ivory: The Smithsonian collections of engraved drill bows with stories from the Arctic. Washington (DC): Arctic Studies Center, Smithsonian Institution.Google Scholar
Pinard, C. & Gendron, D., 2009. The Dorset occupation on the south shore of the Hudson Strait: how late?, in The Northern World: AD 900–1400, eds Maschner, H., Mason, O. & McGhee, R.. Salt Lake City (UT): University of Utah Press, 249–59.Google Scholar
Pool, C. & Loughlin, M.L., 2017. Creating memory and negotiating power in the Olmec heartland. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 24, 229–60.Google Scholar
Potter, B.A., 2008. Exploratory models of intersite variability in Mid to Late Holocene central Alaska. Arctic 61, 407–25.Google Scholar
Powell, A., Shennan, S. & Thomas, M.G., 2009. Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324, 298301.10.1126/science.1170165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, J.W., 1888. From barbarism to civilization. American Antiquity 1, 97123.Google Scholar
Prentiss, A.M., 2011. Social histories of complex hunter-gatherers: Pacific Northwest prehistory in a macroevolutionary framework, in Hunter–Gatherer Archaeology as Historical Process, eds Sassaman, K.E. & Holly, D.H. Jr. Tucson (AZ): University of Arizona Press, 1733.Google Scholar
Quinlan, A.R. & Woody, A., 2003. Marks of distinction: rock art and ethnic identification in the Great Basin. American Antiquity 68, 372–90.10.2307/3557085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radcliffe–Brown, A.R., [1922] 1964. The Andaman Islanders. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Raghavan, M., Degiorgio, M., Albrechtsen, A., et al., 2014. The genetic prehistory of the New World Arctic. Science 345, 1255832.Google ScholarPubMed
Ramsden, P. & Tuck, J.A., 2001. A comment on the Pre–Dorset/Dorset transition in the eastern Arctic. Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska n.s. 1, 712.Google Scholar
Read, D., 2008. An interaction model for resource implement complexity based on risk and number of annual moves. American Antiquity 73, 599625.10.1017/S0002731600047326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riede, F., Edinborough, K. & Thomas, M., 2009. Tracking Mesolithic demography in time and space and its implications for explanations of culture change, in Chronology and Evolution in the Mesolithic of North–West Europe, eds Crombé, P., Van Strydonck, M., Sergant, J., Boudin, M. & Bats, M.. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars, 181–99.Google Scholar
Rivers, W.H.R., 1926. The disappearance of useful arts, in Psychology and Ethnology, ed. Smith, G.E.. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co, 190210.Google Scholar
Rowley, G., 1940. The Dorset Culture of the Eastern Arctic. American Anthropologist 42, 490–99.Google Scholar
Rowley, S., 1994. The Sadlermiut: mysterious or misunderstood?, in Threads of Arctic Prehistory: Papers in honour of William E. Taylor Jr, eds Morrison, D. & Pilon, J.-L.. Hull (QC): Canadian Museum of Civilization, 361–84.10.2307/j.ctv16qdx.22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, K., 2016. The ‘Dorset Problem’ revisited: the transitional and Early and Middle Dorset periods in the eastern Arctic, in The Oxford Handbook of the Prehistoric Arctic, eds Friesen, T.M. & Mason, O.K.. New York: Oxford University Press, 761–81.Google Scholar
Savelle, J.M., 1986. Historic Inuit pottery in the eastern Canadian Arctic. Polar Record 23, 319–22.10.1017/S0032247400007130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savelle, J.M. & Dyke, A.S., 2002. Variability in Palaeoeskimo occupation on southwestern Victoria Island, Arctic Canada: causes and consequences. World Archaeology 33, 508–22.Google Scholar
Savelle, J.M. & Dyke, A.S., 2009. Paleoeskimo demography on western Boothia Peninsula, Arctic Canada. Journal of Field Archaeology 34, 267–83.10.1179/009346909791070916CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savelle, J.M. & Dyke, A.S., 2014. Paleoeskimo occupation history of Foxe Basin, Arctic Canada: implications for the core area model and Dorset origins. American Antiquity 79, 249–76.Google Scholar
Schledermann, P., 1990. Crossroads to Greenland: 3000 years of prehistory in the eastern High Arctic. Calgary: Arctic Institute of North America.Google Scholar
Schortman, E. & Urban, P., 2011. Power, memory, and prehistory: constructing and erasing political landscape in the Naco Valley, northwestern Honduras. American Anthropologist 113, 521.10.1111/j.1548-1433.2010.01303.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, G., 2013. Memory and its demolition: ancestors, animals and sacrifice at Umm el–Marra, Syria. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 23, 495522.10.1017/S0959774313000504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stearman, A.M., 1984. The Yuquí connection: another look at Sirionó deculturation. American Anthropologist 86, 630–50.Google Scholar
Strathern, P., 2017. The Medici. New York: Pegasus Books.Google Scholar
Taçon, P.S.C., 1983. An analysis of Dorset art in relation to prehistoric culture stress. Études/Inuit/Studies 7, 4165.Google Scholar
Tallavaara, M. & Pesonen, P., 2020. Human ecodynamics in the north-west coast of Finland 10,000–2000 years ago. Quaternary International 549, 2635.10.1016/j.quaint.2018.06.032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, R., 2007. The polemics of eating fish in Tasmania: the historical evidence revisited. Aboriginal History 31, 126.Google Scholar
Taylor, R., 2008. The polemics of making fire in Tasmania: the historical evidence revisited. Aboriginal History 32, 126.Google Scholar
Thomas, E.M., 2006. The Old Way: A story of the first people. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.Google Scholar
Tuck, J.A., 1976. Paleoeskimo cultures of northern Labrador, in ‘Eastern Arctic Prehistory: Paleoeskimo problems’, ed. M.S. Maxwell. Memoirs of the Society for American Archaeology 31, 89102.Google Scholar
Tylor, E., 1865. Researches into the Early History of Mankind and the Development of Civilization London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Vaesen, K., Collard, M., Cosgrove, R. & Roebroeks, W., 2016. Population size does not explain past changes in cultural complexity. PNAS 113(16), E2241E2247.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, R., 2019. Archaeology and social memory. Annual Review of Anthropology 48, 207–25.10.1146/annurev-anthro-102218-011051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Dyke, R. & Alcock, S.E. (eds), 2003. Archaeologies of Memory. Malden (MA): Blackwell.10.1002/9780470774304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanderwal, R.L., 1978. Adaptive technology in southwest Tasmania. Australian Archaeology 8, 107–27.10.1080/03122417.1978.12093344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, A.F.C., 1956. Revitalization movements. American Anthropologist 58, 264–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walls, M., Knudsen, P. & Larsen, F., 2015. Inughuit open water hunting before the nineteenth century: new dates and questions from Washington Land, Northwestern Greenland. American Antiquity 80, 602–9.Google Scholar
Wissler, C., 1923. Man and Culture. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell.Google Scholar
White, J.P. & O’Connell, J.F., 1982. A Prehistory of Australia, New Guinea and Sahul. Sydney: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Witkowski, S.P. & Brown, C.H., 1978. Lexical universals. Annual Review of Anthropology 7, 427–51.10.1146/annurev.an.07.100178.002235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witkowski, S.R. & Burris, H.W., 1981. Societal complexity and lexical growth. Behavioral Science Research 16, 143–59.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. General chronology for Eastern Arctic archaeology.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Map of the North American Arctic, showing maximum extent of Dorset settlement. This map includes regions occupied by Middle Dorset and Late Dorset; other related cultural phases are not included because their relationship with Dorset is less clear. (Map drafted by Susannah Clinker and Max Friesen.)

Figure 2

Figure 2. Three Dorset artifacts from Victoria Island, Nunavut, showing the Dorset method of gouging linear holes rather than drilling round ones. Left to right: harpoon foreshaft (Middle Dorset); needle (Middle Dorset); plaque/box part with incised decoration (Late Dorset). (Photograph: Max Friesen.)