Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-m259h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-27T10:44:08.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gender conflicts in German possessives: comparing inanimate to human reference reveals asymmetries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2025

Christin Schütze*
Affiliation:
Institut für Germanistische Sprachwissenschaft, https://ror.org/01rdrb571 Philipps-Universität Marburg , Marburg, Germany
Jürg Fleischer
Affiliation:
Institut für deutsche Sprache und Linguistik, https://ror.org/01hcx6992 Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin, Germany
Ulrike Domahs
Affiliation:
Institut für Germanistische Sprachwissenschaft, https://ror.org/01rdrb571 Philipps-Universität Marburg , Marburg, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Christin Schütze; Email: christin.schuetze@staff.uni-marburg.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Although German, as a grammatical gender language, requires noun–pronoun agreement in anaphora, exceptions to the rule occur, e.g., in possessive constructions when the gender-incongruent possessive pronoun sein (masculine/neuter, his/its) refers to feminine antecedents instead of congruent ihr (feminine, her). While this violation is merely grammatical for inanimate referents, it can provoke semantic mismatches for human possessors (especially gender-specific female nouns like die Hexei – seini (the witch – his/its), but less so with gender-indifferent human nouns, such as die Kontaktperson (the contact person). A self-paced reading (SPR) experiment tested the acceptability and processing of sentences in which incongruent sein referred to feminine possessors, which differed in animacy status (inanimate versus human). Introducing this agreement violation reduced acceptability and elevated reading and reaction times (RTs), but effects varied by antecedent animacy. These results suggest an animacy restriction in possessive reference and substantiate the impact of meaning-based gender cues on pronominalization.

Information

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. When gender (dis)agrees

‘For every rule, there is an exception’ – and rules of grammar are no exception to this common idiom; otherwise, one cannot explain the plentitude of linguistic cases of doubt (Duden, Reference Duden, Hennig, Osterwinter, Schneider and Steinhauer2016) or the phenomenon of interest here: the existence and possible acceptability of possessive pronouns that fail to agree in gender in German, a language with anaphoric pronominal agreement based on a noun’s gender class. In languages with gender distinctions, pronouns fulfill the task of referent tracking, a crucial requirement to obtain discourse coherence and disambiguation in communication, which makes anaphora interpretation a vital component of language comprehension (Garnham, Reference Garnham2001, 36). Hence, the German possessive pronouns ihr (feminine) and sein (masculine/neuter) must conform to the gender features of their referent to indicate possession of an identified antecedent and maintain syntactic and semantic cohesion and, thus, follow morphological and semantic or pragmatic rules – a functional duality that can cause vast variation, as this paper will explore.

1.1. Possessive anaphoric reference in German

The German gender system assigns one of three values to a noun (feminine, masculine, neuter) and overtly and obligatorily marks this gender category on co-referring expressions like pronouns and other satellite elements (Hellinger & Bußmann, Reference Hellinger, Bußmann, Hellinger and Motschenbacher2015, 7, 13). German requires syntactic gender agreement based on the three grammatical gender categories of all nouns, be they inanimate or animate, but also permits semantic gender agreement based on lexical, referential and/or stereotypical gender inferred for person nouns, as will be exemplified below (Section 1.2). As a consequence, different types of information can control the form of agreement in the human domain: There are two potentially competing routes (sometimes referred to as two available cues, cf. Cacciari et al., Reference Cacciari, Carreiras and Cionini1997, or rules, cf. Oelkers, Reference Oelkers1996, 4) to establish a feature match with human antecedents based on morpho-syntactic grounds or else picking up the underlying lexical or conceptual information to referent gender (Caffarra et al., Reference Caffarra, Janssen and Barber2014). Generally, feature properties (such as gender or number) are ‘inherited’ (Osterhout & Mobley, Reference Osterhout and Mobley1995, 740) from the controller (i.e., possessor noun) to the controllee or target (i.e., possessive pronoun) in Corbett’s (Reference Corbett2006) terminology.

What follows is that the gender features of an anaphoric pronoun are usually predictable from the gender class the noun belongs to, thus imposing syntactic agreement constraints, which we call grammatical or formal gender, e.g., das Kind – seine Eltern (the child (neut.) – its (neut.) parents). In anaphoric reference, successful referent tracking and resolution in discourse is steered by this feature match between the anaphor and the antecedent. While agreement in anaphors to inanimate nouns like die Region – ihr (the region [fem.] – its [fem.]) is very straightforward as it is purely grammatical – here, the formal feature match will be referred to as congruency – anaphoric agreement to animate, human nouns can carry socio-semantic information of referent gender on top of the grammatical feature. The latter surfaces when referring to a child (neut. Kind) of known female or male gender using sie (she [fem.]) or er (he [masc.]) instead of es (it [neut.]) as a pronoun, cf. Birkenes and Fleischer (Reference Birkenes, Fleischer, Diewald and Nübling2022), albeit on Middle High German kint). This type of gender-based agreement will be referred to as convergence with or divergence from referent gender cues of the noun denotation, recurring to Thurmair’s (Reference Thurmair2006) terminology to avoid irritation. If the source of gender agreement is determined on the basis of conceptual properties of the referent rather than formal characteristics of the noun, as in der Sprintstar – ihre Freundinnen (the sprint star [masc.] – her [fem.] female friends, cf. Oelkers, Reference Oelkers1996), we refer to it as referential gender (following Dahl, Reference Dahl, Unterbeck, Rissanen, Nevalainen and Saari2000, 106). Note that grammatical categories are given in abbreviated form in brackets (fem., masc., neut.), while the gender associated with a referent is spelled in full as in gender-specific female, or gender-nonspecific.

1.2. Gender variability in reference processing

Control of (the gender of) a target, like a pronominal element, implies a dependency relation of coreference: in language processing, features are not only compared but potential candidates are checked for coreference based on the matching or mismatching features imposed by the noun reflected on the pronoun (Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2019, 12). Complexities arise because when the comprehension system disentangles this relation, the function of the agreement target, that is a possessive pronoun, can be conceptual pragmatic (Panther, Reference Panther, Brdar-Szabó, Knipf-Komlósi and Péteri2009, 68) in cases like the grammatically neuter, conceptually female (hybrid noun) das Mädchen – ihre Eltern (the girl [neut.] – her [fem.] parents), or morphosyntactic as in das Mädchen – seine Eltern (the girl [neut.] – its [neut.] parents) (see Köpcke et al., Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010, 178, and Hübner, Reference Hübner2021a, Reference Hübner, Binanzer, Gamper and Wecker2021b). Yet, whether semantic convergence or grammatical consistency governs human reference agreement is inconclusive with dozens of studies assessing pronominalization of human anaphors in German: Oelkers (Reference Oelkers1996), Thurmair (Reference Thurmair2006), Panther (Reference Panther, Brdar-Szabó, Knipf-Komlósi and Péteri2009), Köpcke et al. (Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010), Birkenes et al., Reference Birkenes, Chroni and Fleischer2014, Binanzer et al. (Reference Binanzer, Schimke, Schunack, Diewald and Nübling2022), and Hübner (Reference Hübner2021a, Reference Hübner, Binanzer, Gamper and Wecker2021b), among others, investigated under which circumstances and to what extent human antecedents are referred to with (formally) congruent or (conceptually) convergent pronouns, each of them reporting that a gender-based (semantic or pragmatic) pronoun choice often outweighs grammatical cues.

Previous research on how animacy affects grammatical structures like gender agreement has illustrated how this property complicates anaphoric processing by comparing inanimate to animate references. In the context of this paper, we draw on a relatively simple, tripartite version of the Animacy Hierarchy: human > animate > inanimate (Siewierska, Reference Siewierska2004, 46) and center the human–inanimate contrast. Animacy status of an antecedent has repeatedly been reported to modulate the effect of anaphora resolution and to determine the interpretation of different types of pronouns in German (Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Jansma, Lamers and Münte2008; Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 277–280; Bader & Portele, Reference Bader and Portele2025), manifesting that human possessors play a more distinct role for gender agreement phenomena. Most evidently, Köpcke et al. (Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010), 177) illustrated how an animacy switch from an inanimate object to a metonymically denoted (male) human referent determined pronominal reference in die 1. Geige – sein (the first fiddle [fem.] – his [masc.]) / its [neut.]), whereas the instrument is feminine and grammatically only compatible with ihr. Dahl (Reference Dahl, Unterbeck, Rissanen, Nevalainen and Saari2000), 99) therefore stipulated that ‘[grammatical] gender is one of the most obvious places where animacy shows up’, rendering it the most fundamental value distinction of the grammatical gender categorization (Köpcke & Zubin, Reference Köpcke, Zubin and Sieburg1997, 112). Apparently, however, formal agreement in inanimate antecedents is not as irrefutable as one might expect under morpho-syntactic rules: Fleischer (Reference Fleischer2022) registered corpus evidence of numerous incongruent possessive references to inanimate nouns conceived as ‘gender-insensitivity’ of the masculine and neutral pronoun sein, e.g., Qualität hat seinen Preis ‘Quality (fem.) has its (masc./neut.) price’. This observation is backed up by the renowned German grammar Duden (Reference Wöllstein2022), in which the incongruent possessive pronoun sein referring to feminine nouns is listed as a case of doubt, the usage of which is technically inaccurate yet common among (certain) speakers (see regional examples in Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 261). To explain the occasional ignorance of agreement constraints, possessives referring to inanimate possessors are attested as an instance of underspecification regarding gender features (Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 280–283) that ultimately license reference to feminine antecedents with sein where ihr would be prescriptively mandated.

Moreover, the agreement rule applied – grammatical or gender based - may be moderated by the distance between a pronoun and its antecedent noun, as identified in multiple studies (Binanzer et al., Reference Binanzer, Schimke, Schunack, Diewald and Nübling2022; Czech, Reference Czech2014, albeit on relative and personal pronouns; Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Jansma, Lamers and Münte2008; Birkenes et al., Reference Birkenes, Chroni and Fleischer2014, 19; Hübner, Reference Hübner2021a, 13, on personal pronouns; Köpcke et al., Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010, 182–183, on relative and possessive pronouns; Panther, Reference Panther, Brdar-Szabó, Knipf-Komlósi and Péteri2009, 78 on personal, relative and possessive pronouns; Birkenes & Fleischer, Reference Birkenes, Fleischer, Diewald and Nübling2022, 254–257). These studies found a relationship between choice of the agreement pattern and linear syntactic distance such that the probability of exhibiting conceptual gender agreement grew proportionally with the distance between controller and target, meaning that the more constituents separated a pronoun from the noun, the less likely grammatically governed and the more likely conceptually motivated anaphoric pronominalization became. The way this correlation affects different kinds of targets is captured by the Agreement Hierarchy: attributive > predicate > relative pronoun > personal pronoun (Corbett, Reference Corbett2006, 206–237). The implicative hierarchy reliably predicts the (rising) likelihood by which a co-referring expression like a pronoun will be controlled by semantic (meaning based) rather than syntactic (formal-rule-based) agreement. While in this renowned Agreement Hierarchy, possessive pronouns did not explicitly find space, later work places them alongside personal pronouns (Köpcke et al., Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010, 182), demonstrating that they, too, have a strong tendency to follow the controller’s referential gender in agreement relations. In fact, possessive pronouns have been shown to be particularly open for meaning-driven forms of agreement, that is, conceptual gender convergence (Birkenes et al., Reference Birkenes, Chroni and Fleischer2014, 12–13; Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 280; Hübner, Reference Hübner, Binanzer, Gamper and Wecker2021b, 42; Köpcke et al., Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010, 175, 182; Oelkers, Reference Oelkers1996, 10; Panther, Reference Panther, Brdar-Szabó, Knipf-Komlósi and Péteri2009, 76; Thurmair, Reference Thurmair2006, 191, 199–200). The Duden grammar (2022, 434) notes gender-based pronoun choice particularly with somewhat longer distances, yet Panther (Reference Panther, Brdar-Szabó, Knipf-Komlósi and Péteri2009, 79–80) observed semantic being preferred over grammatical agreement for possessive pronouns in particular, even when both elements were in immediate adjacency.

1.3. The present research

The study focuses on grammatically feminine nouns to assess mismatch effects with the grammatically incongruent and potentially gender-divergent masculine/neuter pronoun sein instead of feminine ihr by comparing these anaphors and contrasting characteristics of nouns to which they refer. Our goals were (i) to expand the research on anaphoric reference to possessive pronouns and (ii) to experimentally test the observations of the aforementioned (Section 1.2) corpus data of inanimate agreement deviations (Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022) in an experimental setting.

For this purpose, we constructed sentences containing a grammatically feminine noun phrase (NP) referred to by a possessive pronoun that was either congruent (fem. ihr) or incongruent (masc./neut. sein) with the antecedent’s gender (fem.). The sentences differed in noun types and the distance between the possessor and possessive pronoun. Specifically, we differentiated animacy by contrasting inanimate (die Jahreszeit [the season]) with human (die Zugbegleitung [the train attendant]) possessors. Human antecedents consisted of both gender-specific, typically female referents (die Witwe [the widow]) and gender-nonspecific epicene nouns (die Küchenhilfe [the kitchen help]). This division pertains to social gender information transmitted in human reference, which is not part of the study presented here but a subgroup investigation particularized in Schütze (Reference Schützeaccepted).

Focusing on the role of congruency, animacy and distance in anaphoric relations, the experiment aimed to address

  1. a) how the grammatical features of the antecedent NP would affect sentence acceptability and agreement processing,

  2. b) whether when judging the sentence and in the course of reading, agreement processing is sensitive to the conceptual properties (human/inanimate) of the antecedent and if so,

  3. c) whether this depends on the syntactic position of the antecedent within the sentence (long/short distance).

From Ackerman’s (Reference Ackerman2019, 13) proposal for strict matching in grammatical gender languages, we derive that incongruent sentences fail successful coreference dependency and should mostly be rejected, since φ-features such as person, number, gender of a pronoun and a candidate antecedent are not identical. The evidence on German possessive deviation cited above (Section 1.2), however, casts considerable doubt on the overall rejection expectation. Despite a possibly somewhat diffuse distinction between feminine and masculine/neuter pronouns in inanimate possessive anaphors (cf. Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022), the use of the incongruent form is a grammatical violation expected to be registered as such: We anticipate an agreement restriction reflected in acceptability declines and processing difficulties in the incongruent condition as German has mandatory agreement in gender markings of noun–pronoun anaphors. When the given gender cues are inconsistent with referent gender, we assume that such gender-incongruent possessive pronouns will produce greater processing difficulty compared to congruent ones (Irmen & Kurovskaja, Reference Irmen and Kurovskaja2010, 372). Above all, we expect deviating patterns for inanimate versus human nouns given the additional socio-semantic information that a gendered pronoun carries when referring to humans. In line with the Animacy Hierarchy (Section 1.2), violations in inanimate reference should have a weaker impact, that is higher acceptability and smaller increase in reading time (rt) compared to human referents, such that the availability of both syntactic and semantic agreement cues in human nouns to be checked for agreement may impact processing of the possessives manifested in increased processing costs as reflected in slowed down reading (Esaulova & von Stockhausen, Reference Esaulova, von Stockhausen and Ayoun2022: 56) and reduced acceptability (Irmen & Kurovskaja, Reference Irmen and Kurovskaja2010; Osterhout & Mobley, Reference Osterhout and Mobley1995) for human possessors referred to with the incongruent pronoun, particularly for gender-specific antecedents (when associated referent gender was part of the denotation), as identified in recent work by Schütze (Reference Schützeaccepted).

According to the outlined previous findings, we hypothesize that a lack of syntactic and/or semantic fit between grammatical and referential gender, along with increased possessor–possessee distance, will lead to enhanced processing costs, causing rts to vary depending on sentence configuration. Conversely, an agreement violation effect may be less pronounced in inanimate than human anaphors when distance increases due to information decay over time (Frank, Reference Frank and Frank2019, 92), such that the backward search for antecedents of anaphoric expressions (suggested by Garnham, Reference Garnham2001, 90) would be less elaborate in close pronoun–antecedent proximity, i.e., when less time elapses between activating and re-accessing a referent (Frank, Reference Frank and Frank2019, 92), reducing retrieval effort and yielding facilitation (Niu & Liu, Reference Niu and Liu2022).

We conducted a self-paced reading (SPR) study to assess whether these predictions are borne out in incremental language processing. Participants read sentences online on a computer screen and judged them as grammatically acceptable or not (Schütze, Reference Schütze2016). Following Schütze and Sprouse’s (Reference Schütze, Sprouse, Podesva and Sharma2014, 27–29) terminological notions, we reserve ‘grammaticality’ including the distinction between grammatical and ungrammatical, as an inherent property of prescriptive grammar, and use ‘acceptability’ in connection with a task-dependent, subjective judgment of perceived sentence well formedness. In this dual task paradigm, the offline measure of sentence judgment was combined with the online measure of sentence reading: Acceptability rates reflect a conscious and final decision; response times may reflect sentence-final processing difficulties, but rts are sensitive to interpretations during early comprehension in real time (Garnham, Reference Garnham2001, 61).

Despite extensive research on German gender agreement and animacy interactions (outlined in Section 1.2), there are still gaps in understanding if and how deviant pronouns are processed differently for human versus inanimate referents. To date, the comprehension of possessive pronouns referring to inanimate or human role nouns has not been investigated experimentally in German. Since there is variation of or choice between genders – as attested for personal, demonstrative and relative pronouns (in German, see Section 1.2; cross-linguistically, see Audring, Reference Audring2008) – there is ample reason to expect variation for possessive pronouns, too (as observed by Köpcke et al., Reference Köpcke, Panther, Zubin, Schmid and Handl2010; Oelkers, Reference Oelkers1996; Thurmair, Reference Thurmair2006).

2. Experimental study

In the experiment, we tested whether gender-incongruent possessive pronouns impact sentence acceptability, response time and rt, and if so, whether this depends on the possessor noun’s animacy status and its distance from the anaphoric pronoun. The complete dataset, including stimuli, analysis script and model output, is available at https://osf.io/g9kbt/.

2.1. Method

The experiment used a combined SPR and sentence acceptability judgment (AJ) method similar to Irmen and Kurovskaja’s (Reference Irmen and Kurovskaja2010) procedure, in which sentences were presented phrase by phrase at participants’ own pace (rather than word by word, improving the task by limiting spillover to later regions, cf. Mitchell, Reference Mitchell, Kieras and Just1984: 74–76). In non-cumulative SPR as used in this study, a sentence starts with an initial word or phrase, and participants press a key to reveal each successive sentence fragment upon which the previous phrase disappears, until the entire sentence has been displayed. The time between two key presses indicates the rt of a phrase (e.g., Jegerski, Reference Jegerski, Jegerski and Van Patten2014; Mitchell, Reference Mitchell, Kieras and Just1984). Two of the major advantages of SPR are that (i) it provides a time-sensitive, precise measure of sentence reading and that (ii) it can be easily run online with many participants using personal devices. Sentence judgments were binary classifications instead of scalar or gradient responses (unlike Irmen and Kurovskaja’s (Reference Irmen and Kurovskaja2010) implementation, but see Bader and Häussler (Reference Bader and Häussler2010) on how these measures relate).

The study was created with Experiment Builder in GORILLA and hosted on their platform (Anwyl-Irvine et al., Reference Anwyl-Irvine, Massonnié, Flitton, Kirkham and Evershed2020; Cauldron Science, 2022), a licensed program to combine questionnaires and experiments with reliable precision (Bridges et al., Reference Bridges, Pitiot, MacAskill and Peirce2020) to be run online and distributed over the crowdsourcing web platform Prolific (Prolific, 2014; Peer et al., Reference Peer, Brandimarte, Samat and Acquisti2017) in August 2023. Data collection with these services was liable to charges.

2.1.1. Participants

We recruited 96 native speakers of German through the Prolific participant pool in 2022, who were not necessarily currently living in a German-speaking country. Thirty five women, 55 men, one participant identifying as diverse, and five participants with no indication of gender identity between 19 and 62 years (M Age = 32.38, SD Age = 10.0), representing various economic backgrounds, took part in the experiment and were compensated for participation.

2.1.2. Design

The experiment followed a balanced 2 (congruency: congruent versus incongruent) × 2 (animacy: inanimate versus human) × 2 (distance: short versus long) design. Participants were presented with each condition: By varying congruency and distance configurations, we generated four different versions of each sentence per animacy noun types, which each participant read in one condition. Four item lists resulted from this, to which participants were randomly and equally assigned.

Acceptability was manipulated via sentence grammaticality tied to the pronoun’s gender inflection, which either agreed (fem. Ihr [her/its]) or disagreed (masc./neut. Sein [his/its]) formally with the preceding feminine controller noun. In the congruent versions (Table 1, 1–2 with ihr), the gender of the possessor and pronoun were in concord (feminine), leading to a felicitous sentence (Table 1). The incongruent reference with sein to inanimate antecedents (1) violated formal gender agreement, while the incongruent pronominalization of a human female referent (Section 2.2) with sein constituted both a grammatical and a semantic violation. Only the incongruent (sein pronoun) version of human epicene referents (Section 2.1) could possibly receive a conceptually plausible interpretation as converging with the referent gender.

Table 1. Example sentences for inanimate (1) and human (2) possessors in short (a) and long (b) distances between possessive pronoun and noun antecedent referred to with the congruent or incongruent pronoun

To test the influence of the distance between antecedent noun and pronoun, an adverbial was inserted between possessor and possessee, which yielded (a) short (two-word) or (b) long (five-word) distance between these units. Therefore, the position of the critical phrase containing the congruent or incongruent pronoun varied in sentences with short (phrase 5) and long distance (phrase 6), cf. 1–2a versus 1–2b in Table 1. Sentence content remained unchanged, and sentence structure was held constant across all critical items. We further controlled sentence length, fitting all sentences into the scheme of seven phrases consisting of 10–15 words (see Table 1, phrases separated by vertical bars) and controlling the number of words per phrase across items (though we were less restrictive in the sentence-initial and -final fragments).

2.2. Materials

All selected nouns were definite, feminine and singular, which surfaced in the preceding feminine article die so that anaphoric (masc./neut) sein would lead to a feature mismatch in (at least) grammatical gender. Note that possessive reference to plural nouns in German, also preceded by die, invariably occurs as ihre, overlapping with the singular feminine form, which is why we were cautious to refrain from conceptually plural possessors (see Schütze [Reference Schützeaccepted] on this matter).

Inanimate nouns were mainly drawn from the various corpus examples found by Fleischer (Reference Fleischer2022). Human nouns were added in two subsets of equal size: The gender-specific, typically female-referring feminine nouns were kinship terms (Patentante (godmother)), fabulous or mythical creatures (Hexe [witch]) as well as occupations (Hebamme [midwife]), all of which carry the lexical gender feature [+female] without being overtly marked female-inflected denotations (i.e., through –in-suffixation as in Lehrerin (teacher, female) – except for the less transparent Cousine (cousin, female). The gender-nonspecific epicene nouns were mainly occupations (Teamleitung [team leader, gender-indifferent]), positions (Majestät [majesty]) and a few role nouns (Geisel [hostage]); for a similar distinction, see Kreiner et al. (Reference Kreiner, Sturt and Garrod2008: 247). Other constraints imposed on the possessive phrases – besides the manipulations outlined in Section 2.1.2 – included varied possessee noun gender (groups of fem. and masc./neut. nouns in close to parity to address potential issues of agreement attraction involving ihr or sein, respectively), number features (comparable proportions of singular and plural nouns) and word structure (both simple and compound nouns).

In total, participants evaluated 180 sentence items: 60 critical items containing possessives (30 inanimate, 30 human nominal referents, of which 15 epicenes and 15 female specific), 60 distractor items and 60 fillers. Filler sentences were well formed, whereas distractor sentences were equipped with grammatical inconsistencies of various kinds as well as ‘cases of doubt’ and borderline cases of ungrammaticality to obscure the study focus. The Duden dictionary of German cases of doubt (Duden, Reference Duden, Hennig, Osterwinter, Schneider and Steinhauer2016) lists incongruent sein references to feminine antecedents as one instance of such cases, and we drew numerous examples from this rich source of inspiration to create distractor sentences. See Supplemental Material (https://osf.io/g9kbt/) for further details on stimulus selection.

2.3. Procedure

At the start of the experiment, participants gave informed consent. They then received detailed instructions for the task: They would read sentences split up into phrases and could control the presentation speed themselves. Their first task was to rate the sentence by indicating whether they would accept the sentence as grammatically correct or not. Emphasis was put on the information that this judgment was not about the content of the sentence. The second task was explained as a test of participants’ reading for comprehension that ensured they were engaged in attentive reading throughout, requiring them to respond to ‘yes’/‘no’ questions about the content, with instant feedback (green tick mark or red cross). Participants were instructed to decide as quickly as possible and, if in doubt, rely on their initial intuition, as there would be a time limit. Instructions were followed by a practice block of five items to familiarize with the task procedure before proceeding to the main task. Before each new trial, a black fixation cross appeared in the middle of a gray background on the screen for 250 ms, followed by a 550 ms pause before sentence presentation of the initial to the last phrase, displayed upon the key press centered on the screen. Participants advanced through the seven sentence slides at their own pace by pressing the space bar to reveal phrases incrementally. The next key presses after sentence offset presented an acceptance prompt and, when applicable (after one third of the items), an additional comprehension question (CQ). The binary judgment of the sentence’s acceptability was requested by a large ‘?’ with the response options ‘accept’ or ‘reject’. Within 5000 ms (small countdown icon shown on 3000 ms, prompting timely decisions before a failure to respond; for a similar duration, see De Vogelaer et al., Reference De Vogelaer, Fanta, Poarch, Schimke, Urbanek, De Vogelaer, Koster and Leuschner2020), participants had to come to a decision by pressing the defined keys. CQs were to be answered with ‘yes’ or ‘no’ using the same keys within 7000 ms, but these prompts never targeted the possessive dependency. Assignment of response keys was counterbalanced across participants to control for handedness, yielding two versions. When participants were ready for a new sentence, they pressed the space bar to begin an intertrial interval of 500 ms blank screen.

Items were presented in pseudo-randomized order and arranged in five blocks of 36 sentence items each (12 of each item type, critical items evenly split between inanimate and human referents). Short breaks were allowed in between blocks to rest or move briefly, but participants had been informed of a maximum completion time of 60 minutes. A progress bar was displayed on screen throughout. At the beginning and after half of the blocks, an attention check required pressing a nonresponse key. After completing all blocks, having read and rated all items, participants were asked for additional demographic information and debriefed for feedback on the experiment, particularly their assumptions about the study focus. To successfully finish participation and receive payment, they were redirected to Prolific. The entire experiment took 30–45 minutes on average.

2.4. Data analysis and preprocessing

The dependent variables of interest, i.e., the measurements to be analyzed, were (1) the proportion of participants’ AJ(in percentages), (2) the response reaction time (RT) needed to either accept or reject a sentence and (3) the rt of the critical phrase, that is, the sentence fragment containing the possessive (both in ms).

In this setting, the rt per phrase was measured as the duration it remained visible on screen before the key to proceed is pressed. Analyses were performed using the statistical software R (R Core Team, 2023) in RStudio (RStudio Team, 2024). Prior model comparisons relied on Akaike information criterion(AIC) values for goodness of fit (Gries, Reference Gries2021, 366–370), the step function (lme4 package, Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) for stepwise model complexity comparisons and the predictor strength function of the SfL package (Schmitz & Esser, Reference Schmitz and Esser2021). Numeric predictors (i.e., trial number, graphemic length and Zipf frequency of nouns, cf. Supplemental Material) were centered and z-scaled. Data manipulation applied tidyverse-style syntax (Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François, Grolemund, Hayes, Henry, Hester, Kuhn, Pedersen, Miller, Bache, Müller, Ooms, Robinson, Seidel, Spinu and Yutani2019). Post-hoc comparisons of effects were conducted with Tukey-adjusted pairwise tests for multiple comparisons using the contrast function of the emmeans package (Lenth et al., Reference Lenth, Banfai, Bolker, Buerkner, Giné-Vázquez, Herve, Jung, Love, Miguez, Piaskowski, Riebl and Singmann2024).

Using a fixed threshold, acceptability judgment responses faster than 200 ms and slower than 3500 ms were excluded from the analysis as these are prone to reflect inattentiveness, erroneous or unintentional key presses (Baayen & Milin, Reference Baayen and Milin2010, 15; Jegerski, Reference Jegerski, Jegerski and Van Patten2014, 20). Similarly, only rts above 50 ms and below 1500 ms per phrase were included in data analyses. As a further step, outliers – reaction and rts exceeding 3 standard deviations (SDs) from individual means – were computed and discarded from the dataset (2.83% of response data and 2.43% of rt data; for data trimming, see Baayen & Milin, Reference Baayen and Milin2010, 15–17; Jegerski, Reference Jegerski, Jegerski and Van Patten2014, 21; Whelan, Reference Whelan2008, 477). Due to right-skewed distributions of RT data, log-transformed reaction and rts were analyzed instead (Baayen & Milin, Reference Baayen and Milin2010). We operationalized participant attentiveness as the ratio of CQ accuracy, retaining only datasets from participants who scored 75% or more correct, which all participants achieved (M CQ = 93.6%, SD CQ = 0.24, M RT. CQ = 1688 ms, SD RT.CQ = 740). Besides this accuracy check for eligibility, strategic nonreflective response behavior was screened to ensure that the level of acceptability was neither 0% nor 100% throughout the experiment, verifying no participant consistently accepted or rejected sentences irrespective of stimulus type, that is grammatical inconsistencies, or inattentively. Since participants are referred to Prolific upon successful completion, any incomplete participations were already excluded from the final dataset on the platform.

3. Results

3.1. Acceptability judgments

First, we report results of the analyzed proportion and speed of sentence acceptability as judged by the participants, that is, responses and RTs for the critical sentences with possessives (ihr/sein).

3.1.1. Rating responses

We fitted a generalized mixed-effects model using glmer with a logit link (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015) to predict the binary decision of sentence acceptability, i.e., response (to the AJ task) with response as the dependent variable and an interaction of the factors congruency, distance and animacy, allowing for random intercepts for trial to account for adapted response behavior in the course of the session, as well as for the different noun items and for subjects to account for any individual sensitivity to the agreement phenomenon studied here.Footnote 1

Overall, the sentences with possessive anaphoric pronouns had a mean acceptability rating of 64.9% (SD = 0.48). However, sentences with congruent ihr anaphors were judged twice as acceptable (M = 86.9%, SD = 0.34) as incongruent sein pronominalizations (M = 42.7%, SD = 0.50) as depicted in Figure 1; this negative effect of incongruency on acceptability ratings being highly significant (β = −3.49, SE = 0.18, z = −19.82, p < .001). Surprisingly, even the grammatically congruent inanimate items (M = 83.0%, SD = 0.38) were judged as 8% less acceptable than human anaphors (M = 90.8%, SD = 0.29, depicted in the left panel of Figure 1; β = −1.07, SE = 0.22, z = −4.83, p < .001). Looking further into the animacy distinction, a significant interaction between incongruent and inanimate conditions (β = 1.63, SE = 0.22, z = 7.42, p < .001) revealed a reverse pattern with incongruent possessive pronouns as there was a clear dominance of accepted sein references to inanimate (M = 49.2%, SD = 0.50) over human referents (M = 36.2%, SD = 0.48, a difference visible in the right panel of Figure 1), as confirmed in post-hoc tests (est = 0.53, SE = 0.80, z = −4.23, p < .001). Linear syntactic distance between noun antecedent and pronoun had a significant impact on sentence acceptability: A shorter distance between possessor noun and possessive pronoun was found to be slightly more acceptable (M = 65.6%, SD = 0.48) than longer distance between these anaphoric units (M = 64.2%, SD = 0.48; β = −0.55, SE = 0.20, z = −2.80, p = .005). Interestingly, this became more pronounced when taking the animacy status of the possessive reference into account: Resolving the significant interaction between animacy (inanimate) and distance (β = 0.52, SE = 0.25, z = 2.13, p = 0.03), the negative impact of inanimacy on the judgment responses became less severe as distance increased, but this trend was not supported in post-hoc comparisons where the acceptability rates were comparable (n.s.) for inanimate nouns irrespective of distance. On the contrary, human antecedents were accepted significantly more often in shorter (M = 65.2%, SD = 0.48) than longer distances (M = 61.9%, SD = 0.49); the improved sentence acceptability was also reflected in post-hoc contrasts (est = 0.32, SE = 0.11, z = 2.84, p = 0.025).

Figure 1. Mean acceptability ratings (in %) for congruent (ihr) and incongruent (sein) sentences with human (no pattern) or inanimate (striped) feminine antecedents; error bars represent SD.

For sentence AJs, incongruent sein was by far less accepted than congruent ihr, but asymmetries were observed between human and inanimate possessors referred to with the incongruent possessive pronoun. Taken together, agreement violations of possessive references were tolerated more for inanimate than human nouns. In contrast, incongruent possessive pronouns referring to human nouns, especially at a longer distance, reduced sentence acceptability profoundly (Figure 1).

3.1.2. Reaction times

When analyzing RTs for AJ, the sentence-final decision as (non-)acceptable is likely to influence the time to come to this decision. A linear mixed-effects model was fitted using lme4 (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), with log-transformed RT (logRT) as the dependent variable to predict RT of the acceptability response as a function of the conditions – similar to the AJ analysis – modeling the interaction of congruency, animacy, distance and the response to the AJ while accounting for random intercepts as before.Footnote 2

Overall, response to the AJ had a significant influence as decision times decreased when a sentence was accepted (M = 687 ms, SD = 408) compared to the longer RT when a sentence was rejected (M = 749 ms, SD = 450; β = 0.17, SE = 0.06, t = 2.70, p = .007). However, this effect was reversed in interaction with congruency (or rather lack thereof), when references were incongruent instead of congruent: Rejecting a sentence with an incongruent sein pronoun led to faster response times (M = 716 ms, SD = 424) than for a congruent ihr pronoun (M = 891 ms, SD = 525; β = −0.21, SE = 0.07, t = −2.88 p = .004), whereas the decision to accept resulted in amplified RTs for incongruent (M = 706 ms, SD = 408) compared to congruent anaphors (M = 678 ms, SD = 408), an observation found to be significant in post-hoc tests (est = −0.05, SE = 0.02, z = −2.9, p = 0.02). Unsurprisingly, participants were faster with ‘“match’ decisions (accepting congruency, rejecting incongruency) than in ‘mismatch’ cases. There were no main effects of congruency, animacy or distance on response times.

To sum up, rejection responses required longer decision times than judging sentences as acceptable, especially so for the grammatically correct, congruent pronouns (Figure 2).

Figure 2. Mean RTs (in ms) for congruent (ihr) and incongruent (sein) sentences in short and long distances with human (no pattern) or inanimate (striped) feminine antecedents for sentence approvals (left panel) and rejections (right panel).

3.2. Self-paced reading

Next, we report results of the analysis of reading speed as a measure of participants’ progress through the anaphoric unit containing the possessive pronoun and, thereby, a proxy to referential processing. For rt analyses, we kept both outcomes of AJ responses since the decision (whether to approve or dismiss) can affect how the sentence is read, that is, due to early intuitions about pronoun agreement (e.g., double mismatches like in die Nonne – sein [the nun – his]) or because the decision might be forming while or after reading the entire sentence to resolve the reference successfully (early versus late commitment, discussed in Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Holler and Kidd2007 and below in Section 4.1).

3.2.1. Reading times

Mean rts for post-antecedent NP regions are shown in Figure 3; the critical phrase containing the possessive pronoun is framed in a grey shade. As is immediately evident, both human and inanimate sentences with incongruent possessives (dashed lines with triangles) generally produced large, immediate effects strongly localized to the pronoun (with some delay also spilling over one phrase further downstream in the short distance condition, cf. left panel of Figure 3), compared to congruent sentences (solid lines connected with dots), which do not. rt differences at the possessive phrase (Figure 4) illustrate how incongruent anaphors clearly prolonged continuing to the next phrase, that is, reading the subsequent phrase (next two to five words after the critical possessor noun), both in short and long distance versions.

Figure 3. Mean reading times following a human (left side of the panels) or an inanimate antecedent (right side of the panels) through sentences with a short (left panel) or long distance (right panel) between the noun and the anaphoric pronoun. The possessive phrase containing congruent ihr or incongruent sein pronouns is shaded in grey. Dots indicate the congruent condition; triangles the incongruent.

Figure 4. Mean reading times at the possessive phrase containing congruent ihr or incongruent sein pronouns with human (solid line) or inanimate (dashed line) antecedents in short (left panel) or long distance (right panel) between noun and anaphoric pronoun. Dots indicate the congruent condition; triangles the incongruent; shades represent 95% CIs based on the standard error.

A linear mixed-effects model was fitted to predict log-transformed rts (logrt) using lme4 (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), with an interaction of congruency and animacy, distance (namely pronoun position) and as a function of the response (to AJ task, ‘accept’/‘reject’), trial as well as item-specific measures influencing rts (Zipf frequency and graphemic length of possessee nouns) and random intercepts for nouns (items) and subjects. The model further allowed for individual differences in AJ response by participant with random slopes to account for the varying judgment behavior, i.e., the observed group differences among participants (elaborated in Section 4.4).Footnote 3

Within this model, a main effect of congruency emerged signaling that incongruent possessive pronoun phrases were read more slowly than congruent ones (M = 632 ms, SD = 291 versus M = 675 ms, SD = 323), the effect approaching significance (β = 0.04, SE = 0.02, t = 2.02, p = 0.04). Subsequent post-hoc tests for interactions showed that the slowdown imposed by incongruent sein pronouns was significant for both human and inanimate antecedents (more than 50 ms increase from congruent human (M = 611 ms, SD = 272) to incongruent human anaphors (M = 663 ms, SD = 310; est = −0.04, SE = 0.02, t = −2.61, p = 0.04); inanimates had a smaller difference of 34 ms between congruent (M = 653 ms, SD = 308) versus incongruent sentences (M = 687 ms, SD = 335); est = −0.04 for, SE = 0.01, t = −2.67, p = 0.03)).

In the short distance condition, when possessor and possessee were two words apart, incongruent anaphors slowed down rts at the possessive phrase by nearly 50 ms (M = 677 ms, SD = 324) compared to congruent ones (M = 628 ms, SD = 294), and post-hoc tests point at a significant congruency difference therein (est = −0.06, SE = 0.02, t = −3.47, p = .003), whereas no effect was found in the long distance. A distance effect occurred only for inanimate, not for human, anaphors. For the former, rts were faster with two words than five words before the possessive pronoun, the post-hoc contrast tending toward marginal significance (est = −0.04, SE = 0.013, t = −2.67, p = 0.04). Moreover, post-hoc comparisons for interactions with the given response revealed that whenever a sentence was subsequently rejected, rts were significantly higher (for ‘accepted’ cases: M = 630 ms, SD = 281, for ‘rejected’ ones: M = 697 ms, SD = 348; est = −0.04, SE = 0.013, t = −3.275, p = .001), hinting at the correlation between the outcome of sentence judgments and reading speed, which can either be interpreted as being hampered by earlier processes during reading or as a penalty for sentences with an augmented processing cost.

4. Discussion

While corpora can ‘record grammatical knowledge as realized in language production’, judgments provide intuitive information about such knowledge while rts tap into parsing processes during language comprehension in real time; they thus ‘complement each other’ methodologically (Myers, Reference Myers2009, 413). What has been found in corpus data by Fleischer (Reference Fleischer2022) can be confirmed experimentally too: There is unexpectedly much variability in the pronominal gender assignment of German possessive constructions. Audring and Booij (Reference Audring and Booij2009, 13) interpret this as speakers’ ‘uncertainty’, ‘a source of language variation and change’. From our perspective, a certain diffusion is to be restricted to the grammatical agreement of pronouns with inanimate possessors. The variability exhibited by human possessors, we believe, is rooted in the socio-semantic agreement with referent gender indexed by pronouns. While some cases may be borderline, and some speakers may have less certainty about linguistic norms, based on our evidence, we are inclined to follow the claims that a mental representation of possessive relations cannot be built from grammatical structures alone, but draws on extra-linguistic knowledge and discourse information, which has formed cognitive accounts of situational models (Zwaan et al., Reference Zwaan, Radvansky, Hilliard and Curiel1998) or the mental model theory (Garnham, Reference Garnham2001).

From the results, we infer that readers attend to both syntactic and socio-semantic cues, i.e., rely on grammatical rules and real-world knowledge about likely referents. Processing involves a combination of both (Cacciari et al., Reference Cacciari, Carreiras and Cionini1997; Caffarra et al., Reference Caffarra, Janssen and Barber2014; Kennison, Reference Kennison2003), because in the reading and judgment experiment, referent characteristics (human versus inanimate) and agreement (congruent versus incongruent) have led to asymmetrical effects.

Our experiment pinpointed slowdowns on the specific part of the sentence containing the possessive, and the incremental method used – albeit admittedly a more artificial reading than, for example, full sentence presentation – thus qualifies as a reliable experimental presentation technique.

4.1. Possessive agreement

In each of the analyses, pronoun agreement significantly affected the outcome and speed of the sentence judgment decision, as well as the reading of possessive anaphors. By contrasting grammatically congruent ihr with incongruent sein pronouns referring to feminine antecedent nouns, we were able to confirm that readers do detect these feature mismatches (Carreiras et al., Reference Carreiras, Garnham, Oakhill and Cain1996; Osterhout & Mobley, Reference Osterhout and Mobley1995) but nevertheless may come to an acceptability decision of such sentences in German. We further found differential incongruity effects because violating the gender feature of the possessive pronoun was less severe for inanimate nouns and in long antecedent–pronoun distance than for human nouns and in short distance. The factorial design helped disentangle what supports and what hinders tolerance of a possessive pronoun that does not agree with its antecedent gender features: Evidently, processing difficulty for a referring expression such as an anaphoric possessive pronoun seems to be sensitive to the animacy status of the antecedent and, under specific circumstances, the distance between the possessive relation, which will be addressed in the next sections, respectively.

Although we observed a clear acceptability advantage and acceleration for congruent as compared to incongruent pronominalizations, grammatical gender features appear less binding than expected. These results can be taken as evidence for a ‘blurred’ gender distinction (De Vogelaer et al., Reference De Vogelaer, Fanta, Poarch, Schimke, Urbanek, De Vogelaer, Koster and Leuschner2020, 288; similarly discussed in Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 280–283) in German, in which sein assumably acts as the unmarked, underspecified form (Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 283; Oppenrieder & Thurmair, Reference Oppenrieder and Thurmair2002, 119). Importantly, ‘underspecified’ does not mean ‘completely unresolved’ (Karimi & Ferreira, Reference Karimi and Ferreira2016, 1014): Even though their acceptability was surprisingly high, incongruent possessive pronouns clearly magnified rts and thus hampered online comprehension. Referential processing was not delayed until the decision-making with the task at hand; rather competition occurred at the pronoun during reading and was potentially revised when judging the sentences. Given the missing effects of animacy in rts but not AJs, we infer that the final pronominal integration of possessives can be delayed until the complete sentence has been read (Garnham, Reference Garnham2001, 83), after which filters for candidates of the best fit can be applied (Chow et al., Reference Chow, Lewis and Phillips2014), resulting in approval or disapproval. Interestingly, some of the factors we investigated – aside from congruency – affected only AJs, that is, the conscious decision after having read the sentence, but not on the immediate measures during reading. This finding alludes to a late commitment to reference relations when all available information has been parsed (in full support of Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Holler and Kidd2007).

We have unveiled that even in a grammatical gender language with a pervasive morphological marking like German, in an anaphoric sentence, the constraint imposed by a morphosyntactic feature alone is neither ‘primarily relevant’ (Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2019, 9) nor does it suffice to provide unambiguous cues as to pronoun gender, or else incongruent sein anaphors referring to feminine nouns should have been disapproved entirely, in particular for inanimate antecedents for which agreement should be completely formal.

4.2. Animacy-based sources of gender variation

Animacy is deemed a pivotal distinctive trait in language, resulting in ‘morphological splits based on animacy and humanness’ (Ortmann, Reference Ortmann, Fabri, Ortmann and Parodi1998, 83). Indeed, our study revealed the influence of antecedent animacy on sentence acceptability. Ratings of sentences with incongruent sein instead of congruent ihr anaphors were lower overall, but the frequent insensitivity to the grammatical violation in inanimates was striking. As expected, these appeared far more acceptable under incongruent sein reference than human antecedents.

There is evidence on how inanimate entities are more often referred to with neutral (i.e., it), less specific or default forms, rather than specific pronouns (Sorlin & Gardelle, Reference Sorlin and Gardelle2018), which could have boosted the acceptability of masculine/neuter sein for inanimates, but due to the syncretism, this cannot be definitely determined. At the same time, referring back to previously activated inanimate possessors was associated with costs, observable in the considerable increase in rts. Together, this suggests a restriction of possessive constructions, even when used grammatically correctly, which leads us to the impression that prototypical possessors are animate; inanimate possessors, though grammatically absolutely viable, are obviously much less common in natural language. Two assumptions can explain the observed asymmetries depending on the animacy status of the possessor noun: Either the use of ‘possessive’ pronouns may be unusual with inanimate nouns (consistent with prior findings of fewer productions of pronouns for inanimate referents, see Exp. 1 from Fukumura & van Gompel, Reference Fukumura and van Gompel2011, as well as with research on variation of genitive attributes with inanimates, see Kopf & Bildhauer, Reference Kopf and Bildhauer2024), or the gender feature loses activation much faster in nonhuman contexts, making it more difficult to maintain hence more effortful to reactivate the discourse reference.

The latter intriguing line of thought, aligning with our findings, draws on a cognitive account of activation and prominence. Anaphoric reference as a coherence device requires to i) create a mental discourse representation in which two entities must be related meaningfully, and ii) keep the introduced referent(s) activated until the coreference with further expressions has successfully been established (Garnham, Reference Garnham2001). It has been argued that cognitive activation, perceptual prominence and discursive relevance are greater for human entities as they are most likely agents of events (Bader et al., Reference Bader, Torregrossa and Rinke2023, 669), carry language-external gender-based meanings (Kalverkämper, 1979, 63 cited in Leiss, Reference Leiss and Sieburg1997, 326) and are more concrete (thus easier to visualize) given their identifying characteristics (such as [+human], [−young], [+teaching] for Lehrkraft [teaching person]), which makes personal nouns more relevant to everyday life of us as social beings. In contrast, referential processes involving inanimates underlie syntactic automatisms (Kalverkämper, 1979, 63 in Leiss, Reference Leiss and Sieburg1997, 326), hence receiving less memory activation (Fukumura & van Gompel, Reference Fukumura and van Gompel2011), ultimately leading to declining levels of activation of an inanimate controller in language comprehension (Köpcke & Zubin, Reference Köpcke, Zubin, Hentschel and Vogel2009: 142) that come with lower salience (Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Jansma, Lamers and Münte2008) namely lower cognitive prominence and, therefore, accelerated decay (Frank, Reference Frank and Frank2019: 103). Such less enhanced processing depth has also been described as shallow processing or a shallow commitment to the initially considered referent (Karimi & Ferreira, Reference Karimi and Ferreira2016: 1024). Shallow processing of inanimate possessives, however, is challenged by our findings as incongruent inanimate references were not processed at the same speed as those with congruent pronouns. Furthermore, the rt penalty for inanimate referred to with sein conflicts with an account of gender insensitivity (Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022). Likewise, this selected gender insensitivity phenomenon is not a case of a ‘good enough’ representation (Karimi & Ferreira, Reference Karimi and Ferreira2016) either, since rts were clearly negatively affected by the deviating pronoun referring to inanimate nouns regardless of a later positive judgment of a sentence with incongruent sein. A more heretic interpretation of the high incongruency acceptability results overall is to claim that grammatical gender information is simply not relevant enough to affect comprehension. If that were the case, however, AJ results among human and inanimate anaphors should be comparable, which is not corroborated. To explain this discrepancy, another, additional, notion of grammatical gender must be assumed – a function it takes over on top of syntactic agreement, which leads us to aspects of social meaning in the human domain. Reconsider that the nature of third-person pronouns lies in the identification of an antecedent indicated by the pronoun. When comparing sentences with a potentially gendered subject (human nouns, e.g., Stellvertretung [deputy]) with those that have purely grammatical gender (inanimate nouns, e.g., Jahreszeit [season]), we essentially compare whether conceptual relevance complements grammatical information or not.

Among human referents, some candidates may arrive at a better socio-semantic fit with the masculine/neuter sein pronoun than others because of a referential gender convergence whereby activated gendered knowledge or inferred stereotypical associations (Irmen et al., Reference Irmen, Holt and Weisbrod2010; Misersky et al., Reference Misersky, Majid and Snijders2019) can override syntactic cues (Cacciari et al., Reference Cacciari, Carreiras and Cionini1997; Caffarra et al., Reference Caffarra, Janssen and Barber2014). Although the differentiation between epicene and typically female referents within human possessors could in principle affect the overall influence of the factor animacy with respect to human antecedents; the results confirm that the animacy effect was robust. We know from the subgroup analysis that incongruent possessive pronouns with epicenes are more accepted, suggesting elevated sein-acceptance under human reference, but not completely discarded for female referents either. Comparing gender-nonspecific to gender-specific human reference in the in-depth analysis it deserves goes beyond the scope of this article, but is covered by Schütze (Reference Schützeaccepted) in broader detail.

The gender of possessives may thus function as a (partly negligible) grammatical category in inanimate anaphors and as an additional gender-classifying feature in animates, especially human nouns. Given the different ways to establish agreement, different reasons for pronoun variation might collude as an interplay of multiple factors in reference processing. Whether these engage different cognitive processes, whether the source of irritation is grammatical or semantic or both, and whether processes differ for each type of antecedent such that different (timings of) components are at work (as reported by Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Jansma, Lamers and Münte2005, 236) remains a question open for future investigation.

4.3. Dependency distance

In our study, an increase of intervening words from two to five between the possessor and possessive pronoun resulted in a small yet significant decline in acceptability ratings. Still, the role of linear distance between an antecedent and its pronoun remains somewhat vague: Against our predictions, distance did not affect acceptability ratings for inanimate or human nouns as expected. In both cases, sentence judgments were not more positive when the distance was longer. This outcome can be attributed to the amount of the intervening material chosen for the present experiment, i.e., two versus five words, for which we opted to leverage German word order flexibility while preserving adequate sentence length for a reading task already challenging the comprehension system. In this exact range, Panther (Reference Panther, Brdar-Szabó, Knipf-Komlósi and Péteri2009, 80) reported small, if any, differences in pronominal references (in conceptual rather than grammatical agreement with Mädchen (girl [neut.]) – in stark contrast to the clear increase from zero to two words distance to antecedents. Kennison (Reference Kennison2003) noted how pronouns embedded ‘in more complex syntactic structures were processed more slowly’, and that possessive pronouns might be processed more slowly in general compared to the more frequent personal pronouns, inferring that different syntactic configurations may come with a different salience or expectancy of a possessive. For some items in the present experiment, the word order resulting from greater distance configurations, farther separating the subject from the object, is less typical for German than for other items. In this concern, the grammatical structure of the stimulus sentences used here may have affected anaphoric processing too.

4.4. Further factors and avenues

With the present experiment, it became evident that among German speakers, ‘a remarkable tolerance is observed toward combinations that are rare in spontaneous production’ (De Vogelaer et al., Reference De Vogelaer, Fanta, Poarch, Schimke, Urbanek, De Vogelaer, Koster and Leuschner2020, 291, though on Dutch data). The strict matching criterion dictates a rejection of a coreference dependency when features do not match and obey the ‘identity relation’ between φ-features (Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2019, 13, 14, cf. Section 1.3). In this respect, and in the context of the pronominal anaphors examined here, our findings do not conclusively support the claim that German consistently and solely ‘access[es] only the [grammatical] feature[s] during coreference resolution’ (Ackerman, Reference Ackerman2019, 19). Below, we discuss an alternative explanation to the unfulfilled rejection hypothesis, namely that readers have overly and readily accepted sentences after diverting to a non-coreferential interpretation, in which feature identity would no longer be necessary.

Given our design – obstructing regressions to previous words and using isolated single-referent sentences with limited context – one could argue that the possessive pronoun’s deictic function to indicate discourse units may not necessarily rule out other antecedents if the gender feature does not match that of the given candidate(s). Instead, if a pronoun cannot be linked to – and cannot be reinspected for (in)congruency with – the only explicitly mentioned antecedent in the presence of nonmatching features, they may further trigger antecedent search processes as a repair strategy to establish coreference to some other discourse referent than the possessor when the parser encounters a lack of satisfactory candidates (Chow et al., Reference Chow, Lewis and Phillips2014). This offers another path to resolve a gender-deviant pronoun: ‘as coreferential with the available antecedent and ungrammatical, or as linking to an unmentioned referent and grammatical’, a coping mechanism which has been attested for ‘particularly skilled readers, [who] may come up with an additional, unmentioned referent’ (Piepers & Redl, Reference Piepers and Redl2018, 98). Rather than relying solely on the gender information cued by the pronoun in an attempt to parse an ungrammatical construction, an alternative explanation is that participants may read the sentences without successfully establishing a coreference relation between the pronoun and the given antecedent (as proposed by van Gompel & Liversedge, Reference van Gompel and Liversedge2003: 7). In fact, shifting to a sentence or discourse external, competing antecedent to integrate an unexpected pronoun could explain the frequent acceptance of incongruent sentences despite the formal agreement violation as well as the amplified rts at the possessive phrase, as this strategy would be costly (Chow et al., Reference Chow, Lewis and Phillips2014, 3). However, such interpretation would have to account for the observed animacy effect. Moreover, Karimi and Ferreira (Reference Karimi and Ferreira2016, 1017) note that, even if feature mismatches pose a risk to impede successful reference resolution, the referent of a referring expression is still reliably identified when prompted to read for comprehension – similar to our design.

When investigating possessive references, one might conceive some referents to be more likely to ‘“possess’ something, i.e., as more prototypical possessors, like we advocated above (Section 4.2). While this is certainly true for human referents, it could also pertain to inanimate referents that are concrete objects rather than abstract concepts, yielding functional differences in processing (Martín-Loeches et al., Reference Martín-Loeches, Hinojosa, Fernández-Frías and Rubia2001; also, note that the version of the Animacy Hierarchy proposed by Corbett (Reference Corbett2006: 185) is subdivided accordingly: human > other animate > concrete inanimate > abstract inanimate). In the context of the type of semantic relation between the in−/animate possessor and the possessed, the antecedent’s conceptual role (ownership, agent- or patient-like status) may interact with patterns of variation, too. In addition, the definiteness of the antecedent—here, preceded by the feminine definite article die (the [fem.]) – could be driving gendered perceptions (Imai, Reference Imai, Schalk, Saalbach and Okada2014). Notably, fixed and conventionalized expressions like etwas hat seinen Reiz/Zweck/Grund (something has its [masc./neut.] charm/purpose/reason) may presumably be stored as constructions and hence accepted more readily due to their frequent co-occurrence (Stefanowitsch, Reference Stefanowitsch, Engelberg, Holler and Proost2011, but see Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022, 264–267 for discussion). However, since neither of these post-hoc explored factors was an experimental criterion, meaning items were not controlled accordingly, further work is desirable to assess their impact and advance the understanding of sein-possessives in feminine references. Furthermore, although possessee gender was not explicitly tested, its approximate balance across feminine and masculine/neuter forms in the stimulus set (alongside possessee number) mitigated potential impact on the present findings. Given that prior corpus research (Fleischer, Reference Fleischer2022: 275–276) found no evidence for gender- or number-driven agreement attraction with possessives, these features were not treated as variables in the present design, and no specific analyses were conducted.

Subjective ratings like AJs can vary for many reasons besides the grammaticality of a sentence. Although one third of distractors with grammatical inconsistencies and idiosyncrasies to counter the conspicuousness (Section 2.2) had been added as a ‘red herring’ to lay false trails, and despite a thorough randomization, one out of six subjects (16 of 96, 16.7%) indicated the experimental focus on gender and (pro)nouns in the post-experimental debriefing. Following Osterhout and Mobley (Reference Osterhout and Mobley1995), who detected a similar group effect in AJs, we discovered that their performance based on the overall mean acceptability of incongruent items was affected by group, regardless of antecedent animacy, which is indicative of a general agreement sensitivity. We therefore assume that the extent to which someone is responsive to grammatical gender saliency might differ on an individual level (alongside individual differences in reading strategies; see Mitchell (Reference Mitchell, Kieras and Just1984), and Garnham (Reference Garnham2001: 80), where the pattern of results was largely confined to those who read more slowly in the experiments, suggesting that strategic processing could be decisive). Deeming individual variation an essential characteristic in language perception and comprehension research, we did not exclude these datasets. In fact, by-subject differences in dealing with anaphora have previously been reported, e.g., preference groups in Hübner (Reference Hübner2021a): 14) that exclusively adhered to grammatical or pragmatic agreement patterns. Future experiments could focus on how real-time comprehension strategies of referential relations vary systematically across subjects; here, we addressed this issue through the random effect structure for participants (Sections 3.1–3.3). Stimulus repetition (pronominal incongruencies with sein) and a forced choice decision task may have also upweighted structural cues and may have highlighted the violation more prominently than a mere content question or probe may not have (Leeser et al., Reference Leeser, Brandl, Weissglass, Trofimovich and McDonough2011).

Finally, besides individual (dis)approval of referential mismatches, an influence of participant age on preferences in referential agreement type has been reported by Oelkers (Reference Oelkers1996: 11–12), Shake and Stine-Morrow (Reference Shake and Stine-Morrow2011), Foley and Ahn (Reference Foley and Ahn2024), and Steriopolo and Schütze (Reference Steriopolo and Schütze2025). When inspecting generational effects on sentence judgments, Schütze (Reference Schützeaccepted) showed that age indeed emerged as an influential factor, with higher acceptability rates of incongruent possessive pronouns by younger than older individuals. The question of whether younger participants are more open, flexible, and even innovative in using gender-deviant pronouns or adults in later life stages are more resistant to grammatical deviations (in general, or in pronominal reference specifically) is unresolved at this point (see Eckert, Reference Eckert and Coulmas1998, for discussion). Notably, age-related variability in anaphora resolution, retrieval and interpretation is also tied to cognitive abilities, such as working memory capacity (Karimi & Ferreira, Reference Karimi and Ferreira2016, 1033–1034), as well as gender-sensitive language development. On an individual level, evidence suggests that readers with better language processing skills are generally more sensitive to formal referential ambiguity than less proficient ones (Nieuwland & van Berkum, Reference Nieuwland and van Berkum2006). While we accounted for subject variation in the analyses, measuring these skills could be highly informative.

5. Conclusion

A two-fold task structure involving SPR and judgment of German sentences explored the acceptability and processing of possessive anaphoric pronouns that do not align with the grammatical gender of their referents (grammatically masculine/neuter sein instead of feminine ihr for feminine antecedents). Our manipulations introduced a gender violation in inanimate and human references and thereby created a potential conflict on formal and/or conceptual grounds.

We found that native readers of German were sensitive to gender agreement, reflected in a decrease of acceptability ratings and an increase of rts at the site of the pronominal violation. Effects varied by antecedent animacy, with different consequences for inanimate versus human antecedents, confirming that both linguistic structure and conceptual factors influence language processing.

A central implication is that research on gender agreement in languages with gender systems must take the type of reference (grammatical or gender based) into account and must not neglect the difference nor the overlaps between formal and conceptual gender features that guide agreement patterns. By and large, the referential inconsistencies induced by the pronoun led to markedly distinct effects largely depending on the animacy property of the possessor, which elucidates one source of gender conflicts in real-time reading comprehension.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2025.10032.

Data availability statement

The complete dataset, containing preprocessed data files, an R script for statistical analyses and the stimulus set, is stored in the corresponding Open Science Framework repository (https://osf.io/g9kbt/).

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable suggestions and constructive feedback. We are also very thankful to Elisa Schmied for the invaluable assistance in stimulus design and control and to Ronja Eberhard and Anne Heinzmann for their support in creating stimulus materials. We are grateful to Simon Kasper for sharing materials on German language varieties (REDE project). Many thanks to Sol Lago and the psycholinguistics colloquium at Frankfurt University for fruitful discussions on this topic.

Funding statement

This work was funded by a research grant to the first author as part of the Research Training Group 2700: ‘Dynamics and Stability of Linguistic Representations’ (DFG-441119738).

Competing interests

The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for this study was obtained from Ethikkommission der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaften (Approval Number: 2023–04).

Consent to participate

Informed consent to participate was written. Participants read the consent statement and explicitly clicked to agree if they wished to participate and continue to the experiment.

Consent for publication

In the consent form, we have asked for and obtained written informed consent to publish their data anonymously, explaining that identifying details would be removed.

Footnotes

The research was conducted at Philipps-Universität Marburg (now Universität Marburg), Neurolinguistics lab. All authors consent for the manuscript to be submitted in its current form.

1 Model term: Response ~ Congruency * Animacy * Distance + (1|Item) + (1|Subject) + (1|Trial).

2 Model term: logRRT ~ Congruency * Animacy * Distance * Response + (1|Item) + (1|Subject) + (1|Trial).

3 Model term: logrt ~ Congruency * Animacy * Distance + Response + Frequency + (1|Graphemic Length) + (1|Item) + (1|Trial) + (Response|Subject).

References

Ackerman, L. (2019). Syntactic and cognitive issues in investigating gendered coreference. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 4(1), 117127. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.721Google Scholar
Anwyl-Irvine, A. L., Massonnié, J., Flitton, A., Kirkham, N., & Evershed, J. K. (2020). Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods, 52(1), 388407.10.3758/s13428-019-01237-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audring, J. (2008). Gender assignment and gender agreement: Evidence from pronominal gender languages. Morphology, 18(2), 93116. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-009-9124-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Audring, J., & Booij, G. (2009). Genus als probleemcategorie. Taal en tongval, 61(3), 1337.10.5117/TET2009.1.AUDRCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 1228. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bader, M., & Häussler, J. (2010). Toward a model of grammaticality judgements. Journal of Linguistics, 46(2), 273330. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226709990260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bader, M., & Portele, Y. (2025). Animacy outweighs topichood when choosing pronouns and word order. Journal of Memory and Language, 142, 104615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2025.104615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bader, M., Torregrossa, J., & Rinke, E. (2023). Pinning down the interaction between animacy and syntactic function in the interpretation of German and Italian personal and demonstrative pronouns. Discourse Processes, 60(9), 655673. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2023.2252699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binanzer, A., Schimke, S., & Schunack, S. (2022). Syntaktische Domäne oder lineare Distanz – Welcher Faktor steuert semantische Kongruenz im Kontext von hybrid nouns und Epikoina in stärkerem Maß? In Diewald, G. & Nübling, (Eds.), Genus – Sexus – Gender (pp. 193218). de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110746396-006.Google Scholar
Birkenes, M. B., Chroni, K., & Fleischer, J. (2014). Genus- und Sexuskongruenz im Neuhochdeutschen: Ergebnisse einer Korpusuntersuchung zur narrativen Prosa des 17. Bis 19. Jahrhunderts. Deutsche Sprache, 42(1), 124. https://doi.org/10.37307/j.1868-775X.2014.01.02.Google Scholar
Birkenes, M. B., & Fleischer, J. (2022). Genus- und Sexuskongruenz im Mittelhochdeutschen: Eine Paralleltextanalyse zum lexical hybrid kint. In Diewald, G. & Nübling, D. (Eds.), Genus – Sexus – Gender (pp. 241265). de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110746396-008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridges, D., Pitiot, A., MacAskill, M. R., & Peirce, J. W. (2020). The timing mega-study: Comparing a range of experiment generators, both lab-based and online. PeerJ, 8, e9414. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacciari, C., Carreiras, M., & Cionini, C. B. (1997). When words have two genders: Anaphor resolution for Italian functionally ambiguous words. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 517532. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caffarra, S., Janssen, N., & Barber, H. A. (2014). Two sides of gender: ERP evidence for the presence of two routes during gender agreement processing. Neuropsychologia, 63, 124134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M., Garnham, A., Oakhill, J., & Cain, K. (1996). The use of stereotypical gender information in constructing a mental model: Evidence from English and Spanish. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(3), 639663. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cauldron Science (2022). Gorilla experiment builder. https://gorilla.sc.Google Scholar
Chow, W. Y., Lewis, S., & Phillips, C. (2014). Immediate sensitivity to structural constraints in pronoun resolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 630. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbett, G. G. (2006). Agreement. Cambridge textbooks in linguistics. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Czech, H. (2014). Zur variation grammatischer und semantischer Genuskongruenz bei personal- und Relativpronomen im Deutschen: Variablenbindung und lineare Distanz als potenzielle Einflussfaktoren. Tagungsband zur StuTS, 55, 140. https://doi.org/10.1515/zgl-2015-0015.Google Scholar
Dahl, Ö. (2000). Animacy and the notion of semantic gender. In Unterbeck, B., Rissanen, M., Nevalainen, T., & Saari, M. (Eds.), Gender in grammar and cognition. Trends in linguistics: Studies and monographs 124. Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
De Vogelaer, G., Fanta, J., Poarch, G., Schimke, S., & Urbanek, L. (2020). Syntactic or semantic gender agreement in Dutch, German and German learner Dutch: A speeded grammaticality judgement task. In De Vogelaer, G., Koster, D., & Leuschner, T. (Eds.), German and Dutch in contrast: Synchronic, diachronic and psycholinguistic perspectives (Vol. 11, pp. 271298). De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110668476-010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duden, (2016). Das Wörterbuch der sprachlichen Zweifelsfälle. Richtiges und gutes Deutsch. (Hennig, M. (Ed.); with Osterwinter, R., Schneider, J. G., Steinhauer, A. and the Dudenredaktion, 8th ed.). Duden Verlag (Duden – Deutsche Sprache in 12 Bänden).Google Scholar
Duden (2022). Die Grammatik. Struktur und Verwendung der deutschen Sprache. Sätze - Wortgruppen – Wörter . (Wöllstein, A. (Ed.); With the collaboration of the Dudenredaktion, 10th ed.). Duden Verlag (Duden – Deutsche Sprache in 12 Bänden).Google Scholar
Eckert, P. (1998). Age as a sociolinguistic variable. In Coulmas, F. (Ed.), The handbook of sociolinguistics (Vol. 9, pp. 151167). Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405166256.CH9.Google Scholar
Esaulova, Y., & von Stockhausen, L. (2022). Empirical evidence for subtle gender biases in language. In Ayoun, D. (Ed.), The Acquisition of Gender (pp. 4970). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/sibil.63.03esaCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleischer, J. (2022). Qualität hat seinen Preis“: Genus-insensitives “sein” im Gegenwartsdeutschen. Linguistische Berichte, 271, 251288.Google Scholar
Foley, S., & Ahn, B. (2024). Processing cataphoric they amidst pronominal innovation. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 8, 289308. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-024-00153-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. (2019). Das anaphorische Referenzsystem. In Frank, M. (Ed.), Phorische Verkettung im Deutschen: Eine exemplarische Untersuchung anhand von Diskursrelationen der kausalen Gruppe, Linguistik – Impulse & Tendenzen, Vol. 79, Kapitel 3, 80281. de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110621662-003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fukumura, K., & van Gompel, R. P. (2011). The effect of animacy on the choice of referring expression. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(10), 14721504. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.506444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garnham, A. (2001). Mental models and the interpretation of anaphora. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gries, S. (2021). Statistics for linguistics with R: A practical introduction. De Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110718256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammer, A., Jansma, B. M., Lamers, M., & Münte, T. F. (2005). Pronominal reference in sentences about persons or things: An electrophysiological approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 227239.10.1162/0898929053124947CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, A., Jansma, B. M., Lamers, M., & Münte, T. F. (2008). Interplay of meaning, syntax and working memory during pronoun resolution investigated by ERPs. Brain Research, 1230, 177191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellinger, M., & Bußmann, H. (2015). Gender across languages: The linguistic representation of women and men. In Hellinger, M. & Motschenbacher, H. (Eds.), Gender across languages (Vol. 4, pp. 125). John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/impact.36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hübner, J. (2021a). Genus und Sexus im Konflikt. Kongruenzformen hybrider Nomina im Sprachproduktionsprozess. Linguistik online, 107(2), 919. https://doi.org/10.13092/lo.107.7682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hübner, J. (2021b). Das Mädchen und ihr Liebhaber. Pragmatik als motivierender Faktor von Sexuskongruenz. In Binanzer, A., Gamper, J., & Wecker, V. (Eds.), Prototypen – Schemata – Konstruktionen (pp. 3152). De Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110710595-003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imai, M., Schalk, L., Saalbach, H., & Okada, H. (2014). All giraffes have female-specific properties: Influence of grammatical gender on deductive reasoning about sex-specific properties in German speakers. Cognitive Science, 38(3), 514536. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irmen, L., Holt, D. V., & Weisbrod, M. (2010). Effects of role typicality on processing person information in German: Evidence from an ERP study. Brain Research, 1353, 133144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irmen, L., & Kurovskaja, J. (2010). On the semantic content of grammatical gender and its impact on the representation of human referents. Experimental Psychology, 57(5), 367375. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jegerski, J. (2014). Self-paced reading. In Jegerski, J. & Van Patten, B. (Eds.), Research methods in second language psycholinguistics (pp. 2049). Routledge.Google Scholar
Karimi, H., & Ferreira, F. (2016). Good-enough linguistic representations and online cognitive equilibrium in language processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 10131040. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1053951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennison, S. M. (2003). Comprehending the pronouns her, him, and his: Implications for theories of referential processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(3), 335352. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00071-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, K. M., Panther, K. U., & Zubin, D. A. (2010). Motivating grammatical and conceptual gender agreement in German: empirical studies. In Schmid, H.-J. & Handl, S. (Eds.), Cognitive Foundations of Linguistic Usage Patterns (Vol. 13, pp. 171194). Berlin, New York: de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110216035.171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, K. M. & Zubin, D. A. (1997). Sechs Prinzipien für die Genuszuweisung im Deutschen: Ein Beitrag zur natürlichen Klassifikation. In Sieburg, H. (Ed.), Sprache – Genus/Sexus. Dokumentation germanistischer Forschung 3, 86115. Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Köpcke, K. M., & Zubin, D. A. (2009). Genus. In Hentschel, E. & Vogel, P. M. (Eds.), Deutsche Morphologie (pp. 132154). De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kopf, K., & Bildhauer, F. (2024). The genitive alternation in German. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory. https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2024-0017.Google Scholar
Kreiner, H., Sturt, P., & Garrod, S. (2008). Processing definitional and stereotypical gender in reference resolution: Evidence from eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 239261.10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leeser, M., Brandl, A., & Weissglass, C. (2011). Task effects in second language sentence processing research. In Trofimovich, P. & McDonough, K. (Eds.), Applying priming methods to L2 learning, teaching, and research: Insights from psycholinguistics (pp. 179198). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.30.13lee?locatt=mode:legacy.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leiss, E. (1997). Genus und Sexus. Kritische Anmerkungen zur Sexualisierung der Grammatik. In Sieburg, H. (Ed.), Sprache – Genus/Sexus. Dokumentation germanistischer Forschung (Vol. 3, pp. 322345). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lenth, R. V., Banfai, B., Bolker, B., Buerkner, P., Giné-Vázquez, I., Herve, M., Jung, M., Love, J., Miguez, F., Piaskowski, J., Riebl, H., & Singmann, H. (2024). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means (Version 1.10.6) [R package]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeansGoogle Scholar
Martín-Loeches, M., Hinojosa, J. A., Fernández-Frías, C., & Rubia, F. J. (2001). Functional differences in the semantic processing of concrete and abstract words. Neuropsychologia, 39(10), 10861096. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00033-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Misersky, J., Majid, A., & Snijders, T. M. (2019). Grammatical gender in German influences how role-nouns are interpreted: Evidence from ERPs. Discourse Processes, 56(8), 643654. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2018.1541382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, D. C. (1984). An evaluation of subject-paced reading tasks and other methods for investigating immediate processes in reading. In Kieras, D. & Just, M. A. (Eds.), New methods in reading comprehension. Earlbaum.Google Scholar
Myers, J. (2009). Syntactic judgement experiments. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 406423. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00113.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M. S., & van Berkum, J. J. (2006). When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 10981111. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niu, R., & Liu, H. (2022). Effects of syntactic distance and word order on language processing: An investigation based on a psycholinguistic Treebank of English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 51, 10431062. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-022-09878-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oelkers, S. (1996). Der Sprintstar und ihre Freundinnen“. Ein empirischer Beitrag zur Diskussion um das generische Maskulinum. Muttersprache, 106, 115.Google Scholar
Oppenrieder, W., & Thurmair, M. (2002). Doppelt genäht hält besser“: Mehrfachmarkierung im Gegenwartsdeutschen. Jahrbuch für Internationale Germanistik, Reihe A, Band, 54, 117123.Google Scholar
Ortmann, A. (1998). The role of [±animate] in inflection. In Fabri, R., Ortmann, A., & Parodi, T. (Eds.), Models of inflection. Linguistische Arbeiten (Vol. 388, pp. 6084). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700238267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L. A. (1995). Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(6), 739773. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panther, K. U. (2009). Grammatische versus konzeptuelle Kongruenz. Oder: Wann siegt das natürliche Geschlecht? In Brdar-Szabó, R., Knipf-Komlósi, E., & Péteri, A. (Eds.), An der Grenze zwischen Grammatik und Pragmatik (Vol. 3, pp. 6786). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Samat, S., & Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 153163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.01.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piepers, J., & Redl, T. (2018). Gender-mismatching pronouns in context: The interpretation of possessive pronouns in Dutch and Limburgian. Linguistics in the Netherlands, 35(1), 97110. https://doi.org/10.1075/avt.00007.pie.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prolific (2014). (Version 08/2023). London, UK. https://www.prolific.comGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
RStudio Team (2024). RStudio: Integrated development for R. RStudio version 4.4.2. Boston, MA: PBC. http://www.rstudio.com/Google Scholar
Schmitz, D., & Esser, J. (2021). SfL: Statistics for linguistics (Version 0.4.2) [R package]. https://github.com/dosc91/SfLGoogle Scholar
Schütze, C. (accepted). Die Romanfigur hat seine Geschichte”: Zur Akzeptanz genusinkongruenter Possessivpronomina bei epizönen Personenbezeichnungen. Germanistische Linguistik.Google Scholar
Schütze, C. T. (2016). The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgements and linguistic methodology. Language Science Press. https://doi.org/10.26530/OAPEN_603356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schütze, C. T., & Sprouse, J. (2014). Judgement data. In Podesva, R. J., & Sharma, D. (Eds.). Research methods in linguistics 27, Chapter 3, 2750. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139013734.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shake, M. C., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2011). Age differences in resolving anaphoric expressions during reading. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 18(6), 678707. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.607228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siewierska, A. (2004). Person. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511812729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorlin, S., & Gardelle, L. (2018). Anthropocentrism, egocentrism and the notion of Animacy hierarchy. International Journal of Language and Culture, From Culture to Language and Back: The Animacy Hierarchy in Language and Discourse, 5(2). https://doi.org/10.1075/ijolc.00004.gar.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. (2011). Keine Grammatik ohne Konstruktionen: Ein logisch-ökonomisches argument für die Konstruktionsgrammatik. In Engelberg, S., Holler, A., & Proost, K. (Eds.), Sprachliches Wissen zwischen Lexikon und Grammatik (pp. 181210). De Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110262339.181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steriopolo, O., & Schütze, C. (2025). Referential means in German: An experimental study comparing feminine epicene nouns with masculine generic nouns. Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2023-0049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, A. J., Holler, J., & Kidd, E. (2007). Shallow processing of ambiguous pronouns: Evidence for delay. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(12), 16801696. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210601160807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thurmair, M. (2006). “Das Model und ihr Prinz”. Kongruenz und Texteinbettung bei genus-Sexus-Divergenz. Deutsche Sprache: ds, 34, 191220.Google Scholar
van Gompel, R. P., & Liversedge, S. P. (2003). The influence of morphological information on cataphoric pronoun assignment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(1), 128139. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.1.128.Google ScholarPubMed
Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. The Psychological Record, 58, 475482. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L., François, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A., Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T., Miller, E., Bache, S., Müller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D., Spinu, V., … & Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Radvansky, G. A., Hilliard, A. E., & Curiel, J. M. (1998). Constructing multidimensional situation models during reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(3), 199220. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0203_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Example sentences for inanimate (1) and human (2) possessors in short (a) and long (b) distances between possessive pronoun and noun antecedent referred to with the congruent or incongruent pronoun

Figure 1

Figure 1. Mean acceptability ratings (in %) for congruent (ihr) and incongruent (sein) sentences with human (no pattern) or inanimate (striped) feminine antecedents; error bars represent SD.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Mean RTs (in ms) for congruent (ihr) and incongruent (sein) sentences in short and long distances with human (no pattern) or inanimate (striped) feminine antecedents for sentence approvals (left panel) and rejections (right panel).

Figure 3

Figure 3. Mean reading times following a human (left side of the panels) or an inanimate antecedent (right side of the panels) through sentences with a short (left panel) or long distance (right panel) between the noun and the anaphoric pronoun. The possessive phrase containing congruent ihr or incongruent sein pronouns is shaded in grey. Dots indicate the congruent condition; triangles the incongruent.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Mean reading times at the possessive phrase containing congruent ihr or incongruent sein pronouns with human (solid line) or inanimate (dashed line) antecedents in short (left panel) or long distance (right panel) between noun and anaphoric pronoun. Dots indicate the congruent condition; triangles the incongruent; shades represent 95% CIs based on the standard error.

Supplementary material: File

Schütze et al. supplementary material

Schütze et al. supplementary material
Download Schütze et al. supplementary material(File)
File 233 Bytes