Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-kl2l2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-26T15:55:30.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Affective dynamics and emotional reactivity in social anxiety disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2025

Beth Foote
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA, USA
Femke Lamers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Mike Xiao
Affiliation:
Child Mind Institute, New York, USA
Lihong Cui
Affiliation:
Genetic Epidemiology Research Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
Vadim Zipunnikov
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
Mathilde M. Husky
Affiliation:
Bordeaux Population Health Research Center, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
Kathleen R. Merikangas*
Affiliation:
Genetic Epidemiology Research Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
*
Corresponding author: Kathleen R. Merikangas; Email: kathleen.merikangas@nih.gov
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although heightened anxiety associated with social interaction or evaluation is the core diagnostic criterion for social anxiety disorder (SAD), there is growing evidence that SAD is characterized by more pervasive reactivity beyond social situations. We employed Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) to describe the affective dynamics and emotional reactivity to daily events in a community-based sample of adults with SAD compared with other anxiety disorders, and controls without anxiety or mood disorders.

Methods

A sample of 236 adults with a lifetime diagnosis of SAD (n = 53), other anxiety disorders (n = 120), and no mood or anxiety disorder (n = 63) based on comprehensive diagnostic interviews answered brief electronic interviews that assessed daily life events and mood and anxiety symptoms four times a day for two weeks. Linear mixed models were used to quantify reactivity to daily life events.

Results

Persons with SAD had higher average levels of sad and anxious mood than those with other anxiety disorders or controls. Irrespective of comorbid mood disorders, people with SAD also demonstrated significantly greater decreases in both sad and anxious mood following positive events, and a greater increase in anxious mood following negative, particularly non-social events.

Conclusions

Our findings regarding pervasive reactivity beyond the social context in people with SAD confirm the need for broader conceptualization of this disorder as well as expansion of interventions beyond the social context. This work also demonstrates the utility of EMA as a powerful tool to track individual variability and reactivity in daily life that can inform etiology, treatment and prevention.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is a work of the US Government and is not subject to copyright protection within the United States. Published by Cambridge University Press
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© National Institutes of Health, 2025.

Introduction

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) affects up to 12% of the population and is associated with significant personal and societal burdens (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, Cui, Vandeleur, Castelao, Zarate, Preisig and Merikangas2019; Dalrymple & Zimmerman, Reference Dalrymple and Zimmerman2007; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005). Characterized by increased sensitivity to social contexts involving the scrutiny or judgment of others, people with this disorder tend to avoid such situations or endure them with intense distress (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Heimberg et al., Reference Heimberg, Hofmann, Liebowitz, Schneier, Smits, Stein, Hinton and Craske2014). However, it has been difficult to identify specific treatment targets for SAD and its consequences because of the pervasive comorbidity of SAD with other anxiety disorder subtypes and mood disorders that have been documented in both clinical and community samples (Stein et al., Reference Stein, Lim, Roest, de Jonge, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Bruffaerts, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Harris, He, Hinkov, Horiguchi and Hu2017).

Aside from the disabling impact of social anxiety on life functioning, suicide attempts, and substance use disorders are also well-established consequences of SAD in adults (Compton, Thomas, Stinson, & Grant, Reference Compton, Thomas, Stinson and Grant2007; Cougle, Keough, Riccardi, & Sachs-Ericsson, Reference Cougle, Keough, Riccardi and Sachs-Ericsson2009; Swendsen et al., Reference Swendsen, Conway, Degenhardt, Glantz, Jin, Merikangas, Sampson and Kessler2010; Thibodeau, Welch, Sareen, & Asmundson, Reference Thibodeau, Welch, Sareen and Asmundson2013) and youth (Leigh, Chiu, & Ballard, Reference Leigh, Chiu and Ballard2023; Lemyre, Gauthier-Legare, & Belanger, Reference Lemyre, Gauthier-Legare and Belanger2019). Our earlier work on the familial aggregation and co-aggregation of suicide attempts with mood and anxiety disorders showed that SAD was associated with an elevated risk of suicide attempts in both individuals and families of people with SAD, beyond the effects of comorbid mood and substance use disorders (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, Cui, Vandeleur, Castelao, Zarate, Preisig and Merikangas2019). We speculated that while bipolar disorder, particularly comorbid with substance use disorders, was the most potent correlate of suicide attempts, SAD may reflect a proximate trigger of suicide attempts through increased reactivity to acute life stressors. Here, we examine this potential mechanism by examining patterns of daily reactivity and variability of mood and anxiety with ecological momentary assessment (EMA).

Previous studies have demonstrated that people with SAD may have broader reactivity to life events beyond those involving social appraisal. Studies using self-reported or physiologic measures have shown that people with SAD have increased reactivity to social events in general (Barker, Troller-Renfree, Pine, & Fox, Reference Barker, Troller-Renfree, Pine and Fox2015; Goldin, Manber, Hakimi, Canli, & Gross, Reference Goldin, Manber, Hakimi, Canli and Gross2009; Kagan, Reznick, & Snidman, Reference Kagan, Reznick and Snidman1987; Nelemans et al., Reference Nelemans, Hale, Branje, van Lier, Koot and Meeus2017; Roelofs, Minelli, Mars, van Peer, & Toni, Reference Roelofs, Minelli, Mars, van Peer and Toni2009; Yoon & Joormann, Reference Yoon and Joormann2012) as well as to social stressors, neutral events (Bowen, Baetz, Hawkes, & Bowen, Reference Bowen, Baetz, Hawkes and Bowen2006; Farmer & Kashdan, Reference Farmer and Kashdan2015; Kashdan & Collins, Reference Kashdan and Collins2010) and even positive events such as praise (Clark & Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Doorley et al., Reference Doorley, Goodman, Disabato, Kashdan, Weinstein and Shackman2021; Hur, Tillman, Fox, & Shackman, Reference Hur, Tillman, Fox and Shackman2019; Rapee & Heimberg, Reference Rapee and Heimberg1997). Some studies suggest that SAD may reflect broader mood alterations or vulnerabilities that extend beyond the social context (Auerbach, Richardt, Kertz, & Eberhart, Reference Auerbach, Richardt, Kertz and Eberhart2012; Kashdan & Collins, Reference Kashdan and Collins2010; Piccirillo & Rodebaugh, Reference Piccirillo and Rodebaugh2022). However, few of the earlier studies of reactivity of SAD included comprehensive diagnostic interviews of the full range of psychopathology that could characterize comorbid conditions such as mood disorders which are also associated with both event reactivity as well as general affective dynamics (Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Swendsen, Cui, Husky, Johns, Zipunnikov and Merikangas2018). Moreover, most of the evidence of reactivity in SAD has been based on either unselected or clinical samples of young adults.

The present study examines the real-time affective dynamics and emotional reactivity to daily life events in a community-ascertained sample of a broad age range of adults who were characterized by the full range of anxiety and mood disorder subtypes with comprehensive diagnostic assessments. The specific aims are to: (1) evaluate the differences, variability, and instability of sad and anxious moods in people with SAD, other anxiety and/or mood disorders, and controls without mood or anxiety disorders; (2) examine whether there is enhanced mood reactivity to both negative and positive events among people with and without SAD, and (3) determine whether there is an interaction between social versus non-social event type with mood reactivity.

Methods

Sample

A sample of 236 individuals 18 years and older participated in this study as part of the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Family Study of Affective Spectrum Disorders, a large family study based on DSM-IV-TR mood and anxiety disorders. Probands were recruited from the greater Washington DC metropolitan area and enriched for mood disorders through referrals from the National Institute of Health Clinical Center or the National Institute of Mental Health Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program. The community sample, designed to be a nontreatment or nonclinical group with and without mental health disorders, was ascertained by mail contact via a list of households within a 50-mile radius of Washington, DC. Participants who were recruited from the community could be currently in treatment or have been previously treated for their mental health problems or disorders. Inclusion criteria were the ability to speak English, availability to participate, and consent to contact at least two living relatives. All participants provided written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Combined Neuroscience IRB at the National Institutes of Health. Additional details of the family study methods are described elsewhere (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, Cui, Heaton, Nakamura, Roca, Ding, Qin, Guo, Shugart, Zarate and Angst2014).

We compared EMA-based emotional ratings between those with a lifetime SAD (n = 53), other lifetime anxiety disorders (n = 120), and no lifetime mood or anxiety disorders (n = 63). Of the participants with lifetime SAD, 42 (79%) met criteria for current SAD. Of the 53 individuals with lifetime SAD, 37 (69.8%) also had a lifetime history of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and/or panic disorder, 23 (43.4%) with specific phobia, and 46 (86.8%) with a mood disorder (Bipolar I, Bipolar II, or Major Depressive Disorder). Dysthymia was not incuded as a separate disorder because of overlap with one of the mood disorder subtypes. Individuals in the ‘other anxiety disorder’ group most frequently had GAD or panic disorder (n = 73; 60.8%), specific phobia (n = 59; 49.2%), or other anxiety disorders (separation anxiety or agoraphobia) (n = 14; 49.2%). Panic disorder and GAD were combined as only 22 subjects in the entire sample presented with lifetime panic disorder and 73% of those with panic disorder also met the criteria for lifetime GAD.

Measures

Diagnostic interview

The NIMH Family Study Diagnostic Interview for Affective Spectrum Disorders ascertains diagnostic criteria as well as subthreshold phenomenology related to current and lifetime disorders from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). The interview was developed based on earlier diagnostic interviews for genetic epidemiologic studies such as the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia and the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies, and does not adhere to skip-outs based on frequency or duration at the probe level. Experienced clinicians conducted direct interviews of probands and direct, blinded interviews of relatives. Inter-rater reliability of all diagnostic categories was high, with intraclass correlations of 0.87 and above across all diagnostic categories. Best estimate DSM-IV diagnoses were based on all available information, including direct interviews, family history, and consensus ratings from experienced clinicians. Family members of the proband were interviewed about their own mental health as well as that of their relatives. More detailed information concerning the semi-structured interview and family study methods can be found in an earlier publication of this work (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, Cui, Heaton, Nakamura, Roca, Ding, Qin, Guo, Shugart, Zarate and Angst2014).

EMA procedures

Participants completed brief electronic questionnaires four times a day over a 2-week period using a Personal Digital Assistant (Tungsten E2 PDA). Fixed assessment times were used for each participant with an average delay of 4 hours between assessments, and signaling schedules varied across participants. Participants completed EMA within the 12 months following the clinical interview (and the majority within the first 2 months following the clinical assessments) and were trained and monitored by research assistants on administrative procedures. The NIMH EMA was subsequently administered on research mobile devices and more recently is available through an open-source platform (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Clucas, Krishnakumar, Ghosh, Van Auken, Thonet, Sabram, Acuna, Keshavan, Rossiter, Xiao, Semenuta, Badioli, Konishcheva, Abraham, Alexander, Merikangas, Swendsen, Lindner and Milham2021).

The EMA variables examined in the present study included mood states based on the mood circumplex model (Larsen & Diener, Reference Larsen and Diener1992) and daily events based on the inventory of small life events that was adapted for use in EMA (Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Swendsen, Cui, Husky, Johns, Zipunnikov and Merikangas2018; Zautra, Guarnaccia, & Dohrenwend, Reference Zautra, Guarnaccia and Dohrenwend1986). The circumplex model of affect describes emotional experiences using orthogonal dimensions reflecting valence (pleasant to unpleasant) and activation (high to low activation) and their combination (Feldman-Barrett & Russell, Reference Feldman-Barrett and Russell1998; Larsen & Diener, Reference Larsen and Diener1992; Posner, Reference Posner and Peterson2005; Russell, Reference Russell1980; Yik, Reference Yik and Barrett1999). Larsen and Diener’s (Larsen & Diener, Reference Larsen and Diener1992) mood circumplex model further proposes four bipolar scales reflecting the full circumplex structure of affective states. Participants were asked to rate their anxious mood at the time of the EMA assessment on a Likert scale from (1) very calm to (7) very anxious, and their sad mood from (1) very happy to (7) very sad. Other mood states were also assessed through EMA but were not examined in the present study. For daily events, participants were first asked to identify the event that affected them the most since the previous assessment, and then to rate the impact of the event on a Likert scale from (1) extremely positive to (7) extremely negative. This impact variable was then categorized into positive events (a score of 1–3), neutral events (score of 4), and negative events (scores of 5–7). In addition, participants were asked to classify the type of event they had experienced with the option of choosing any of the following: education, family or friends relationships, interactions with colleagues, interactions with strangers, housing or residence, leisure, health, finances, religion or spirituality, legal or judicial, traveling or commuting, after school activities, interactions with students, or other events. Participants were instructed to identify one category (out of a list of 14 mutually exclusive types of events) that reflected the nature of the event they identified at each assessment. They were provided examples during training to elicit unpleasant events at work as either ‘interaction with colleagues’ or as a ‘work’ event if it was not specifically related to interpersonal relationships. These event types were then categorized according to their social (family and friends relationships, or interactions with colleagues, students, or strangers) or non-social nature (all other events).

Statistical analyses

Affective dynamic analyses were conducted in SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) using the Generalized Least Squares (GLS) method and Restricted Maximum Likelihood (REML) to estimate the fixed effects and the covariance parameters, respectively. The outcome variables used in the models were mean analog mood rating level, mood variability, and instability for the spectrums of both anxious and sad mood states (resulting in six models). These effects were modeled with the fixed effect of anxiety disorder lifetime status and the reference group included non-SAD anxiety disorders (Other Anxiety), and Controls without lifetime mood and anxiety disorders. The models adjusted for the presence of lifetime-specific phobia, GAD, or panic disorder to investigate the specificity of the findings regarding SAD versus other anxiety disorder subtypes. Mood variability was based on the within-day average standard deviation (SD) of assessments, and instability was defined as the average within-day mean squared successive difference (MSSD) and was transformed using ln (MSSD + 1) to a normal distribution. The following covariates were used in the affective dynamic models: age, sex, lifetime anxiety or mood disorder, and Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) score.

For all analyses on event reactivity SPSS version 22 and linear mixed models, with random intercepts and a repeated effect with an AR (1) correlation structure was used to account for multiple observations per person. This method allows us to fully harness the power of repeated measures in our data. Compliance was high for the sample with participants completing an average of 44 (SD 9.9; 77.8%) of the 56 programmed assessments. All data were used with no restrictions on a minimum number of EMA assessments completed due to high compliance missing assessments were not imputed. Reactivity to positive and negative events was examined through two models with the outcome variable of mood state (sad or anxious) and fixed effects of lifetime diagnosis status, event valence (positive or negative events, with neutral events as the reference group), interaction of SAD and event type, and with adjustment for age, sex, comorbid mood disorder, GAF score, and mood rating at the previous assessment. The analyses for reactivity to positive and negative events were conducted separately for social events and non-social events and had the following fixed effects of anxiety diagnoses, event valence (positive, negative, neutral as the reference group), mood rating at the previous assessment, and the interactions of both SAD and event valence. For more information concerning multilevel modeling (see Twisk, Reference Twisk2006).

Results

Table 1 presents the sociodemographic clinical characteristics of the three study groups included in the sample The sample included 236 participants, 62.7% of whom were female with an average age of 48.2 years (SD = 16.2, range = 18–84). The sex distribution of individuals with SAD did not differ from those with other anxiety disorders and controls. However, controls were older than the case groups and there were fewer female controls (41.3%) than cases of SAD (64.2%) or other anxiety disorders (74.2%). Participants with SAD were more likely to have a current diagnosis compared with those with other anxiety disorders and they had significantly lower GAF scores than the combined reference group (t = −7.935, p < .001), the other anxiety disorder (t = −5.144, p < .001) and the control groups (t = −13.363, p < .001). The other anxiety group had significantly lower GAF scores than the control group (t = 11.07, p < .001).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of sample by lifetime anxiety and mood disorders

Note: The reference group includes participants who have an anxiety disorder but do not have SAD and controls who do not meet the criteria for a mental disorder. The current disorder is defined as any mood and anxiety disorder in the prior year of the date of the interview. SAD: Social Anxiety Disorder. GAD: Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Mood disorder is a lifetime diagnosis of Bipolar I, Bipolar II, or Major Depressive Disorder. GAF: Global Assessment of Functioning. Kruskal–Wallis Test (KW).

Participants completed an average of 44 (SD = 9.9; 77.8%) of the 56 programmed assessments. People with SAD were less compliant than controls in completing an average of 41 assessments (SD = 12.5; 74%), t = 2.042, p < .05. There was an average of 8.3 neutral events (SD = 7.4), 27.6 positive events (SD = 12.2), and 7.7 negative events (SD = 6.9) across the assessment period. Of all reported events, there were approximately equal proportions of events that were described as having a social (M = 20, SD = 10.6) or non-social nature (M = 23.3, SD = 10.6). There were no significant differences in the frequency of neutral events, or type of event, between individuals with SAD compared with controls. However, people with SAD reported significantly fewer positive events (t = 4.931, p < .001) and significantly more negative events (t = 3.977, p < .001) than the combined reference group. In addition, people with SAD reported significantly fewer positive events than the other anxiety disorder group and controls, (F = 13.239, p < .001), while there was no difference between controls and the other anxiety disorder group. Likewise, people with SAD reported significantly more negative events than both the other anxiety group and controls, and the other anxiety group had significantly more negative events than controls (h [Kruskal–Wallis] = 26.949, p < .001).

Intensity, variability, and instability of sad and anxious moods

There was a moderate level of variability of both sad mood (SAD group M = 1.1, SD = 0.3; other anxiety disorder group M = 2.8, SD = 0.9; and control group M = 2.4, SD = 0.8), and anxious mood (SAD group M = 3.2, SD = 1; other anxiety disorder group M = 2.5, SD =0.9; and control group M = 2, SD = 0.7) across the 2-week period. The affective dynamics linear mixed models by lifetime disorder status are presented in Table 2. After adjustment for mood disorders, only individuals with lifetime SAD had significantly greater sad mood (coef = 0.526, p < .001). Likewise, the presence of lifetime SAD was significantly associated with increased anxious mood (coef = 0.381, p < .05), as was the presence of lifetime GAD/Panic disorder (coef = 0.369, p = 0.009). The findings of greater sad (coef = 0.482, p < .01) and anxious (coef = 0.350, p < .05) mood remained significant when including only individuals with a current SAD diagnosis. Furthermore, there were no differences in the variability or instability of sad and anxious mood among those with current SAD. However, those with GAD/Panic disorder also had significantly greater instability in anxious mood (coef = 0.128, p = 0.04).

Table 2. Within-day means, variability, and instability of sad and anxious mood in individuals by lifetime anxiety disorder status

Note: All disorders are lifetime diagnoses. Mood disorder includes either Bipolar I, Bipolar II, or Major Depressive Disorder. SAD: Social Anxiety Disorder; GAF: Gobal Assessment of Functioning; †MSSD = mean square successive difference had the following transformation: ln(mssd+1), SD = standard deviation. Linear mixed model adjusted for age, sex, and comorbid mood disorders. Outcome-based on sad and anxious mood rating at each assessment (d, t). Confidence intervals are 95%. Significant findings are marked in bold.

Mood reactivity to positive and negative events

Table 3 shows the associations between mood reactivity and the valence of life events by the presence or absence of lifetime disorders. There were significant main effects for the associations between both positive and negative events, with decreases in sad and anxious moods following a positive event and increases following a negative event. There were statistically significant interactions for positive events for sad mood and both positive and negative events for anxious mood among those with SAD. These interactive influences reflected a greater decrease in both sad mood (slope = −0.687) and anxious mood after positive events (slope = −0.608) for those with SAD compared with controls, and a greater increase in anxious mood following negative events (slope = 0.769).

Table 3. Reactivity of ratings of sad and anxious mood following daily events by lifetime disorder status

Note: All disorders are lifetime diagnoses. Mood disorder is a lifetime diagnosis of Bipolar I, Bipolar II, or Major Depressive Disorder.SAD: Social Anxiety Disorder. GAF: Global Assessment of Functioning. Linear mixed model corrected for age, sex, GAF score, and mood at previous assessment (d, t−1). Significant findings are indicated in bold. Interaction with events only for SAD, other comorbid disorders are only corrected for as main effects. Outcome-based on sad and anxious mood rating at each assessment (d, t) and confidence interval are 95%.

Mood reactivity to social and non-social events

We also evaluated the association between SAD with mood reactivity by the social significance of the event. Significant interactions were observed between event type and SAD. Individuals with lifetime SAD experienced greater decreases in sad (coef = −0.322, p < .001) and anxious mood (coef = −0.315, p < .01) following positive events, and a greater increase in anxious mood (coef = 0.353, p < .05) following negative events. However, these effects were observable only for non-social events. These differences remained significant when including only those individuals with current SAD.

Discussion

Using real-time mobile tracking of emotions and events in a comprehensively characterized community-based sample, we found that SAD may be characterized by a broader range of emotional dysregulation than is recognized by current nosology. Specifically, patterns of affective dynamics and emotional reactivity to both positive and negative events in individuals with this disorder differed from those with other anxiety disorder subtypes and controls without a history of mood or anxiety disorders and persisted after controlling for a lifetime mood disorder. The findings indicate that SAD may be characterized by enhanced sensitivity and reactivity to daily perturbations beyond those of a social nature. The persistence of SAD across the lifespan as demonstrated by similar patterns of reactivity among those with either current or lifetime SAD also highlights the enduring nature of the temperamental sensitivity of this condition that distinguishes it from the more episodic manifestations of other anxiety disorders such as panic disorder and major depression. More broadly, this work demonstrates the utility of the application of real-time mobile technology to track emotional states in daily life that may enhance our ability to characterize the dynamics of SAD and define targets for interventions, that could ultimately inform suicide prevention (Farmer & Kashdan, Reference Farmer and Kashdan2015; Hur et al., Reference Hur, DeYoung, Islam, Anderson, Barstead and Shackman2020; Rashid, Chakraborty, & Fraser, Reference Rashid, Chakraborty and Fraser2020).

The greater average levels of both sad and anxious mood among those with SAD compared with those with other anxiety disorders and controls highlights its distinction from other anxiety disorders and further emphasizes the broad range of emotional manifestations of SAD (Farmer & Kashdan, Reference Farmer and Kashdan2015; Piccirillo & Rodebaugh, Reference Piccirillo and Rodebaugh2022). The persistence of these differences after adjustment for comorbid mood disorders suggests that elevated emotional arousal may comprise a core phenomenological trait in SAD (Rapee & Heimberg, Reference Rapee and Heimberg1997).

Our finding of significantly greater decreases in both sad and anxious moods following positive events in those with SAD, and greater increase in anxious mood following negative events regardless of event type further supports the expansion of definitions of SAD to reflect its broader emotional reactivity beyond the traditional conceptualization of greater sensitivity to social interactions (Clark & Wells, Reference Clark, Wells, Heimberg, Liebowitz, Hope and Schneier1995; Doorley et al., Reference Doorley, Goodman, Disabato, Kashdan, Weinstein and Shackman2021; Dryman & Heimberg, Reference Dryman and Heimberg2018; Hur, Stockbridge, Fox, & Shackman, Reference Hur, Stockbridge, Fox, Shackman and Srinivasan2019). Moreover, this increased reactivity of people with SAD was not attributable to comorbid mood disorders as shown by earlier EMA studies (Farmer & Kashdan, Reference Farmer and Kashdan2015; Piccirillo & Rodebaugh, Reference Piccirillo and Rodebaugh2022). These findings argue for a broader conceptualization of SAD that distinguishes this subgroup both from other anxiety disorder subtypes as well as from mood disorders.

The lack of a significant increase in anxiety following social events among those with SAD was unexpected, considering that increased anxiety in social situations is a core feature of this disorder. Potential explanations for this negative finding could be that there was overlap in the context, valence, impact, and social nature of the events that may have obscured these distinctions, or methodologic in which our instructions for EMA may have overemphasized the negative versus positive impact over-classification of the nature of the event. Furthermore, we did not collect detailed information on the social context in terms of familial versus unfamiliar people present at the time of the ratings so could not test this potential effect.

The present results have implications for understanding the course of SAD, treatment, and prevention of its consequences. The broad mood reactivity observed in these individuals may contribute to the lifetime persistence of this condition that distinguishes it from both mood and other anxiety disorders that tend to fluctuate across the lifespan (Angst, Gamma, Baldwin, Ajdacic-Gross, & Rössler, Reference Angst, Gamma, Baldwin, Ajdacic-Gross and Rössler2009; Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Yonkers, Otto, Eisen, Weisberg, Pagano, Shea and Keller2005). Sad and anxious moods, albeit elevated, tended to be more stable in people with SAD because they did not exhibit the greater variability and instability of emotions that characterize people with mood disorders (Koval, Pe, Meers, & Kuppens, Reference Koval, Pe, Meers and Kuppens2013; Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Swendsen, Cui, Husky, Johns, Zipunnikov and Merikangas2018; Schwartz, Schultz, Reider, & Saunders, Reference Schwartz, Schultz, Reider and Saunders2016). Moreover, the salience of mood symptoms as well as enhanced reactivity to daily events may play an important role in the increased rates of suicide attempts in people with SAD, particularly when comorbid with bipolar disorder (Ballard et al., Reference Ballard, Cui, Vandeleur, Castelao, Zarate, Preisig and Merikangas2019). Mechanistic studies that investigate the role of underlying traits such as behavioral inhibition (Clauss & Blackford, Reference Clauss and Blackford2012), anxiety sensitivity (Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Boffa, Rogers, Hom, Albanese, Chu, Capron, Schmidt and Joiner2018), and physiologic reactivity (Venables et al., Reference Venables, Yancey, Kramer, Hicks, Krueger, Iacono, Joiner and Patrick2018) in the link between interpersonal sensitivity and suicidal ideation and attempts (Buckner, Lemke, Jeffries, & Shah, Reference Buckner, Lemke, Jeffries and Shah2017) are needed. Such reactivity should be particularly considered in the context of mood and substance use disorders that could facilitate the translation of these findings to prevention and interventions. Therefore, these broader manifestations of SAD should be incorporated into program content and measures of outcome in both adults and youth (Butler, O’Day, Swee, Horenstein, & Heimberg, Reference Butler, O’Day, Swee, Horenstein and Heimberg2021; Leigh & Clark, Reference Leigh and Clark2018).

The strengths of the present study include its use of a large community-based sample, the wide age range and inclusion of both male and female participants, the application of comprehensive diagnostic assessments that include the full range of mood and anxiety disorder subtypes and substance use disorders, and the balance between those with current versus lifetime disorders. Most prior EMA studies of SAD were either based on laboratory studies or convenience samples without a comprehensive diagnostic assessment. Adjustment of statistical models for comorbid mood disorders provided a more conservative investigation of the specificity of alterations in affective dynamics and event sensitivity in SAD. In addition, the control for emotional states before event occurrence permitted a highly precise examination of the direction of associations among mood states and daily events.

However, the present findings should also be interpreted relative to specific limitations or characteristics of the methods. First, the analyses assessed within-day associations among emotions and events over a 2-week period. While these timeframes correspond to the natural trajectories of mood changes following daily events (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Husky, Grondin, Mazure, Doron and Swendsen2008), analyses over longer timeframes may produce different results. Second, as described above, the lack of specificity of reactivity to social events may in part be the misclassification of distinctions between social and non-social events in the EMA ratings. Third, participants were asked to rate the positive or negative ‘impact’ of each event, reflecting both the level of disruption caused by the event as well as its valence. This may have confounded valence and impact. However, similar methods have been used in prior EMA studies assessing individuals’ experience of daily life stressors (Husky et al., Reference Husky, Mazure, Maciejewski and Swendsen2009; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Husky, Grondin, Mazure, Doron and Swendsen2008; Khazanov et al., Reference Khazanov, Ruscio and Swendsen2019; Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Swendsen, Cui, Husky, Johns, Zipunnikov and Merikangas2018). Fourth, our findings are focused on an adult sample, and therefore may not apply to SAD in children and adolescents. Although our sample is older than prior clinical samples, we observed similar results for reactivity to negative events as those of previous studies (Farmer & Kashdan, Reference Farmer and Kashdan2015). Moreover, although the findings are based on lifetime SAD, there is abundant evidence that the core feature of social sensitivity tends to persist across the lifetime irrespective of fluctuations in the salience of its associated features and impairment (Bruce et al., Reference Bruce, Yonkers, Otto, Eisen, Weisberg, Pagano, Shea and Keller2005; Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & Wittchen, Reference Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Wittchen2012). One important topic for future research is whether SAD may reflect a lack of maturation of either physiologic or cognitive reactivity to environmental events that could be a target for developmentally sensitive interventions (Richey et al., Reference Richey, Brewer, Sullivan-Toole, Strege, Kim-Spoon, White and Ollendick2019). Additionally, future studies should include greater specificity for characterizing events as social or non-social to capture more nuanced reactivity patterns to different social contexts.

Conclusion

People with SAD from a non-clinical community-based sample had elevated levels of both sad and anxious mood, and enhanced reactivity to both positive and negative daily events, regardless of the social nature of the event. This increase in general reactivity that tends to persist across the lifetime suggests that expansion of interventions for SAD beyond the social context may be more effective in reducing its negative life consequences. Findings support the application of tools to track daily fluctuations among those with enhanced vulnerability to suicide as a preventive intervention. More broadly, this work further demonstrates the utility of EMA as a powerful tool to track individual variability and reactivity in daily life that can inform etiology, treatment, and prevention.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725000121.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health through grant ZIAMH002804 and the National Institute of Health postbaccalaureate Intramural Research Training Award (Ms. Foote). We express our sincere gratitude for the central contribution of Professor Joel Swendsen from the Laboratory of Psychology at the University of Bordeaux, France, to this work and our research program and for his leadership role in the field of Ecological Assessment in Neuropsychiatry. Dr. Swendsen died unexpectedly on July 14, 2022.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health (ZIA MH002804 and ZIA MH002954). The views and opinions expressed in this article are those of the authors and should not be construed to represent the views of any of the sponsoring organizations, agencies, or U.S. Government. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standard

The research was conducted under clinical protocol 03-M-0211 (NCT00071786). The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub.Google Scholar
Angst, J., Gamma, A., Baldwin, D. S., Ajdacic-Gross, V., & Rössler, W. (2009). The generalized anxiety spectrum: Prevalence, onset, course and outcome. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 259(1), 3745. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-008-0832-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auerbach, R. P., Richardt, S., Kertz, S., & Eberhart, N. K. (2012). Cognitive vulnerability, stress generation, and anxiety: Symptom clusters and gender differences. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 5(1), 5066. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2012.5.1.50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, E. D., Cui, L., Vandeleur, C., Castelao, E., Zarate, C. A. Jr., Preisig, M., & Merikangas, K. R. (2019). Familial aggregation and coaggregation of suicide attempts and comorbid mental disorders in adults. JAMA Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, T. V., Troller-Renfree, S., Pine, D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2015). Individual differences in social anxiety affect the salience of errors in social contexts. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15(4), 723735. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0360-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowen, R., Baetz, M., Hawkes, J., & Bowen, A. (2006). Mood variability in anxiety disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 91(2–3), 165170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, S., Yonkers, K., Otto, M., Eisen, J., Weisberg, R., Pagano, M., Shea, M., & Keller, M. (2005). Influence of psychiatric comorbidity on recovery and recurrence in generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, and panic disorder: A 12-year prospective study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(6), 11791187. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, J. D., Lemke, A. W., Jeffries, E. R., & Shah, S. M. (2017). Social anxiety and suicidal ideation: Test of the utility of the interpersonal-psychological theory of suicide. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 45, 6063. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.11.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, R. M., O’Day, E. B., Swee, M. B., Horenstein, A., & Heimberg, R. G. (2021). Cognitive behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder: Predictors of treatment outcome in a quasi-naturalistic setting. Behavior Therapy, 52(2), 465477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2020.06.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In Heimberg, R. G., Liebowitz, M.R., Hope, D. A., & Schneier, F. R. (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 6993). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Clauss, J. A., & Blackford, J. U. (2012). Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10), 10661075.e1061. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compton, W. M., Thomas, Y. F., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2007). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Archives Of General Psychiatry, 64(5), 566576. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.566CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cougle, J. R., Keough, M. E., Riccardi, C. J., & Sachs-Ericsson, N. (2009). Anxiety disorders and suicidality in the National Comorbidity Survey-Replication. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(9), 825829. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.12.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalrymple, K. L., & Zimmerman, M. (2007). Does comorbid social anxiety disorder impact the clinical presentation of principal major depressive disorder? Journal of Affective Disorders, 100(1–3), 241247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2006.10.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doorley, J. D., Goodman, F. R., Disabato, D. J., Kashdan, T. B., Weinstein, J. S., & Shackman, A. J. (2021). The momentary benefits of positive events for individuals with elevated social anxiety. Emotion, 21, 595606. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000725CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dryman, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2018). Emotion regulation in social anxiety and depression: A systematic review of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 1742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.07.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, A. S., & Kashdan, T. B. (2015). Stress sensitivity and stress generation in social anxiety disorder: A temporal process approach. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 102114. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman-Barrett, L., & Russell, J. (1998). Independence and Bipolarity in the Structure of Current Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(4), 967984. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.967CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin, P. R., Manber, T., Hakimi, S., Canli, T., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: Emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation during social and physical threat. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(2), 170180. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.525CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heimberg, R. G., Hofmann, S. G., Liebowitz, M. R., Schneier, F. R., Smits, J. A., Stein, M. B., Hinton, D. E., & Craske, M. G. (2014). Social anxiety disorder in DSM-5. Depress Anxiety, 31(6), 472479. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hur, J., DeYoung, K. A., Islam, S., Anderson, A. S., Barstead, M. G., & Shackman, A. J. (2020). Social context and the real-world consequences of social anxiety. Psychological Medicine, 50(12), 19892000. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hur, J., Stockbridge, M. D., Fox, A. S., & Shackman, A. J. (2019). Chapter 16 - Dispositional negativity, cognition, and anxiety disorders: An integrative translational neuroscience framework. In Srinivasan, N. (Ed.), Progress in brain research (Vol. 247, pp. 375436). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.03.012Google Scholar
Hur, J., Tillman, R. M., Fox, A. S., & Shackman, A. J. (2019). The value of clinical and translational neuroscience approaches to psychiatric illness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, e11, Article e11. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X18001036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Husky, M. M., Mazure, C. M., Maciejewski, P. K., & Swendsen, J. D. (2009). Past depression and gender interact to influence emotional reactivity to daily life stress. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33(3), 261271. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-008-9212-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E., Husky, M., Grondin, O., Mazure, C., Doron, J., & Swendsen, J. (2008). Mood trajectories following daily life events. Motivation and Emotion, 32(4), 251259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9106-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58(6), 14591473. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3691195 10.2307/1130685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashdan, T. B., & Collins, R. L. (2010). Social anxiety and the experience of positive emotion and anger in everyday life: An ecological momentary assessment approach. Anxiety Stress Coping, 23(3), 259272. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615800802641950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khazanov, G. K., Ruscio, A. M., & Swendsen, J. (2019). The “Brightening” Effect: Reactions to Positive Events in the Daily Lives of Individuals With Major Depressive Disorder and Generalized Anxiety Disorder. Behavior Therapy, 50(2), 270284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2018.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Wittchen, H. U. (2012). Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3), 169184. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, A., Clucas, J., Krishnakumar, A., Ghosh, S. S., Van Auken, W., Thonet, B., Sabram, I., Acuna, N., Keshavan, A., Rossiter, H., Xiao, Y., Semenuta, S., Badioli, A., Konishcheva, K., Abraham, S. A., Alexander, L. M., Merikangas, K. R., Swendsen, J., Lindner, A. B., & Milham, M. P. (2021). Remote digital psychiatry for mobile mental health assessment and therapy: MindLogger platform development study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 23(11), e22369. https://doi.org/10.2196/22369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koval, P., Pe, M. L., Meers, K., & Kuppens, P. (2013). Affect dynamics in relation to depressive symptoms: Variable, unstable or inert? Emotion, 13(6), 11321141. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033579CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamers, F., Swendsen, J., Cui, L., Husky, M., Johns, J., Zipunnikov, V., & Merikangas, K. R. (2018). Mood reactivity and affective dynamics in mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(7), 659669. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. In Emotion (pp. 2559). Sage Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Leigh, E., Chiu, K., & Ballard, E. D. (2023). Social anxiety and suicidality in youth: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 51(4), 441454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-022-00996-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leigh, E., & Clark, D. M. (2018). Understanding social anxiety disorder in adolescents and improving treatment outcomes: Applying the cognitive model of clark and wells (1995). Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 21(3), 388414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-018-0258-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemyre, A., Gauthier-Legare, A., & Belanger, R. E. (2019). Shyness, social anxiety, social anxiety disorder, and substance use among normative adolescent populations: A systematic review. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 45(3), 230247. https://doi.org/10.1080/00952990.2018.1536882CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikangas, K. R., Cui, L., Heaton, L., Nakamura, E., Roca, C., Ding, J., Qin, H., Guo, W., Shugart, Y. Y., Zarate, C., & Angst, J. (2014). Independence of familial transmission of mania and depression: Results of the NIMH family study of affective spectrum disorders. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(2), 214219. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2013.116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelemans, S. A., Hale, W. W. III, Branje, S. J. T., van Lier, P. A. C., Koot, H. M., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2017). The role of stress reactivity in the long-term persistence of adolescent social anxiety symptoms. Biological Psychology, 125, 91104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piccirillo, M. L., & Rodebaugh, T. L. (2022). Personalized networks of social anxiety disorder and depression and implications for treatment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 298, 262276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.10.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, J. R., JA; Peterson, B. S (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 715734. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(8), 741756. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00022-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rashid, T. A., Chakraborty, C., & Fraser, K. (2020). Advances in telemedicine for health monitoring: Technologies, design and applications. Institution of Engineering and Technology.10.1049/PBHE023ECrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richey, J. A., Brewer, J. A., Sullivan-Toole, H., Strege, M. V., Kim-Spoon, J., White, S. W., & Ollendick, T. H. (2019). Sensitivity shift theory: A developmental model of positive affect and motivational deficits in social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 72, 101756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101756CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roelofs, K., Minelli, A., Mars, R. B., van Peer, J., & Toni, I. (2009). On the neural control of social emotional behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 5058. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. (1980). A Circumplex Model of Affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 11611178. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S., Schultz, S., Reider, A., & Saunders, E. F. H. (2016). Daily mood monitoring of symptoms using smartphones in bipolar disorder: A pilot study assessing the feasibility of ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 8893. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, I. H., Boffa, J. W., Rogers, M. L., Hom, M. A., Albanese, B. J., Chu, C., Capron, D. W., Schmidt, N. B., & Joiner, T. E. (2018). Anxiety sensitivity and suicidal ideation/suicide risk: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86, 946960. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, D. J., Lim, C. C. W., Roest, A. M., de Jonge, P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., Bruffaerts, R., de Girolamo, G., Florescu, S., Gureje, O., Haro, J. M., Harris, M. G., He, Y., Hinkov, H., Horiguchi, I., Hu, C., … WHO World Mental Health Survey Collaborators (2017). The cross-national epidemiology of social anxiety disorder: Data from the World Mental Health Survey Initiative. BMC Medicine, 15(1), 143. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-017-0889-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swendsen, J., Conway, K. P., Degenhardt, L., Glantz, M., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., Sampson, N., & Kessler, R. C. (2010). Mental disorders as risk factors for substance use, abuse and dependence: Results from the 10-year follow-up of the National Comorbidity Survey. Addiction, 105(6), 11171128. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.02902.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thibodeau, M. A., Welch, P. G., Sareen, J., & Asmundson, G. J. (2013). Anxiety disorders are independently associated with suicide ideation and attempts: Propensity score matching in two epidemiological samples. Depress Anxiety, 30(10), 947954. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twisk, J. (2006). Applied multilevel analysis: A practical guide. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511610806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, N. C., Yancey, J. R., Kramer, M. D., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., Joiner, T. E., & Patrick, C. J. (2018). Psychoneurometric assessment of dispositional liabilities for suicidal behavior: Phenotypic and etiological associations. Psychological Medicine, 48(3), 463472. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001830CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yik, M. R., JA; Barrett, LF (1999). Structure of Self-Reported Current Affect: Integration and Beyond. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 600619. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, K. L., & Joormann, J. (2012). Stress reactivity in social anxiety disorder with and without comorbid depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 250255. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zautra, A. J., Guarnaccia, C. A., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (1986). Measuring small life events. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14(6), 629655. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00931340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of sample by lifetime anxiety and mood disorders

Figure 1

Table 2. Within-day means, variability, and instability of sad and anxious mood in individuals by lifetime anxiety disorder status

Figure 2

Table 3. Reactivity of ratings of sad and anxious mood following daily events by lifetime disorder status

Supplementary material: File

Foote et al. supplementary material

Foote et al. supplementary material
Download Foote et al. supplementary material(File)
File 21.8 KB