Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-m259h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-12T11:28:15.768Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Autistic traits modulate neural and behavioral responses to social vs nonsocial rewards

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 September 2025

Anthony Haffey*
Affiliation:
Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Whiteknights Campus, Reading RG6 6AL, UK
Chun-Ting Hsu*
Affiliation:
Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Whiteknights Campus, Reading RG6 6AL, UK
Bhismadev Chakrabarti
Affiliation:
Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Whiteknights Campus, Reading RG6 6AL, UK Department of Psychology, Ashoka University, Sonipat, India India Autism Center, Kolkata, India
*
Corresponding author: Anthony Haffey or Chun-Ting Hsu; Emails: a.haffey@reading.ac.uk, hsuchunting@gmail.com
Corresponding author: Anthony Haffey or Chun-Ting Hsu; Emails: a.haffey@reading.ac.uk, hsuchunting@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Social rewards (e.g. smiles) powerfully shape human behavior, starting from early childhood. Yet, the neural architecture that enables differential processing of social and nonsocial rewards remains largely unknown. Few previous studies that directly compared social vs nonsocial stimuli have used stimuli that have low ecological validity or are not matched on low-level stimulus parameters – limiting the scope of inference. To address this gap in knowledge, social and nonsocial reward images taken from the real world were matched on valence, arousal, and key low-level stimulus properties and presented to 37 adults in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. Individual self-reported preference for social images was associated with the functional connectivity between the left anterior insula (LAI) and medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), as well as that between the left Fusiform Gyrus (LFG) and the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC). Autistic traits negatively modulated LAI – mOFC connectivity and LFG – ACC connectivity. Reduced functional connectivity between these regions may contribute to the lower social reward responsivity in individuals with high autistic traits, as also noted from their lower valence ratings to social rewards. This study provides evidence for a new experimental paradigm to test social reward processing at a behavioral and neural level, which can contribute to potential transdiagnostic biomarkers for social cognitive processes.

Information

Type
Empirical Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Social rewards such as smiles are crucial components of social interactions, act as powerful reinforcers, and help shape human behavior and create rapport. Studies in young infants have shown how most children prefer to orient to a face/voice compared to comparable nonsocial stimuli (Morton & Johnson, 1991; Vouloumanos et al., Reference Vouloumanos, Hauser, Werker and Martin2010). Studies in adults have shown a similar preference for social over nonsocial rewards (Chakrabarti et al., Reference Chakrabarti, Haffey, Canzano, Taylor and McSorley2017; Fletcher-Watson et al., Reference Fletcher-Watson, Findlay, Leekam and Benson2008). The preference for social over nonsocial rewards has been tested extensively using behavioural and eye-tracking paradigms (Hedger et al., Reference Hedger, Dubey and Chakrabarti2020; Frazier et al., Reference Frazier, Strauss, Klingemier, Zetzer, Hardan, Eng and Youngstrom2017). In contrast, comparatively few studies have directly investigated the neural processing of social vs nonsocial rewards.

The reward system is one of the most-studied networks of brain regions. Insights from electrophysiological studies in rodent and non-human primates as well as from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in humans have revealed the importance of a few key nodes in this network, that include the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), ventral striatum (VS), and amygdala (Haber & Knutson, Reference Haber and Knutson2010). There are fiber tracts between the VS and the mOFC (Haber et al., Reference Haber, Kim, Mailly and Calzavara2006; Lehéricy et al., Reference Lehéricy, Ducros, Van De Moortele, Francois, Thivard, Poupon, Swindale, Ugurbil and Kim2004). The VS also receives input from the amygdala (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Aggleton and Saunders2002; Fudge et al., Reference Fudge, Kunishio, Walsh, Richard and Haber2002; Russchen et al., Reference Russchen, Bakst, Amaral and Price1985) in primates, which plays a key role in tagging salience of emotional (Anderson & Phelps, Reference Anderson and Phelps2001) and rewarding stimuli (Mahler & Berridge, Reference Mahler and Berridge2009). The majority of studies on the neuroanatomy of reward processing comes from the processing of nonsocial rewards such as food, chocolate, or money.

Studies that have explored social reward processing have found social rewards to activate a similar set of regions to those described earlier (Fareri, Chang, & Delgado, Reference Fareri, Chang and Delgado2012; Fareri, Chang, & Delgado, Reference Fareri, Chang and Delgado2015; Izuma et al., Reference Izuma, Saito and Sadato2008; Lin, Adolphs, et al., Reference Lin, Adolphs and Rangel2012; Wake & Izuma, Reference Wake and Izuma2017; Zink et al., Reference Zink, Tong, Chen, Bassett, Stein and Meyer-Lindenberg2008). The VS, mOFC, and amygdala have been shown to respond to social rewards, including smiles (Scott-Van Zeeland et al., Reference Scott-Van Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack and Bookheimer2010), positive feedback (Izuma et al., Reference Izuma, Saito and Sadato2008), cooperation (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Völlm, Drury, McKie, Richardson and William Deakin2006), social status (Zink et al., Reference Zink, Tong, Chen, Bassett, Stein and Meyer-Lindenberg2008), charitable giving (Kuss et al., Reference Kuss, Falk, Trautner, Elger, Weber and Fliessbach2013), and interacting with friends (Güroğlu et al., Reference Güroğlu, Haselager, van Lieshout, Takashima, Rijpkema and Fernández2008). In an early study, mOFC activity in response to monetary reward was increased by co-operation to win this money (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Völlm, Drury, McKie, Richardson and William Deakin2006), suggesting that this activity could constitute a common neural currency for both social and nonsocial rewards (Grabenhorst et al., Reference Grabenhorst, D’Souza, Parris, Rolls and Passingham2010). More recently, the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) has been shown to modulate social value processing in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Strombach et al. Reference Strombach, Weber, Hangebrauk, Kenning, Karipidis, Tobler and Kalenscher2015). In addition, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) responds to rewards and punishment for conspecifics, supporting its role in social reward processing (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Sciarillo, Nudelman, Cheer and Roesch2020).

Most studies mentioned above presented social rewards in isolation, which do not allow for a direct comparison of social and nonsocial reward processing within the same experimental paradigm (but see Rademacher et al., Reference Rademacher, Salama, Gründer and Spreckelmeyer2014; Scott-Van Zeeland et al., Reference Scott-Van Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack and Bookheimer2010 for notable exceptions). In recent years, a class of paradigms comparing social vs nonsocial rewards directly involves the use of the monetary incentive delay and the social incentive delay task. (Gu et al., Reference Gu, Huang, Camilleri, Xu, Wei, Eickhoff and Feng2019). This paradigm requires participants to make and remember associations between shapes/ symbols with reward outcomes and primarily indexes the anticipatory phase of reward processing. In contrast, paradigms directly presenting images of social and nonsocial rewards are likely to primarily index the consummatory phase of reward processing. In the minority of studies that formally compared responses to images of social vs nonsocial rewards, the stimuli either have low ecological validity (e.g. point-light figures) or are not matched for their low-level stimulus properties – which poses challenges for interpretation (Saygin et al., Reference Saygin, Wilson, Hagler, Bates and Sereno2004; Sasson et al., Reference Sasson, Turner-Brown, Holtzclaw, Lam and Bodfish2008).

Differences in individual responsivity to social rewards exist across the population but can be particularly notable in conditions such as autism. Autistic individuals often show reduced responsivity to social rewards in lab-based assessments (Hedger et al., Reference Hedger, Dubey and Chakrabarti2020). Lower activity in reward-related brain regions such as mOFC and VS to social rewards has also been reported in autistic compared to non-autistic adults (Kohls, Schulte-Rüther, et al., Reference Kohls, Schulte-Rüther, Nehrkorn, Müller, Fink, Kamp-Becker, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Schultz and Konrad2013; Scott-Van Zeeland et al., Reference Scott-Van Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack and Bookheimer2010) but see Dichter et al. (Reference Dichter, Felder, Green, Rittenberg, Sasson and Bodfish2012) for an exception. A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies reported differential reward system responses to both social and nonsocial rewards in autistic individuals (Clements et al., Reference Clements, Zoltowski, Yankowitz, Yerys, Schultz and Herrington2018). Similar to the studies on non-autistic individuals, the majority of studies included in this meta-analysis examined social or nonsocial reward processing in isolation.

To address the gaps in the literature, the current study aimed to develop and test a new stimuli set and paradigm to investigate neural processing of social vs. nonsocial rewards, when these different categories of stimuli are closely matched on stimulus parameters. The stimuli were drawn from well-characterized real-world images and hence have higher ecological validity than schematic faces/ posed expressions. However, it is worth noting that all the pictures may not evoke strong subjective feelings of reward. To this end, our use of the term “reward” is more aligned with the definition of emotion proposed by Ralph Adolphs – in proposing that it is a functional state that can enable subjective feelings of pleasure, as well as behavioral changes (Fox et al., Reference Fox, Lapate, Shackman and Davidson2018). A secondary aim was to investigate if individual differences in functional connectivity between key brain nodes involved in reward and social processing was associated with autistic traits. In light of the previous studies on autism and social rewards, we hypothesized that individuals with high autistic traits will have reduced preference for social rewards, and have reduced functional connectivity between brain regions involved in social reward processing (Chevallier et al., Reference Chevallier, Kohls, Troiani, Brodkin and Schultz2012)

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

40 participants completed the study from the local population in and around the university. Two participants were excluded from the analysis due to a programming error and one participant was removed due to excessive movement (see data analysis), leaving 37 participants (19 females; mean age = 22.89 years, S.D. = 5.02 years, min = 18, max = 40). All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision and completed the 50-item Autism Spectrum Quotient with binarized response format (AQ; Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin and Clubley2001). The mean AQ score was 14.3 (SD = 7.74), the scores ranged from 3–33 and a higher score reflects more autistic traits. Participants were not asked about whether they had a clinical diagnosis of any neurodevelopmental conditions, such as autism. Autistic traits are continuously distributed between those with and without a diagnosis (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Koenen, McCormick, Munir, Hallett, Happé, Plomin and Ronald2011).

2.2. Design and materials

Forty pairs of social and nonsocial reward images (images can be requested from the authors) were taken from the International Affective Picture System (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Bradley and Cuthbert1999) and downloaded from the website Flickr. Social reward images included one or more humans (e.g. happy individuals), while nonsocial reward images included rewarding nonsocial objects (e.g. food and scenery). The social images comprised happy children and adults in different real-world contexts ranging from weddings, sports events, social events, and family photographs. Half of the nonsocial images included food, and the remaining images were of scenery, vehicles, money, and one piece of jewellery.

The images were modified with ImageJ (Abràmoff, Reference Abràmoff2004) and Adobe Photoshop to match them for size and multiple image parameters (e.g. contrast and luminance). The Koch toolbox (Walther & Koch, Reference Walther and Koch2006) was used to calculate image saliency for each pair of images in a previous paper, and there was no mean difference in luminosity, root mean square (RMS) contrast (local and global), as well as Koch toolbox metric of image saliency between social and nonsocial images (Chakrabarti et al., Reference Chakrabarti, Haffey, Canzano, Taylor and McSorley2017). Previous studies using these stimuli have demonstrated greater gaze fixation for social over nonsocial images using eye-tracking, and quicker subjective awareness of social over nonsocial stimuli using continuous flash suppression (Chakrabarti et al., Reference Chakrabarti, Haffey, Canzano, Taylor and McSorley2017; Gray et al., Reference Gray, Haffey, Mihaylova and Chakrabarti2018; Hedger et al., Reference Hedger, Haffey, McSorley and Chakrabarti2018, Reference Hedger, Dubey and Chakrabarti2020).

All stimuli were displayed using E-prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, PA, USA) on a ViewSonic VE510s monitor (ViewSonic Corporation, California, USA).

2.3. Procedures

Participants performed a rating task outside the scanner and a fMRI task. During the rating task, participants were presented with an image to give a valence rating on a 9-point Likert scale using the manikins from (Bradley & Lang, Reference Bradley and Lang1994). Participants were instructed to rate how happy the images were by clicking one of 9 manikins conveying a range of emotions from unhappy to happy.

During the fMRI task, the social and nonsocial images were presented in pseudo-randomized blocks of four images of the same condition (social/ nonsocial), followed by a block of four subsequent fixation crosses. The order of presentation of social and nonsocial images was randomized within these constraints. Each image was presented for 4 seconds, fixation crosses for 3 seconds, followed by 1 second of blank screen. To maintain attention to the task, participants were required to press a button with their right index finger during each presentation of an image or fixation cross. The 50-item Autism Quotient questionnaire (AQ; Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin and Clubley2001) was completed online after the scanning session.

2.4. Valence ratings

Across the whole sample, t-tests were used to test a) whether the images are perceived as rewarding and b) that social images were not significantly different in their reward value to nonsocial images. At an individual level, mean valence ratings were calculated separately for social reward images and nonsocial reward images. Pearson’s r was computed for correlations between AQ and these mean ratings to test whether autistic traits were associated with diminished reward value for social rewards. Additionally, a composite metric for quantifying individual preference for social vs. nonsocial rewards (i.e. sociality bias ) was computed by subtracting nonsocial from social mean valence ratings. Valence ratings that were 3 or more SDs away from the mean across both social and nonsocial images were removed as outliers for each participant.

2.5. Regions of interest

Based on our a priori hypotheses, we selected our specific regions of interest (ROIs) based on meta-analyses of sociality- (Atzil et al., Reference Atzil, Satpute, Zhang, Parrish, Shablack, MacCormack and Lindquist2023) and reward-related (Liu, et al., Reference Liu, Hairston, Schrier and Fan2011) brain regions. The masks for sociality-related regions were defined functionally by the activation maps of Atzil et al.’s multilevel peak kernel density analysis on the contrast: [All social stimuli > No social stimuli] (bilateral amygdala, bilateral FG, and right TPJ, Fig. 1A). The masks for reward-related regions were defined functionally by making 10mm spheres centered on coordinates of brain areas activated by reward outcome (bilateral VS: [12, 10 −6] and [−10 8 −4], and one mask made of two overlapping spheres of bilateral mOFC: [−2 56 −6] and [2 48 −14], and ACC [8 24 32], Fig. 1B) reported in Liu et al.

Figure 1. Predefined regions of interest. A. Axial sections at MNI z = –18 (left) and z = 7 (right) showing ROIs defined functionally by the activation maps of Atzil et al.’s multilevel peak kernel density analysis: left amygdala (red), right amygdala (blue), left FG (green), right FG (magenta), and right TPJ (yellow). B. Sections at MNI x = 0 (upper right), y = 9 (upper left), and z = 32 (lower) showing ROIs defined functionally by making 10 mm spheres centered on coordinates of brain areas activated by reward outcome reported in Liu et al.: left VS (red) and right VS (blue)], mOFC (green), and ACC (magenta).

2.6. fMRI data acquisition and preprocessing

Scans were conducted using a 3T Siemens TIM Trio MRI scanner with 12-channel head coil. One run of 322 volumes was measured per participant using a T2*-weighted echo-planar sequence [slice thickness: 3 mm, no gap, 37 slices, repetition time (TR): 2s, echo time (TE): 30ms, echo spacing: 0.53ms, generalized autocalibrating partially parallel acquisitions (GRAPPA) acceleration factor: 2, flip angle: 90°, matrix: 64 × 64, field of view (FOV): 192 mm, voxel size: 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm] and individual high-resolution T1- weighted anatomical data (MPRAGE sequence) were acquired (TR: 2.2, TE: 2.9, FOV: 250, matrix: 256 × 256, sagittal plane, slice thickness: 1 mm, 176 slices, resolution: 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm). The phase and magnitude images for the construction of the field map were collected with the same positions and dimensions as the functional scans [slice thickness: 3 mm, no gap, 32 slices, TR: 400 ms, TE: 5.19 and 7.65 ms, flip angle: 60°, matrix: 64 × 64, FOV: 192 mm, voxel size: 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm].

The data were then further preprocessed and analyzed using MATLAB 2024b and the software package SPM25 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Preprocessing consisted of slice-timing correction, realignment for motion correction, unwarping with the field map in the first and second order for rotations along the x and y axis, and coregistration of the structural image to the mean realigned functional image. Structural images were segmented into gray matter, white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, bone, soft tissue, and air/background with the “New Segment” module (Ashburner & Friston, Reference Ashburner and Friston2005). A group anatomical template was created with DARTEL (Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponentiated Lie algebra; Ashburner, Reference Ashburner2007) toolbox from the segmented gray and white matter images. Transformation parameters for structural images were then applied to functional images to normalize them to the brain template of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) supplied with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM). Functional images were spatially smoothed with a kernel of 6 mm full-width-at-half-maximum after normalization.

2.7. fMRI mass univariate GLM analysis

Two participants were removed due to programming error: blocks were presented in an entirely random order to the first two participants, resulting in clusters of 5 blocks of the same stimulus type in a row for both. The block order of the remaining participants was pseudo-randomized so that half the participants received one order of social and nonsocial blocks, whereas the other received the reversed order. Due to the difference between the block structure in the first two participants and the remaining 38, these two participants were not included in the analysis. One further participant’s data was removed due to excessive movement, having moved more than 3 mm between two volumes, leaving 37 participants.

Statistical parametric maps were calculated with multiple regressions of the data onto a model of the hemodynamic response (Friston et al., Reference Friston, Holmes, Poline, Grasby, Williams, Frackowiak and Turner1995). The first level general linear model analyses contained two psychological regressors of interest for “Social” and “Nonsocial” conditions in the design matrix. Each block in each condition lasted 16 seconds. Psychological regressors were convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function. Six motion parameters were included as nuisance regressors. A high-pass filter with a cut-off period of 128 s was applied, and the temporal autocorrelation was accounted for with the AR(1) option. For each ROI, the first eigenvariate of the contrast [social – nonsocial] for each participant were extracted with MarsBaR (version 0.45).

2.8. fMRI mass univariate PPI analysis

In order to test whether the sociality-and reward-associated brain activity in ROIs is functionally coupled, a generalized Psychophysiological Interaction analysis (gPPI toolbox v13.1; Friston et al., Reference Friston, Buechel, Fink, Morris, Rolls and Dolan1997; McLaren et al., Reference McLaren, Ries, Xu and Johnson2012) was performed. ROI’s from Atzil’s et al. (Reference Atzil, Satpute, Zhang, Parrish, Shablack, MacCormack and Lindquist2023) were defined as the seed region, while ROIs from Liu et al. (Reference Liu, Hairston, Schrier and Fan2011) others were defined as target regions. The vector of neural response was estimated by deconvoluting the first eigenvariate of the BOLD signal extracted from the seed region. The interaction vector of each condition was calculated as the product of the estimated neural response vector and the condition vector’s ON times. We then performed the general linear model analysis for the whole brain, including the interaction vectors, the condition vectors, and the estimated neural response vector of the seed region as regressors. For each target region, the first eigenvariate of the univariate contrast between the interaction term of the social condition and that of the nonsocial condition for each participant were extracted with MarsBaR (version 0.45) and used for the group-level one-sample t-test. To test whether autistic traits modulated the functional connectivity between brain activity associated with social stimuli processing and reward value responsiveness, AQ was correlated with the contrast values of the target regions in the gPPI analysis.

For the purposes of completeness, we provide whole-brain analyses in the supplementary materials. Family-wise error corrected p < 0.05 at the peak level, cluster size ≧ 5, was used as the significant threshold for whole-brain results.

Participants were identified as correlational outliers if their Cook’s D value was greater than 4/35 (i.e. 4/(N-1-K)) and were removed from the specific correlation that they were outliers for.

All analyses use 1-tailed p-values to reflect the directional nature of the hypotheses that: autistic traits will be associated with reduced sociality bias and reduced connectivity between social and reward regions; sociality bias will be associated with increased connectivity between social and reward regions. Bootstrapped confidence intervals with replacement based on 10,000 repetitions are included for all correlations to address concerns about small sample sizes. Holm–Šidák corrections for multiple testing are applied to correlations between sociality bias and connectivity for each pair of regions (see supplementary materials 2).

3. Results

3.1. Rating task

T-tests of the mean ratings of the images were conducted to test the assumption that the images were similarly pleasant to participants in this sample. No significant difference between social and nonsocial was found across the whole sample (t(36) = .47, p = .643, 95% CI [−0.3, 0.18], d = .08). One sample t-tests found that the mean ratings for social (M = 6.47, SD=.69, t(36) = 13.01, p < .001, 95% CI [6.24, 6.69], d = 2.14) and nonsocial images (M=6.52, SD=.63, t(36) = 14.66, p < .001, 95% CI [6.31, 6.73], d = 2.41) were significantly above the neutral rating of 5, suggesting that the participants found the images rewarding. Against expectations, some participants rated some images as unpleasant (less than 5), but this was only true for 5.45% of social images and 4% of nonsocial images across all participants.

AQ correlated negatively with valence ratings of social rewards (r(32) = −.49, p = .001; bootstrap CI: [−.68; −.3]) but no such pattern was seen for valence ratings of nonsocial rewards (r(33) = −.022, p = .901; bootstrap CI: [−.26; .23]). The negative association between AQ with sociality bias was significant (r(35) = −.36, p = .016; bootstrap CI: [−.54; −.19]), consistent with autistic traits being associated with reduced preference for social than nonsocial images (see Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Scatterplot of associations between autistic traits measured using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and social and nonsocial valence ratings. Note: These results visualize how autistic traits (AQ) are negatively associated with valence ratings for social (r(32) = –.49, p = .001) but not nonsocial images (r(33) = –.02, p = .9).

3.2. Brain data

3.2.1. Whole-brain GLM results

At the whole brain level, the following regions showed significantly stronger activity in the social than in the nonsocial condition: bilateral amygdala, right fusiform gyrus, right medial orbitofrontal cortex (gyrus rectus), and bilateral temporo-occipital junction. Bilateral fusiform gyrus, visual cortex, and left lateral frontopolar cortex were more active in the nonsocial than in the social condition (Table 1). No significant result was found in the whole-brain correlation of the social-nonsocial contrast values with AQ or social bias.

Table 1. Whole brain results of Social vs. Nonsocial conditions

* Voxel level FWE-corrected for the whole brain.

Abbreviations: H = hemisphere; L = left; R = right; p = p-value; T = T-value; B.A. = Brodmann area; x, y, z = MNI coordinates.

3.2.2. Brain-behavior relationship

After applying Holm–Šidák corrections to alpha thresholds to address multiple testing, sociality bias was positively associated with functional connectivity from the LFG to ACC (r(30) = .64, p < .001; bootstrap CI: [.36; .78]) and the LAI to mOFC (r(34) = .52, p < .001; bootstrap CI: [.35; .66]). AQ was significantly associated with both LFG to ACC (r(31) = −.3, p = .04; bootstrap CI: [−.54; −.05]) and LAI to mOFC connectivity (r(32) = −.45, p = .004; bootstrap CI: [−.67; −.15]).

Mediation analysis found that LFG to ACC connectivity fully mediated the association between AQ and sociality bias as the indirect effect was significant (b = −.015, p = .044, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval lower limit/LL = −.035, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval upper limit/UL < −.001) but the direct effect was not significant (b = −.005, p = .689, bootstrapped LL = −.035, UL = .019; See Fig.3). Mediation analysis found that LAI to mOFC connectivity fully mediated the association between AQ and sociality bias as the indirect effect was significant (b = − .022, p = .031, bootstrapped LL = −.049, UL = −.002) but the direct effect was not significant (b = −.01, p = .551, LL = −.056, UL = .022; See Fig.4).

Figure 3. Grid of scatterplots summarizing the associations between AQ, LFG to ACC connectivity and sociality bias (in blue), with the indirect and direct from mediation analysis (in red). Note: all correlations are significant under non-parametric Spearman’s rank correlation (see supplementary analysis 1).

Figure 4. Grid of scatterplots summarizing the associations between AQ, LAI to mOFC connectivity and sociality bias (in blue), with the indirect and direct from mediation analysis (in red). Note: all correlations are significant under non-parametric Spearman’s rank correlation (see supplementary analysis 1).

4. Discussion

We compared response to social vs nonsocial rewards in a sample of young adults using self-report and fMRI. We found that behavioral preference for social over nonsocial rewards (i.e. sociality bias) was positively associated with functional connectivity of LFG-ACC and LAI-mOFC. We further found that the relationship between autistic traits and sociality bias is fully mediated by the functional connectivity of LFG-ACC and LAI-mOFC.

A large number of behavioral and eye-tracking studies have previously compared processing of social vs nonsocial stimuli (Hedger et al., Reference Hedger, Dubey and Chakrabarti2020; Frazier et al., Reference Frazier, Strauss, Klingemier, Zetzer, Hardan, Eng and Youngstrom2017). Most of these studies have been conducted in young children, and included both rewarding and neutral stimuli. The few studies that have directly compared social and nonsocial reward processing within a single paradigm have tested the anticipatory component of reward processing rather than the consummatory component of it (Ait Oumeziane et al., Reference Ait Oumeziane, Schryer-Praga and Foti2017; Lin, Adolphs, et al., Reference Lin, Adolphs and Rangel2012; Lin, Rangel, et al., Reference Lin, Rangel and Adolphs2012; Spreckelmeyer et al., Reference Spreckelmeyer, Krach, Kohls, Rademacher, Irmak, Konrad, Kircher and Gründer2009; Wake & Izuma, Reference Wake and Izuma2017; although see Rademacher et al., Reference Rademacher, Krach, Kohls, Irmak, Gründer and Spreckelmeyer2010). The current study focuses on the consummatory component of reward processing, by presenting the social and nonsocial reward stimuli to the participants without them having to perform any action. The current design thus allows us to infer the differential neural response to receipt of these different types of reward, without the impact of trade-offs between effort minimization and value maximization. Additionally, the close matching of the social and nonsocial reward stimuli allows us to make inferences that are less subject to confounds due to stimulus features such as contrast, luminosity, and saliency.

We found that even when social reward images are matched on basic stimulus properties, and rated as equally rewarding to nonsocial reward images across the full sample, autistic traits were coupled with individual preference for social rewards both behaviorally and neurally. At a behavioral level, autistic traits were inversely proportional to the valence ratings for social reward images. This relationship was not seen for nonsocial rewards. This observation is consistent with earlier empirical reports showing a reduced rating of social stimuli or situations by autistic individuals in lab-based studies (Chevallier et al., Reference Chevallier, Kohls, Troiani, Brodkin and Schultz2012; Sasson et al., Reference Sasson, Dichter and Bodfish2012). At the neural level, we found that the functional coupling of LFG-ACC and LAI-mOFC was weaker in individuals with higher autistic traits. While the role of the fusiform gyrus in conspecific face and body processing is well established, a recent study has also identified it as a hub of expression for genes involved in synaptic transmission as well as autism (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Zhang, Wang, Jiang, Cai, Zhao and Yu2022). Neurochemically, an earlier study reported reduced GABAB receptors in both fusiform gyrus as well as the ACC – which are involved both in maintaining synapses and the excitation-inhibition balance in the brain (Oblak et al., Reference Oblak, Gibbs and Blatt2010). Functionally, a weaker coupling between fusiform gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex corresponds to reduced links between social perceptual encoding and value encoding. Assuming a certain degree of functional specialization in the brain, the fusiform gyrus and nearby areas are more involved in perceptual encoding of stimuli (Naspi et al., Reference Naspi, Hoffman, Devereux and Morcom2021) – while the ACC is arguably involved more in encoding stimulus value (Cai and Padoa-Schioppa, Reference Cai and Padoa-Schioppa2012). This observation is also consistent with earlier accounts of reduced links between reward and social processing in autism (Neufeld et al., Reference Neufeld, Hsu and Chakrabarti2019; Sims et al., Reference Sims, Neufeld, Johnstone and Chakrabarti2014).

The LAI-mOFC functional coupling represented another link between social perceptual and value encoding, which we found to be negatively modulated by autistic traits and further modulate social bias. Anterior insula receives interoceptive and nociceptive afferent input from inner organs and skin (Craig, Reference Craig2009), and has long been associated with pain empathy (Lamm et al., Reference Lamm, Batson and Decety2007, 2009, Bernhardt and Singer, Reference Bernhardt and Singer2012) and social norm compliance (Bellucci et al., Reference Bellucci, Feng, Camilleri, Eickhoff and Krueger2018). Previous studies have investigated the role of interoception in social cognition (Arnold et al., Reference Arnold, Winkielman and Dobkins2019). For example, higher interoceptive awareness is associated with less negative affect after challenging social situations, due to the ability to properly attribute the physiological response as resulting from the external social situation, resulting in better emotional regulation in social situations (Werner et al., Reference Werner, Kerschreiter, Kindermann and Duschek2013). The mOFC is associated with reward value encoding (Kringelbach, Reference Kringelbach2005; Rolls et al., Reference Rolls, Cheng and Feng2020). In mammals, there is direct projection from anterior insula to orbitofrontal cortex (Mathiasen et al., Reference Mathiasen, Aggleton and Witter2023). In humans, the coactivation of LAI and mOFC has been associated with the pro-social behavior of self-impression/reputation management when being observed by others (Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Kim, Jung and Kim2021), which was said to be a behavioral manifestation of allostatic regulatory process, to prevent homeostatic disturbances accompanied by social threat/stress/punishment (Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Way, Eisenberger and Taylor2010). In the present study, participants showing preference for social rewards had enhanced functional coupling when viewing social stimuli with human figures, which might be associated with stronger incentive or preparedness for self-impression/reputation management and the consequential social rewards (Tennie et al., Reference Tennie, Frith and Frith2010).

Interestingly, the functional coupling of LFG-ACC and LAI-mOFC fully mediated the association between AQ and the behavioral preference for social over nonsocial rewards (i.e. sociality bias). While autism has been associated with reduced preference for social rewards in several prior studies (Frazier et al., Reference Frazier, Strauss, Klingemier, Zetzer, Hardan, Eng and Youngstrom2017; and specifically using these stimuli by Hedger et al., Reference Hedger, Haffey, McSorley and Chakrabarti2018), direct investigations of underlying mechanisms linking neural and behavioral data have been rare. To this end, the mediation analysis suggests an indicative neural mechanism to explain the behavioral observation of reduced preference for social rewards in individuals with high autistic traits. However, similar to correlations, claims about causality from mediation in cross-sectional data are more vulnerable to overlooking causal covariates or confounds than experimental or longitudinal designs (Rohrer et al., Reference Rohrer, Hünermund, Arslan and Elson2022).

Finally, we discuss the results of the whole-brain univariate contrast for the sake of completeness. These analyses (Table 1) showed that social condition more specifically activated the right fusiform gyrus and bilateral amygdala, both of which were reported as brain regions consistently activated for social stimuli (Atzil et al., Reference Atzil, Satpute, Zhang, Parrish, Shablack, MacCormack and Lindquist2023). In addition, the right medial orbitofrontal cortex (gyrus rectus) was more active in the social condition, which meta-analyses have reported as related to monitoring, learning, and memory of reward reinforcers (Kringelbach, Reference Kringelbach2005; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Hairston, Schrier and Fan2011). The results also confirmed the validity of our social reward stimuli at a neural level.

Some caveats need to be considered while interpreting these results. First, even though the social reward images are real-world images – and offer more ecological validity than point light figures or isolated faces – they are still unrelated to the participant’s current context. The participant does not interact with these stimuli. Future studies should aim to improve further on the ecological validity of social reward conditions by maximizing sensory richness of stimuli and participant engagement (Stijovic et al., Reference Stijovic, Siegel, Kocan, Bojkovska, Korb and Silani2024). Second, the measure of autistic traits used in this study is a self-report measure, which renders it likely to be influenced by a number of sociological factors including gender, and culture. Future studies could consider moving beyond self-report to index autism-related phenotypic variation in the general population, e.g. through the use of simple behavioral tasks that pertain to different domains of the autistic phenotype (Dubey et al., Reference Dubey, Bishain, Dasgupta, Bhavnani, Belmonte, Gliga and Chakrabarti2024). Third, larger samples in which there are enough autistic participants would allow analysis of whether patterns are found equally in both diagnosed and non-diagnosed samples, or could highlight if there are qualitative differences in how autistic traits relate to social reward value between these groups. Finally, the limited sample size in the current study did not allow for a meaningful examination of potential sex differences. Future studies with larger, balanced samples should test the impact of sex and its interaction with autistic traits on the reported variables.

In conclusion, we compared the consummatory aspect of social and nonsocial reward processing within a single paradigm in a sample of young adults, using behavioral and fMRI measures. We found that greater functional coupling between the LFG and the ACC was associated with a significantly higher behavioral preference for social over nonsocial rewards. We further found that LFG to ACC connectivity fully mediated the association between autistic traits and the behavioral preference for social over nonsocial rewards. This result points to a potential neural mechanism underlying the observation that individuals with high autistic traits often find social stimuli less rewarding in lab-based tasks, compared to those with low autistic traits. Future work should test the generalisability of this mechanism by probing it further in larger samples and with clinically diagnosed autistic adults and children.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2025.10003.

Data availability statement

Data and scripts analyzing it can be accessed at: https://osf.io/jmqv9/

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Garret O’Connell for his help with data collection and our participants for their time.

Author contributions statement

AH and BC conceptualized the experiment. AH collected the data. BC provided research supervision. AH and C-TH analyzed the results. AH, C-TH and BC co-wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the Medical Research Council UK (G1100359/1) and the Leverhulme Trust (PLP-2015-329) to BC, and an ESRC-MRC interdisciplinary studentship to AH.

Competing Interests

The authors declare they have no financial or non-financial interests.

Ethics approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Ethics approval was obtained from the University Research Ethics Committee (Ref. UREC 13/35).

Consent

All participants gave written informed consent to experiment participation and experimental data publication.

Open Practices Statement

Data, material, and analysis code are available by contacting the corresponding author. None of the experiment was preregistered.

Footnotes

*

Equally contributed, co-corresponding authors.

References

Abràmoff, D. M. D. (2004). Image Processing with ImageJ. Biophotonics International, 11, 3641.Google Scholar
Ait Oumeziane, B., Schryer-Praga, J., & Foti, D. (2017). “Why don’t they ‘like’ me more?”: Comparing the time courses of social and monetary reward processing. Neuropsychologia, 107, 4859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305309. https://doi.org/10.1038/35077083 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, A. J., Winkielman, P., & Dobkins, K. (2019). Interoception and Social Connection. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2589. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02589 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage, 38, 95113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26, 839851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atzil, S., Satpute, A. B., Zhang, J., Parrish, M. H., Shablack, H., MacCormack, J. K., … & Lindquist, K. A. (2023). The impact of sociality and affective valence on brain activation: a meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 268, 119879.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.119879CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 517.10.1023/A:1005653411471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellucci, G., Feng, C., Camilleri, J., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2018). The role of the anterior insula in social norm compliance and enforcement: evidence from coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 378389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernhardt, B. C., & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 123. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150536 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 4959. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, X., & Padoa-Schioppa, C. (2012). Neuronal encoding of subjective value in dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 37913808.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3864-11.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chakrabarti, B., Haffey, A., Canzano, L., Taylor, C. P., & McSorley, E. (2017). Individual differences in responsivity to social rewards: insights from two eye-tracking tasks. PLOS ONE, 12, e0185146. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185146 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., Zhang, C., Wang, R., Jiang, P., Cai, H., Zhao, W., … & Yu, Y. (2022). Molecular basis underlying functional connectivity of fusiform gyrus subregions: a transcriptome-neuroimaging spatial correlation study. Cortex, 152, 5973. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2022.03.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clements, C. C., Zoltowski, A. R., Yankowitz, L. D., Yerys, B. E., Schultz, R. T., & Herrington, J. D. (2018). Evaluation of the social motivation hypothesis of Autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 797808. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—Now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 5970. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dichter, G. S., Felder, J. N., Green, S. R., Rittenberg, A. M., Sasson, N. J., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 160172. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq095 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubey, I., Bishain, R., Dasgupta, J., Bhavnani, S., Belmonte, M. K., Gliga, T., ... & Chakrabarti, B. (2024). Using mobile health technology to assess childhood autism in low-resource community settings in India: An innovation to address the detection gap. Autism, 28, 755769.10.1177/13623613231182801CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R., Völlm, B., Drury, A., McKie, S., Richardson, P., & William Deakin, J. F. (2006). Co-operation with another player in a financially rewarded guessing game activates regions implicated in theory of mind. Social Neuroscience, 1, 385395. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910601041358 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Effects of direct social experience on trust decisions and neural reward circuitry. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 148.10.3389/fnins.2012.00148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fareri, D. S., Chang, L. J., & Delgado, M. R. (2015). Computational substrates of social value in interpersonal collaboration. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 81708180. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4775-14.2015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008). Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene. Perception, 37, 571583. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5705 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (2018). The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frazier, T. W., Strauss, M., Klingemier, E. W., Zetzer, E. E., Hardan, A. Y., Eng, C., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2017). A meta-analysis of gaze differences to social and nonsocial information between individuals with and without autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56, 546555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, D. P., Aggleton, J. P., & Saunders, R. C. (2002). Comparison of hippocampal, amygdala, and perirhinal projections to the nucleus accumbens: Combined anterograde and retrograde tracing study in the Macaque brain. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 450, 345365. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.10336 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage, 6, 218229. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1997.0291 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Poline, J. B., Grasby, P. J., Williams, S. C., Frackowiak, R. S., & Turner, R. (1995). Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited. NeuroImage, 2, 4553. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1995.1007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fudge, J. L., Kunishio, K., Walsh, P., Richard, C., & Haber, S. N. (2002). Amygdaloid projections to ventromedial striatal subterritories in the primate. Neuroscience, 110, 257275. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00546-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grabenhorst, F., D’Souza, A. A., Parris, B. A., Rolls, E. T., & Passingham, R. E. (2010). A common neural scale for the subjective pleasantness of different primary rewards. NeuroImage, 51, 12651274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K. L. H., Haffey, A., Mihaylova, H. L., & Chakrabarti, B. (2018). Lack of privileged access to awareness for rewarding social scenes in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48, 33113318. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3595-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gu, R., Huang, W., Camilleri, J., Xu, P., Wei, P., Eickhoff, S. B., & Feng, C. (2019). Love is analogous to money in human brain: coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses of social and monetary reward anticipation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 100, 108128.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.02.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Güroğlu, B., Haselager, G. J. T., van Lieshout, C. F. M., Takashima, A., Rijpkema, M., & Fernández, G. (2008). Why are friends special? Implementing a social interaction simulation task to probe the neural correlates of friendship. NeuroImage, 39, 903910. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, S. N., Kim, K.-S., Mailly, P., & Calzavara, R. (2006). Reward-related cortical inputs define a large striatal region in primates that interface with associative cortical connections, providing a substrate for incentive-based Learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 83688376. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0271-06.2006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haber, S. N., & Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 426. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.129 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedger, N., Dubey, I., & Chakrabarti, B. (2020). Social orienting and social seeking behaviors in ASD. A meta analytic investigation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 119, 376395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.10.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedger, N., Haffey, A., McSorley, E., & Chakrabarti, B. (2018). Empathy modulates the temporal structure of social attention. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285, 20181716. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.1716 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izuma, K., Saito, D. N., & Sadato, N. (2008). Processing of social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Neuron, 58, 284294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohls, G., Schulte-Rüther, M., Nehrkorn, B., Müller, K., Fink, G. R., Kamp-Becker, I., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Schultz, R. T., & Konrad, K. (2013). Reward system dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 565572. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss033 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci, 6, 691702. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1747 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuss, K., Falk, A., Trautner, P., Elger, C. E., Weber, B., & Fliessbach, K. (2013). A reward prediction error for charitable donations reveals outcome orientation of donators. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 216223. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 4258. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1999). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. http://www.hsp.epm.br/dpsicobio/Nova_versao_pagina_psicobio/adap/instructions.pdf Google Scholar
Lehéricy, S., Ducros, M., Van De Moortele, P.-F., Francois, C., Thivard, L., Poupon, C., Swindale, N., Ugurbil, K., & Kim, D.-S. (2004). Diffusion tensor fiber tracking shows distinct corticostriatal circuits in humans: DTI Corticostriatal Fibers. Annals of Neurology, 55, 522529. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.20030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, A., Adolphs, R., & Rangel, A. (2012). Social and monetary reward learning engage overlapping neural substrates. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 274281. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, A., Rangel, A., & Adolphs, R. (2012). Impaired learning of social compared to monetary rewards in autismFrontiers in Neuroscience6, 143. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00143 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, X., Hairston, J., Schrier, M., & Fan, J. (2011). Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 12191236.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahler, S. V., & Berridge, K. C. (2009). Which cue to “Want?” Central amygdala opioid activation enhances and focuses incentive salience on a prepotent reward cue. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 65006513. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3875-08.2009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathiasen, M. L., Aggleton, J. P., & Witter, M. P. (2023). Projections of the insular cortex to orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex: a tracing study in the rat. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 17, 1131167. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2023.1131167 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaren, D. G., Ries, M. L., Xu, G., & Johnson, S. C. (2012). A generalized form of context-dependent psychophysiological interactions (gPPI): a comparison to standard approaches. NeuroImage, 61, 12771286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.068 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naspi, L., Hoffman, P., Devereux, B., & Morcom, A. M. (2021). Perceptual and semantic representations at encoding contribute to true and false recognition of objects. Journal of Neuroscience, 41, 83758389.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0677-21.2021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neufeld, J., Hsu, C.-T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2019). Atypical reward-driven modulation of mimicry-related neural activity in autism. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 327. https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/psychiatry/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00327/full 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00327CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oblak, A. L., Gibbs, T. T., & Blatt, G. J. (2010). Decreased GABA B receptors in the cingulate cortex and fusiform gyrus in Autism. Journal of Neurochemistry, 114, 14141423. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06858.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rademacher, L., Krach, S., Kohls, G., Irmak, A., Gründer, G., & Spreckelmeyer, K. N. (2010). Dissociation of neural networks for anticipation and consumption of monetary and social rewards. NeuroImage, 49, 32763285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.089 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rademacher, L., Salama, A., Gründer, G., & Spreckelmeyer, K. N. (2014). Differential patterns of nucleus accumbens activation during anticipation of monetary and social reward in young and older adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 825831. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst047 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, E. B., Koenen, K. C., McCormick, M. C., Munir, K., Hallett, V., Happé, F., Plomin, R., & Ronald, A. (2011). Evidence that autistic traits show the same etiology in the general population and at the quantitative extremes (5%, 2.5%, and 1%). Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 11131121.10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohrer, J. M., Hünermund, P., Arslan, R. C., & Elson, M. (2022). That’sa lot to process! Pitfalls of popular path models. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 5, 25152459221095827.10.1177/25152459221095827CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolls, E. T., Cheng, W., & Feng, J. (2020). The orbitofrontal cortex: reward, emotion and depression. Brain Communications, 2, fcaa196. https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcaa196 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russchen, F. T., Bakst, I., Amaral, D. G., & Price, J. L. (1985). The amygdalostriatal projections in the monkey. An anterograde tracing study. Brain Research, 329, 241257. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(85)90530-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasson, N. J., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Affective responses by adults with autism are reduced to social images but elevated to images related to circumscribed interests. PLoS ONE, 7, e42457. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042457 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1, 3142.10.1002/aur.4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saygin, A. P., Wilson, S. M., Hagler, D. J., Bates, E., & Sereno, M. I. (2004). Point-light biological motion perception activates human premotor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 61816188. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0504-04.2004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, K. N., Sciarillo, X. A., Nudelman, J. L., Cheer, J. F., & Roesch, M. R. (2020). Anterior cingulate cortex signals attention in a social paradigm that manipulates reward and shock. Current Biology, 30, 37243735.10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Van Zeeland, A. A., Dapretto, M., Ghahremani, D. G., Poldrack, R. A., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2010). Reward processing in Autism. Autism Research, 3, 5367. https://doi.org/10.1002/Aur.122 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sims, T. B., Neufeld, J., Johnstone, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2014). Autistic traits modulate frontostriatal connectivity during processing of rewarding faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 20102016. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 1481714822. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1009164107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreckelmeyer, K. N., Krach, S., Kohls, G., Rademacher, L., Irmak, A., Konrad, K., Kircher, T., & Gründer, G. (2009). Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 158165. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stijovic, A., Siegel, M., Kocan, A. U., Bojkovska, I., Korb, S., & Silani, G. (2024). Defining social reward: A systematic review of human and animal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 150, 14721509. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/bul0000455 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strombach, T., Weber, B., Hangebrauk, Z., Kenning, P., Karipidis, I. I., Tobler, P. N., & Kalenscher, T. (2015). Social discounting involves modulation of neural value signals by temporoparietal junction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112 , 16191624.10.1073/pnas.1414715112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Reputation management in the age of the world-wide web. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 482488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vouloumanos, A., Hauser, M. D., Werker, J. F., & Martin, A. (2010). The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child Development, 81, 517527. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01412.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wake, S. J., & Izuma, K. (2017). A common neural code for social and monetary rewards in the human striatum. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 15581564. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walther, D., & Koch, C. (2006). Modeling attention to salient proto-objects. Neural Networks, 19, 13951407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2006.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, N. S., Kerschreiter, R., Kindermann, N. K., & Duschek, S. (2013). Interoceptive awareness as a moderator of affective responses to social exclusion. Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 3950. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000086 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, L., Kim, K., Jung, D., & Kim, H. (2021). Roles of the MPFC and insula in impression management under social observation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16, 474483. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zink, C. F., Tong, Y., Chen, Q., Bassett, D. S., Stein, J. L., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2008). Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron, 58, 273283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Predefined regions of interest. A. Axial sections at MNI z = –18 (left) and z = 7 (right) showing ROIs defined functionally by the activation maps of Atzil et al.’s multilevel peak kernel density analysis: left amygdala (red), right amygdala (blue), left FG (green), right FG (magenta), and right TPJ (yellow). B. Sections at MNI x = 0 (upper right), y = 9 (upper left), and z = 32 (lower) showing ROIs defined functionally by making 10 mm spheres centered on coordinates of brain areas activated by reward outcome reported in Liu et al.: left VS (red) and right VS (blue)], mOFC (green), and ACC (magenta).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Scatterplot of associations between autistic traits measured using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and social and nonsocial valence ratings. Note: These results visualize how autistic traits (AQ) are negatively associated with valence ratings for social (r(32) = –.49, p = .001) but not nonsocial images (r(33) = –.02, p = .9).

Figure 2

Table 1. Whole brain results of Social vs. Nonsocial conditions

Figure 3

Figure 3. Grid of scatterplots summarizing the associations between AQ, LFG to ACC connectivity and sociality bias (in blue), with the indirect and direct from mediation analysis (in red). Note: all correlations are significant under non-parametric Spearman’s rank correlation (see supplementary analysis 1).

Figure 4

Figure 4. Grid of scatterplots summarizing the associations between AQ, LAI to mOFC connectivity and sociality bias (in blue), with the indirect and direct from mediation analysis (in red). Note: all correlations are significant under non-parametric Spearman’s rank correlation (see supplementary analysis 1).

Supplementary material: File

Haffey et al. supplementary material 1

Haffey et al. supplementary material
Download Haffey et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 11.3 MB
Supplementary material: File

Haffey et al. supplementary material 2

Haffey et al. supplementary material
Download Haffey et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 20.1 KB