Hostname: page-component-65b85459fc-hn4tn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-16T06:05:29.119Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Populism and Polarization: A Nostalgic Narrative of ‘Us’ and ‘Them’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 October 2025

Mariken A.C.G. van der Velden*
Affiliation:
Department of Communication Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Alberto López Ortega
Affiliation:
Department of Communication Science, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Denise J. Roth
Affiliation:
Strategic Communication Group, Wageningen University and Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
Puck Guldemond
Affiliation:
Strategic Communication Group, Wageningen University and Research, Wageningen, The Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: Mariken A.C.G. van der Velden; Email: m.a.c.g.vander.velden@vu.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The vast presence of populism in contemporary political discourse has introduced a narrative steeped in nostalgia, evoking images of a revered national past and delineating a stark division between the ‘authentic us’ and the ‘alien them’. While these messages resonate with a substantial portion of citizens, they concurrently foster identity-driven animosity and derogation. In a pre-registered experiment in the Netherlands (with data collected between January and March 2023), we distinguish the influence of nostalgic narratives and scapegoating on societal sentiments, revealing their pivotal role in exacerbating current levels of polarization. Our findings underscore the potential of nostalgic narratives to shape affective sentiments towards ideological and social in-groups, while also influencing sentiments towards out-groups. Messages featuring scapegoats were found to intensify positive sentiment towards in-groups, while simultaneously diminishing positive sentiment towards out-groups. This research underscores a crucial mechanism underpinning the ebb and flow of identity-based sentiments. The results indicate that nostalgic discourse, particularly when intertwined with scapegoating, can serve as a catalyst for the intensification of in-group affinity and the exacerbation of out-group aversion. In essence, our study underscores the far-reaching implications of nostalgic narratives in perpetuating societal animosity and polarization and sheds light on a critical facet of contemporary political discourse.

Information

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NC
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Government and Opposition Ltd.

Populist parties across Europe have gained momentum by capitalizing on anti-elitist and nostalgic narratives which frame the present as a deviation from an idealized past while attributing blame to societal out-groups. In times of several contemporaneous salient societal divisions, nostalgia behaves as a vehicle for different potential out-groups that can be targeted and vilified, thereby departing from the single focus on migrants as the scapegoat (van Spanje Reference van Spanje2011). Alongside the Brexit debate about the United Kingdom leaving the European Union (Hobolt et al. Reference Hobolt, Leeper and Tilley2021; Tilley and Hobolt Reference Tilley and Hobolt2023), the controversy over Catalan independence in Spain (Balcells and Kuo Reference Balcells and Kuo2023), or diverging positions regarding Covid-19 countermeasures (Bor et al. Reference Bor, Jørgensen and Petersen2023a), clashes between pro- and anti-environmentalists, Nimbys (people who object to development in their neighbourhood, that is, ‘not in my back yard’) and their opponents (Devine-Wright Reference Devine-Wright2009), as well as conflicting stances on vaccination and abortion rights, offer ample opportunity for tensions to arise (Bor et al. Reference Bor, Jørgensen and Petersen2023b; Crawford et al. Reference Crawford2022; Wagner and Eberl Reference Wagner and Eberl2022). Populist parties often exploit widespread dissatisfaction with the status quo, positioning themselves as alternatives to mainstream politics (Hinterleitner et al. Reference Hinterleitner, Kammermeier and Moffitt2024; Inglehart and Norris Reference Inglehart and Norris2016; Taggart Reference Taggart, Fitzi, Mackert and Turner2018). As voices of the people, populist leaders invoke a romanticized version of the past (Elçi Reference Elçi2022; Smeekes et al. Reference Smeekes, Verkuyten and Martinovic2015, Reference Smeekes, Wildschut and Sedikides2021; Van Prooijen et al. Reference Van Prooijen2022), emphasizing cultural homogeneity and traditional values while casting blame on the establishment and out-groups, such as immigrants, for societal grievances (Hunger and Paxton Reference Hunger and Paxton2022; Mudde Reference Mudde2004; Mudde and Rovira Kaltwasser Reference Mudde and Rovira Kaltwasser2013). This nostalgic rhetoric has become a hallmark of the populist strategy, as it effectively resonates with voters’ insecurities and longing for a simpler, idealized time.

The interplay between nostalgia and scapegoating forms a powerful rhetorical strategy that potentially exacerbates affective polarization – the phenomenon where individuals develop warmer feelings towards their own political group and colder feelings towards opposing groups, leading to heightened animosity and reduced willingness to compromise (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar2019) – by targeting ideological and social groups. We argue that nostalgia operates as a narrative vehicle that creates a dichotomy between an idyllic past and a troubled present, with out-groups portrayed as responsible for the decline. This mechanism strengthens in-group cohesion while fostering hostility towards perceived adversaries. By linking nostalgic narratives with scapegoating, populist leaders amplify these divisions, deepening societal fractures and intensifying emotional antipathy between groups. Building on existing literature (Bonikowski Reference Bonikowski2017; Hameleers et al. Reference Hameleers, Bos and De Vreese2017; Smeekes and Verkuyten Reference Smeekes and Verkuyten2017; Wodak Reference Wodak2015), this study integrates insights from research on affective polarization, nostalgia and populism to clarify how nostalgic narratives and scapegoating mechanisms intensify affective polarization.

In times of societal uncertainty and division, nostalgic messaging becomes a powerful tool for mobilizing support and amplifying polarization. Nostalgia implicitly suggests an idealized past with diminished out-group influence, while scapegoating explicitly attributes societal decline to these groups. Together, these mechanisms reinforce affective polarization by intensifying positive emotions towards in-groups and negative emotions towards out-groups.

In our study we go beyond the traditional focus on affective polarization between the ideological left and right to encompass broader social groups that individuals identify with or oppose, broadening the scope of analysis to include diverse societal dynamics. Our novel theoretical argument is that nostalgic narratives act as a dual mechanism, implicitly invoking an idealized past while explicitly scapegoating out-groups as the root of contemporary societal issues. This duality serves as a potent driver of affective polarization by simultaneously strengthening in-group identity and vilifying out-groups. To examine the effects of nostalgic and scapegoating messages on affective polarization, we conducted a pre-registered experiment embedded in the fourth wave of the Vrije Universiteit (VU) Election Studies 2023 (Van der Velden et al. Reference Van der Velden2023). This five-wave panel survey, conducted between 11 January and 20 March 2023 by Kieskompas, a Dutch polling company, focused on the pre- and post-regional election period in the Netherlands (Van der Velden et al. Reference Van der Velden2023). The Netherlands was chosen as a test case due to its relatively low levels of affective polarization and limited prevalence of nostalgic rhetoric, making it a challenging yet insightful context for studying these mechanisms (Harteveld Reference Harteveld2021; Müller and Proksch Reference Müller and Proksch2024).

Our study makes several key contributions to the literature on populism, nostalgia and affective polarization. First, we provide a novel perspective on the contemporary political Zeitgeist by linking nostalgia and scapegoating with group affect. This integration sheds light on how these narratives shape in-group and out-group dynamics, offering a comprehensive framework for understanding their role in contemporary politics. Second, we critically evaluate prior research on affective polarization and populism, identifying gaps and providing insights into the nuanced effects of nostalgic rhetoric. Third, we highlight the evolving nature of populist narratives, which have transcended ideological boundaries to influence political discourse across the spectrum (Bonikowski and Stuhler Reference Bonikowski and Stuhler2022; Tilley and Hobolt Reference Tilley and Hobolt2023). By elucidating the dual role of nostalgia as both a sentimental and a polarizing force, this article advances our understanding of its impact on political behaviour. In doing so, it contributes to the ongoing discourse on the intersections of nostalgia, scapegoating and affective polarization, offering valuable insights for scholars and practitioners alike. Our findings underscore the need for further research into the emotional mechanisms underpinning populist rhetoric and their broader implications for democratic societies.

A nostalgic narrative of ‘us’ and ‘them’

The antagonistic portrayal of social structures is arguably a central characteristic of the current climate of increased affective polarization. Affective polarization refers to the growing emotional divide between opposing political or ideological groups, characterized by heightened in-group favouritism and increased hostility towards out-groups (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar2019). Unlike ideological or political polarization, which focuses on disagreements over policies or ideologies, affective polarization captures the social and emotional dimensions of group dynamics (Dias and Lelkes Reference Dias and Lelkes2022; Hartman et al. Reference Hartman2022; Jost et al. Reference Jost, Baldassarri and Druckman2022). This phenomenon is evident in individuals’ negative perceptions of out-groups and positive bias towards in-groups, even when substantive ideological differences are absent (Mason Reference Mason2018). This distinction is crucial because it underscores the role of social identity in driving polarization independently of policy disagreements.

Classic partisan rivalries have evolved, with ideological identities becoming more prominent (Ellis and Stimson Reference Ellis and Stimson2012; Oshri et al. Reference Oshri, Yair and Huddy2022). Voters increasingly align themselves with ideological labels like ‘liberal’ or ‘left-wing’, rather than with specific political parties. These ideologies often divide citizens along lines similar to the Republican–Democrat divide in the United States, creating stark distinctions such as ‘right versus left’, with the latter being frequently characterized as ‘woke’ (Comellas and Torcal Reference Comellas and Torcal2023). In Europe, party affect primarily orbits around three ideological spheres: left, right and radical right (Bantel Reference Bantel2023; Kekkonen and Ylä-Anttila Reference Kekkonen and Ylä-Anttila2021). Additionally, specific issues can foster strong ‘us versus them’ dynamics, serving roles akin to partisan identities. Examples include the Brexit debate (Hobolt et al. Reference Hobolt, Leeper and Tilley2021; Tilley and Hobolt Reference Tilley and Hobolt2023), the controversy over Catalan independence (Balcells and Kuo Reference Balcells and Kuo2023) and positions regarding Covid-19 countermeasures (Bor et al. Reference Bor, Jørgensen and Petersen2023a). Other significant divides include pro- and anti-environmentalism, Nimbys against their adversaries (Devine-Wright Reference Devine-Wright2009) and conflicting stances on vaccination and abortion rights (Bor et al. Reference Bor, Jørgensen and Petersen2023b; Crawford et al. Reference Crawford2022; Wagner and Eberl Reference Wagner and Eberl2022). These identities amplify societal divisions, complicating the landscape of polarization.

In the expansion of antagonisms across social groups, populists find an opportune environment to thrive. Cas Mudde’s (Reference Mudde2004) ideational approach conceptualizes populism as a thin-centred ideology that seldom exists in isolation. Instead, it tends to affiliate itself with a broader host ideology, thereby displaying contextual sensitivity (Mudde and Rovira Kaltwasser Reference Mudde and Rovira Kaltwasser2013). This ideological framework creates a Manichaean tension within society, fundamentally dividing it into two homogeneous and antagonistic groups (Mudde Reference Mudde2004; Mudde and Rovira Kaltwasser Reference Mudde and Rovira Kaltwasser2013): the morally righteous ‘us’ juxtaposed against the evil ‘them’. Scholarly investigation often focuses on the populist dichotomy between the perceived homogeneous people and the perceived malevolent elite (for overviews, see Berman Reference Berman2021; Hunger and Paxton Reference Hunger and Paxton2022; Noury and Roland Reference Noury and Roland2020). Yet the adaptability inherent in the thin-centred populist ideology and its contextual sensitivity allows political leaders espousing populist rhetoric to broaden the definition of out-groups deemed malevolent, thus framing them as threats to their envisioned societal ideal. Traditionally, immigrants have been frequently depicted as an external source of malevolence, supposedly corroding the fabric of society (for overviews, see Shehaj et al. Reference Shehaj, Shin and Inglehart2021; van Spanje Reference van Spanje2011). However, in these times of intense group-based polarization, populists are employing new tropes to articulate their out-group narratives.

Populist politicians exhibit a propensity to delineate distinct in- and out-groups. Conversely, non-populist politicians utilize communication strategies to construct a narrative aimed at an ideal societal framework; however, the adaptability of such strategies is often constrained by ideological limitations in comparison to the more flexible approach of populists. Scholars specializing in populist communication have highlighted the prevalence of emotionally charged rhetoric in numerous communication studies (Aalberg et al. Reference Aalberg2016; Widmann Reference Widmann2021). This kind of rhetoric is designed to evoke profound emotional responses, aiming to resonate deeply with the audience while consistently emphasizing the sovereignty of the people (Schumacher et al. Reference Schumacher, Rooduijn and Bakker2022). Populist rhetoric inherently revolves around asserting the existence of a severe political crisis that poses an existential threat to ordinary citizens. This rhetoric vehemently vilifies the out-group, allocating blame to them (Schumacher et al. Reference Schumacher, Rooduijn and Bakker2022). Recent studies have underscored the efficacy of nostalgia as a potent vehicle employed within this narrative construction (Elçi Reference Elçi2022; Smeekes et al. Reference Smeekes, Wildschut and Sedikides2021; Van Prooijen et al. Reference Van Prooijen2022).

Populists need a compelling narrative to channel their antagonistic depictions, which they often achieve by contrasting the perceived decay of the present – where purported enemies abound – with an idealized, pristine past where such issues were non-existent, essentially employing nostalgia. Nostalgia, derived from the Greek words nostos (home) and algia (pain or sorrow), encapsulates a complex blend of sentiments, representing both a pain of loss and a repository of pleasant memories. Regardless of its more positive connotation as a pleasant memory or the poignant aspect as a pain of loss, nostalgia fundamentally revolves around the notion of loss (Pickering and Keightley Reference Pickering and Keightley2006). At its core, nostalgia can be understood as an individual-level emotion: a tender yearning for one’s past, usually focusing on the innocence of childhood or the formative years of adolescence (Marchegiani and Phau Reference Marchegiani and Phau2013). This psychological interpretation predominates in individualistic frameworks. However, within the realm of the social sciences, the emphasis often shifts to collective nostalgia. Defined as an emotive attachment or fond feelings associated with perceiving oneself as a member of a group, collective nostalgia is grounded in idealized perceptions of the history or past events of this group (Baldwin et al. Reference Baldwin, White and Sullivan2018). These events often have direct connections to the in-group’s shared experiences. Recollection of such events is commonly transmitted through collective memory, perpetuated within families, documented in literature or disseminated through mass-media narratives (Kao Reference Kao, Gürhan-Canli, Otnes and Zhu2012). The emergence of collective nostalgia is frequently linked to a perception among individuals that their group enjoyed a superior status or quality of life in the past compared to the present circumstances (Cheung et al. Reference Cheung2017). Despite its abstract nature, collective nostalgia tends to exhibit a remarkable consistency among individuals within the same cultural group and generation, reflecting shared experiences and cultural influences (Kao Reference Kao, Gürhan-Canli, Otnes and Zhu2012).

Nostalgia, with its intricate blend of sentimental reminiscence and a sense of loss, is intertwined with the dynamics of the in- and out-groups, delineated in the seminal work of Tajfel and Turner (Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979). The concept of collective nostalgia (Smeekes et al. Reference Smeekes, Verkuyten and Martinovic2015, Reference Smeekes, Wildschut and Sedikides2021; Van Prooijen et al. Reference Van Prooijen2022), shaped by idealized perceptions of the past, often finds resonance within group identities. These nostalgic yearnings, rooted in shared historical events and cultural experiences, can reinforce the distinctions between in-groups and out-groups. Nostalgia often implies more than a simple longing for the past – it also frames the present as a deviation from this ideal state. This deviation requires explanation, and scapegoating emerges as a complementary mechanism by which blame for societal decline is attributed to specific out-groups. By constructing these groups as obstacles to reclaiming the past, scapegoating translates the implicit contrasts of nostalgia into explicit narratives of blame. This process not only reinforces the in-group’s cohesion but also sharpens its boundaries by portraying out-groups as threats or antagonists. We argue that political elites make hay of the nostalgic discourse by framing these identities as either an in- or an out-group and implicitly or explicitly shifting blame to the out-groups for unwanted changes in society.

Thus, the interaction between nostalgia and scapegoating has significant implications for affective polarization (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar2019). Nostalgia intensifies emotional attachment to the in-group by evoking pride and solidarity, while scapegoating amplifies hostility towards out-groups by framing them as responsible for societal decline. Together, these mechanisms contribute to affective polarization by deepening emotional divides between groups, strengthening in-group favouritism and exacerbating out-group hostility (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar2019). Importantly, while nostalgia can function independently of scapegoating, its polarizing effects are potentially heightened when coupled with explicit out-group blame.

Building on our argument, we contend that nostalgic narratives are not merely rhetorical devices but pivotal tools in the populist strategy to deepen societal divisions, exploiting affective polarization by framing out-groups as obstacles to reclaiming an idealized past. These narratives wield significant emotional power, fostering a sense of in-group unity through shared memories of a glorified history while simultaneously amplifying hostility towards those cast as responsible for societal decline. By intertwining nostalgic appeals with scapegoating, populist actors transform collective longing into a mechanism of exclusion, reinforcing intergroup boundaries and intensifying animosities. Existing research on populist rhetoric has significantly advanced our understanding of how out-groups are constructed and mobilized, particularly in contexts where immigrants or elites are depicted as threats to societal cohesion (van Spanje Reference van Spanje2011). However, much of this work has centred on these traditional or static out-groups, often overlooking the fluid and evolving nature of these categorizations in response to shifting political and social dynamics. Recent developments illustrate how nostalgic narratives are increasingly coupled with scapegoating to target a broader spectrum of social groups (Balcells and Kuo Reference Balcells and Kuo2023; Bor et al. Reference Bor, Jørgensen and Petersen2023b; Wagner and Eberl Reference Wagner and Eberl2022). This adaptability allows populist rhetoric to reframe grievances and identify new out-groups, reinforcing affective polarization in diverse and evolving contexts. By examining the dynamic interplay between nostalgia and scapegoating, we highlight their dual role in shaping polarization. Together, these mechanisms extend beyond political rhetoric, deepening societal divisions and posing challenges to democratic cohesion. Understanding how these narratives are mobilized in evolving contexts provides critical insights into the mechanisms driving contemporary polarization. We therefore test the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1a (H1a): Nostalgic messages will increase affective sentiment for ideological social in-groups.

Hypothesis 1b (H1b): Messages mentioning scapegoats will increase affective sentiment for ideological social in-groups.

Hypothesis 2a (H2a): Nostalgic messages will decrease affective sentiment for ideological social out-groups.

Hypothesis 2b (H2b): Messages mentioning scapegoats will decrease affective sentiment for ideological social out-groups.

Data, measurement and method

Data

To investigate the nostalgic narrative of ‘us versus them’ across ideological lines, we conducted a pre-registered experiment (see https://aspredicted.org/YST_2GH) within the framework of the VU Election Studies 2023 (Van der Velden Reference Van der Velden2023). The study utilizes a five-wave panel survey administered between 11 January 2023 and 20 March 2023 by Kieskompas. The survey was conducted with members of the Kieskompas VIP panel: a dedicated group of respondents who voluntarily participate in research without financial compensation. Although the panel was based on an opt-in sample, it was representative of the population in key demographic variables. This article draws on data from the first and fourth waves of the survey. The first wave, conducted from 11 to 18 January 2023, collected information on participants’ political preferences, nostalgic attitudes and populist orientation. The fourth wave, conducted from 7 to 14 March 2023, incorporated an experiment designed to disentangle the effects of nostalgic messages from ideological content. A total of 2,185 participants completed the first wave of the survey, while 1,560 completed the fourth wave. To address missing data, we followed the recommendations of Alan Gerber (Reference Gerber, Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011), imputing missing values using the mean value within each wave and flagging variables with more than 10% missing data.

Experimental set-up

To examine the affective consequences of nostalgic and scapegoating messages on social in-groups and out-groups, we conducted an experiment designed to cue participants with nostalgic values and scapegoats. The participants were randomly assigned to different conditions within this framework, as shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Overview of Experimental Procedure

The first step in the experimental set-up involved classifying participants into ideological groups based on their self-identified political orientation. Participants reported their ideological stance on a five-point Likert scale, indicating whether they considered themselves very progressive, progressive, centrist, conservative or very conservative. This classification was critical because specific objects of nostalgia which people look back on fondly are known to differ between ideological positions (Lammers and Baldwin Reference Lammers and Baldwin2018; Stefaniak et al. Reference Stefaniak2021). Individuals who identified as ‘centre’, ‘conservative’ or ‘very conservative’ were grouped into the ‘conservative’ category, while those who identified as ‘progressive’ or ‘very progressive’ were assigned to the ‘progressive’ category. While we acknowledge that ideological in-/out-grouping can occur at various levels of granularity (from party factions to broader ideological camps), our study focuses on the primary division between the progressive and conservative ideological spheres. This division is substantiated by research indicating that European citizens, even in highly fragmented contexts, tend to align affectively within two main blocks (Bantel Reference Bantel2023; Hagevi Reference Hagevi2015; Kekkonen and Ylä-Anttila Reference Kekkonen and Ylä-Anttila2021; Simón Reference Simón2020).

For each ideological group, we developed tailored messages to reflect the narratives most relevant to their respective ideological positions. These narratives were derived from two primary sources: open-ended responses collected earlier in the study and existing literature in sociology and marketing. The open-ended responses provided direct insights into the themes and symbols the participants associated with nostalgia (see Van der Velden et al. Reference Van der Velden2023), while the literature offered a broader theoretical foundation to shape these messages. Specifically, insights from studies on the modalities of nostalgia, the role of belonging and the sociocultural dimensions of nostalgic messaging informed the construction of the experimental stimuli (Hartmann and Brunk Reference Hartmann and Brunk2019; May Reference May2017; Pickering and Keightley Reference Pickering and Keightley2006).

The experimental design followed a 2 × 2 factorial framework. The messages varied along two key dimensions: whether they invoked nostalgia or not, and whether they included a scapegoating element or not. This set-up resulted in four distinct experimental groups, allowing us to disentangle the effects of nostalgia and scapegoating independently and in combination. The participants of the two groups were presented with messages specifically tailored to resonate with their ideological perspective (right-wing conservative or left-wing progressive). These messages emphasized either nostalgia or scapegoating or both nostalgia and scapegoating or neither nostalgia nor scapegoating, depending on the assigned condition.

To ensure the robustness of the findings, additional analyses were conducted to account for the potential influence of the participants who identified as centrists. These analyses involved running the data both with and without centrists, and the results demonstrated that the inclusion or exclusion of this group did not substantively affect the outcomes (see the Supplementary Material, Online Appendix C). For further details about the experimental design and procedures, see the Supplementary Material, Online Compendium and Online Appendix A.

We randomly assigned the participants to see just one of four different potential campaign messages (see Figure 1), telling them that a party close to their position was testing campaigning material for the next Dutch elections. Figure 2 demonstrates that the participants who voted for more conservative parties (as ranked by Kieskompas), displayed in the top lines of Figure 2, also positioned themselves as more conservative, and the voters who supported more progressive parties positioned themselves as more progressive. An example of the treatment text for a left-wing voter, including nostalgia and a scapegoat, reads as follows (see Online Appendix A for all the treatment texts):

Looking back, we remember a time when solidarity, workers’ rights and respect for the environment were paramount. People lived a slow and peaceful life in harmony with nature, valuing community over individualism. Today, we see a world plagued by pollution, polarization and stress, caused in large part by large corporations, contamination lobbyists and neoliberal policymakers. It’s time to hold them accountable again for their actions and reclaim those values of solidarity, respect and peace that made our world a better place. Let’s create a world where workers’ rights are protected again, the environment is re-cherished, and the well-being of all people is prioritized again over the interests of a select few.

Figure 2. Baseline of Self-Placement into Ideological Positions

Dependent variables

To examine the affective consequences of nostalgic and scapegoating messages, we measured the participants’ affective sentiment towards 10 social in-groups and out-groups. Specifically, we asked them to indicate the extent to which they experienced positive or negative feelings towards the following groups: big companies, Christians, climate activists, immigrants, left-wing people, materialistic people, Muslims, neoliberal politicians, right-wing people and people living on state support. These groups were referenced in the treatment texts as scapegoats for each ideological group.

While none of these groups is exclusively or equally associated with a particular ideological position, they are all commonly scapegoated for contemporary political issues. This aligns with research demonstrating how ideological groups are increasingly tied to social identity groups, through either identification with or ‘disidentification’ from them (Westheuser and Zollinger Reference Westheuser and Zollinger2024; Zollinger Reference Zollinger2024).

Affective sentiment towards these groups was measured on a scale from zero (very negative) to 10 (very positive). We considered the left-wing progressive in-groups (and the right-wing conservative out-groups) to include climate activists, immigrants, left-wing people, Muslims and people receiving state support. Conversely, we considered the right-wing conservative in-groups (and the left-wing progressive out-groups) to include big companies, Christians, materialistic people, neoliberal politicians and right-wing people.

The correlation between alignment with conservative in-groups (and progressive out-groups) and the participants’ ideology was 0.3, while the correlation between alignment with progressive in-groups (and conservative out-groups) and the participants’ ideology was 0.5. The lower correlation for conservative in-groups suggests greater heterogeneity in how conservatives relate to their supposed in-groups, compared to the stronger alignment with their in-groups found among progressives.

These correlation values indicate a moderate alignment between the participants’ ideological self-identification and their affective attitudes towards the designated in-groups and out-groups. In other words, the individuals with a conservative ideological orientation tended to have more favourable feelings towards their in-groups (e.g. big companies and Christians) and less favourable feelings towards their out-groups (e.g. climate activists and immigrants). The individuals with a progressive ideological orientation exhibited the same trend in feelings towards their own in-groups and out-groups. Although the correlation values are not exceptionally high, they provide sufficient evidence to support the categorization of these groups as ideological in-groups and out-groups for the purposes of this study.

Methods of estimation

We used a multilevel model with the participants clustered in social groups to test our hypotheses. Based on a balance test (see the Supplementary Material for our Online Compendium and Online Appendix B), the only unbalanced demographic covariate we included in our analysis was ‘urbanization’. In the Results section, we only present the visualized coefficients of the experimental treatment (see our Online Compendium for the full regression tables).

Affective consequences of a nostalgic narrative

First, we delve into the descriptive analysis of attitudes towards social groups across political ideology. Figure 3 shows the attitudes towards social groups held by members of conservative and progressive groups, which provide a baseline for the subsequent analysis. It indicates a clear pattern of affective polarization: the left panel shows clearly that right-wing voters (pink dots) are rated most negatively by individuals identified as ‘very progressive’ and most positively by individuals identified as ‘very conservative’. Similarly, the right panel shows that left-wing voters (green dots) are rated most positively by individuals identified as ‘very progressive’ and most negatively by individuals identified as ‘very conservative’. This aligns with the definition of affective polarization (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar2019), as the conservative participants evaluated their in-group (i.e. right-wing voters) more positively than their out-group (i.e. left-wing voters), while the progressive participants evaluated their in-group (i.e. left-wing voters) more positively than their out-group (i.e. right-wing voters).

Figure 3. Descriptive Analysis of Social Groups

Beyond the left–right distinction, our examination of the sentiments of right-wing conservative supporters towards other social groups revealed a similar trend: greater warmth towards Christians, multinationals (many of which have a significant presence in the Netherlands), neoliberal politicians and fellow right-wing voters. However, nuanced variations exist within these sentiments. Notably, the most linear variation in warmth towards right-wing voters can be seen as one moves from left-wing progressive party supporters to right-wing conservative party supporters. It is noteworthy that Christians garnered a generally positive sentiment across the political spectrum. Additionally, materialistic individuals, neoliberal politicians and multinationals did not receive uniformly positive evaluations, even from right-wing party supporters.

Turning our attention to the right side of Figure 3, a similar narrative unfolds. The more politically left-leaning the participants, the more positive their sentiments towards social groups associated with progressive ideologies. Notable entities receiving favourable evaluations include climate activists, immigrants, Muslims, individuals living on benefits and other left-wing voters. However, nuances in sentiment exist within this category as well. Some groups, such as individuals living on benefits, received relatively neutral evaluations from right-wing supporters. Conversely, climate activists, immigrants, Muslims and left-wing voters were viewed with varying degrees of antipathy by the right.

Figure 4 shows the distribution and average affective sentiment towards the social groups of the message per experimental treatment. Social in-groups were more liked than out-groups – with scores of five and lower for the disliked social group and six and higher for the liked social group. The distributions indicate that, on average, the progressive voters liked their in-group more than the conservative voters liked their in-group, regardless of the experimental treatment. However, there was no difference between progressive and conservative voters regarding their social out-groups: they both disliked them equally. Again, there was little variation in levels of support for the social out-group between the messages. This analysis establishes a crucial baseline for our subsequent examination of the effects of the treatment, as it outlines the reference point for changes in sentiments that may arise.

Figure 4. Affective Sentiment for Social Groups

To test whether a nostalgic narrative of ‘us and them’ has polarizing consequences, we present the results of our pre-registered experiment. The left-hand panel in Figure 5 demonstrates the support for social in-groups based on the treatment message. In general, neither the nostalgic messages (H1a) nor the scapegoating messages (H1b) resulted in a statistically significant increase in affective sentiment for the social in-groups. While the effect of the scapegoating messages (H1b) went in the hypothesized direction, it was not statistically significant. The nostalgic messages actually did elicit a statistically significant effect, but in the opposite direction to the hypothesis (H1a): being exposed to a nostalgic message (versus a non-nostalgic message) made the participants feel colder towards their in-group. Hence, we did not find any support for H1a or H1b. The analysis of our pre-registered experiment, as depicted in Figure 5, aimed to assess whether a nostalgic narrative framing of ‘us’ and ‘them’ engenders polarizing consequences, particularly concerning the support for social in-groups.

Figure 5. Predicted Affective Sentiment for Social Groups

To look deeper into this, we ran separate analyses for progressive and conservative voters per social group and found that the progressive voters were an exception (see Online Appendix C). When progressive voters received a nostalgic scapegoating message, although this did not materially affect their feelings towards groups such as left-wing voters and immigrants, it did seem to make them feel more negative about Muslims and more positive about climate activists. This could indicate that while both groups are generally seen more positively by progressives than by conservatives, climate activists are seen as an in-group that resists nostalgic and scapegoating pushes, whereas Muslims are probably seen more as strategic allies for whom at least some voters of the left signal instrumental and fragile positive feelings. Conservative voters also showed an interesting pattern. Under the nostalgic message treatment, especially when it also included scapegoating, they had a less positive response to neoliberal politicians, multinationals and materialistic people. This pattern could indicate that even if these groups are not necessarily out-groups for conservative voters, such voters may still see them as partly responsible for the decline perceived through their nostalgic lens. The implications of this pattern underscore the nuanced dynamics at play within the conservative voter demographic. In a broader context, the findings shed light on the multifaceted nature of conservative voter attitudes.

In conclusion, the lack of support for H1a and H1b in our study, as well as the subgroup analyses, provide valuable insights into the nuanced dynamics of political messaging and its differential impact on diverse voter groups. It is crucial to recognize these subtleties in order to understand the complexities of political sentiment and to avoid broad generalizations about the existence of a populist Zeitgeist based on the examined experimental conditions.

The right-hand panel in Figure 5 demonstrates support for social out-groups after exposure to nostalgic and scapegoating messages. The nostalgic messages (H2a) had no effect on how the participants felt towards their social out-groups: the coefficient is zero. The scapegoating messages (H2b), however, did have the hypothesized polarizing effect: they made the participants colder towards their out-groups. This is an indication that a ‘purely’ nostalgic message could foster more understanding towards some ideological out-groups and that any scapegoating element in a nostalgic narrative could diminish any positive affective sentiment towards the out-groups achieved by the nostalgic appeal. In essence, this suggests that people feel more affectively polarized towards their out-groups when they are exposed to a message that puts the blame on someone else.

Online Appendix D presents the subgroup analyses for conservative and progressive voters. The data show that the nostalgic messages (red coefficients) were associated with an increased affective sentiment towards right-wing voters and Christians (the latter without statistical significance). Combining the nostalgic message with scapegoating (green coefficients) decreased the affective sentiment towards out-groups: the progressive voters became more negative towards right-wing voters (without statistical significance), neoliberal politicians, multinationals and Christians (without statistical significance). Only a scapegoating message (in blue) decreased the sentiment towards right-wing voters, neoliberal politicians, multinationals, Christians and materialistic people – with only the latter being statistically significant.

Hence, while our data do not support H2a, they do support H2b. The subgroup analyses show a more nuanced pattern: nostalgic and scapegoating messages decreased the affective sentiment towards some, but not all, of the groups theorized as out-groups. In light of these results, it is essential to acknowledge the complexity of the impact of political messaging on affective sentiment towards social out-groups. Our study does not decisively indicate the presence of a populist Zeitgeist based on the examined experimental conditions. The nuanced and varied responses observed underscore the importance of considering contextual factors and the diverse nature of political attitudes within the broader population. We explored whether our results are conditional upon ideology, party support, populist attitudes, education and gender, but we did not find evidence for a relationship between nostalgia or scapegoating and affective polarization.

Conclusion

In this article, we argue that a persistent populist sentiment is fuelling the politicization of out-groups, resulting in heightened affective polarization. Central to this phenomenon is the mainstreaming of nostalgia as a prevalent rhetorical device, serving as a conduit for altering group affect. Specifically, we hypothesized that nostalgic messaging, whether explicitly mentioning scapegoats or not, would amplify affect for social in-groups while diminishing affect for social out-groups. Our experimental findings revealed a more nuanced relationship than that hypothesized between nostalgic messaging, affective polarization, and sentiment towards in-group and out-group entities.

Contrary to our initial expectations, nostalgic messaging did not significantly impact feelings towards out-groups, but it did decrease affect for the participants’ in-groups. This suggests that nostalgia does not exacerbate negative sentiments towards already disliked groups but that it fosters a pessimistic sentiment across all social groups (including in-groups), which in turn depolarizes citizens by reducing the emotional distance between in-groups and out-groups. Regarding scapegoating, our results aligned more closely with our expectations. While scapegoating did not yield a statistically significant effect on attitudes towards social in-groups, it seemed to prompt markedly more negative attitudes towards social out-groups. This finding corroborates previous theoretical insights regarding how politicians can foment social animosity by targeting specific social groups as the source of current social issues (Iyengar et al. Reference Iyengar2019).

These findings contribute to our understanding of contemporary politics. We observe that nostalgic narratives, whether or not they contain an element of scapegoating, exert a substantial influence on individuals’ sentiments towards social groups. Interestingly, nostalgia itself appears to depolarize individuals by fostering a pervasive negativity towards social groups in general, including in-groups. This insight is crucial for the ongoing academic discourse on affective polarization, as we demonstrate that heightened negative affect and affective polarization need not necessarily coexist. It underscores the normative query regarding priority – whether the goal is to reduce the emotional distance between social groups, in which case nostalgia proves an effective tool, or to mitigate negative affect towards specific groups, a purpose which nostalgia operates against. In essence, our study contributes to the call for greater clarity in delineating the independent consequences of nostalgia and its relationship with affective polarization (Wagner Reference Wagner2024).

Additionally, we underscore the pervasive impact of scapegoating narratives, which, even without bolstering in-group pride, heighten negativity towards the out-group. The fact that we observe the negative out-group effect only for scapegoating messages but not for nostalgic messages suggests that nostalgia may not inherently exacerbate out-group derogation. Rather, it appears that explicit targeting of an out-group is necessary for nostalgia to have such an effect. This underscores the need for further exploration of nostalgia, its components and its applicability to diverse contexts. While our study only examines the effects of nostalgic messages on ideological in-groups, it is plausible that such messages, even when aimed at motivating in-group voting, may reach individuals from other political affiliations and influence them. Future research should delve into this aspect.

In conclusion, our examination of the reliance of populist rhetoric on creating divisions between in-groups and out-groups, often utilizing the out-group as a scapegoat, aligns with recent research emphasizing the pivotal role of nostalgia in this narrative construction (Elçi Reference Elçi2022; Smeekes et al. Reference Smeekes, Wildschut and Sedikides2021; Van Prooijen et al. Reference Van Prooijen2022). Our study sheds light on how nostalgic messaging reinforces affective negativity, although only against the in-group. This implies that nostalgia attenuates divisions within society by making citizens colder towards all social groups, including those groups of which they are members. However, our results confirm our expectation that scapegoating intensifies negative sentiments towards social out-groups. The absence of a similar negative effect of nostalgia on out-groups suggests that some of the effects attributed to nostalgia may hinge on the explicit identification of perceived culprits of social decline in nostalgic narratives. Ultimately, our findings underscore the critical need to scrutinize the impact of rhetoric on societal attitudes and behaviours. We urge policymakers and citizens alike to consider the broader implications of nostalgic narratives in the political arena.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/gov.2025.10020.

Data availability

The Online Compendium is available at https://github.com/MarikenvdVelden/populism-polarization-nostalgia.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the participants at the workshop ‘Through Thick and Thin: Bridging Innovative Conceptual and Methodological Approaches in Studying Programmatic and Discursive Party Competition’ organized by Fabian Habersack and Sophia Hunger as part of the ECPR Joint Sessions in Toulouse (25–28 April 2023) for their valuable feedback, stimulating discussions and constructive suggestions. We are also grateful to the Political Communication Research Group at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam for their ongoing support and insightful input throughout this project.

References

Aalberg, T et al. (2016) Populist Political Communication in Europe. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315623016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balcells, L and Kuo, A (2023) Secessionist Conflict and Affective Polarization: Evidence from Catalonia. Journal of Peace Research 60(4), 604618. https://doi.org/10.1177/00223433221088112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, M, White, MH and Sullivan, D (2018) Nostalgia for America’s past Can Buffer Collective Guilt. European Journal of Social Psychology 48(4), 433446. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bantel, I (2023) Camps, Not Just Parties. The Dynamic Foundations of Affective Polarization in Multi-party Systems. Electoral Studies 83, 102614. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.electstud.2023.102614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, S (2021) The Causes of Populism in the West. Annual Review of Political Science 24, 7188. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-041719-102503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonikowski, B (2017) Ethno-Nationalist Populism and the Mobilization of Collective Resentment. British Journal of Sociology 68, S181S213. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-4446.12325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonikowski, B and Stuhler, O (2022) Reclaiming the Past to Transcend the Present: Nostalgic Appeals in US Presidential Elections. Sociological Forum 37, 12631293. https://doi.org/10.1111/socf.12838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bor, A, Jørgensen, F and Petersen, MB (2023a) Discriminatory Attitudes against Unvaccinated People during the Pandemic. Nature 613(7945), 704711. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05607-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bor, A, Jørgensen, F and Petersen, MB (2023b) Moralizing the COVID-19 Pandemic: Self-Interest Predicts Moral Condemnation of Other’s Compliance, Distancing, and Vaccination. Political Psychology 44(2), 257279. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, W-Y et al. (2017) Collective Nostalgia Is Associated with Stronger Outgroup-Directed Anger and Participation in Ingroup-Favoring Collective Action. Journal of Social and Political Psychology 5(2), Article 2. https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v5i2.697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comellas, JM and Torcal, M (2023) Ideological Identity, Issue-Based Ideology and Bipolar Affective Polarization in Multiparty Systems: The Cases of Argentina, Chile, Italy, Portugal and Spain. Electoral Studies 83, 102615. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.electstud.2023.102615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, BL et al. (2022) Examining the Relationship between Roe v. Wade Knowledge and Sentiment across Political Party and Abortion Identity. Sexuality Research & Social Policy 19(3), 837848. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-021-00597-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine-Wright, P (2009) Rethinking NIMBYism: The Role of Place Attachment and Place Identity in Explaining Place-protective Action. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology 19(6), 426441. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dias, N and Lelkes, Y (2022) The Nature of Affective Polarization: Disentangling Policy Disagreement from Partisan Identity. American Journal of Political Science 66(3), 775790. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elçi, E (2022) Politics of Nostalgia and Populism: Evidence from Turkey. British Journal of Political Science 52(2), 697714. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123420000666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, C and Stimson, JA (2012) Ideology in America. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139094009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerber, AS (2011) Field experiments and natural experiments. In Druckman, JN, Green, DP, Kuklinski, JH and Lupia, A (eds), Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science. pp. 115140. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511921452.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagevi, M (2015) Bloc Identification in Multi-Party Systems: The Case of the Swedish Two-Bloc System. West European Politics 38(1), 7392. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2014.911480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hameleers, M, Bos, L and De Vreese, CH (2017) ‘They Did It’: The Effects of Emotionalized Blame Attribution in Populist Communication. Communication Research 44(6), 870900. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650216644026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harteveld, E (2021) Fragmented Foes: Affective Polarization in the Multiparty Context of the Netherlands. Electoral Studies 71, 102332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.electstud.2021.102332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, R et al. (2022) Interventions to Reduce Partisan Animosity. Nature Human Behaviour 6(9), 11941205. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01442-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartmann, BJ and Brunk, KH (2019) Nostalgia Marketing and (Re-)Enchantment. International Journal of Research in Marketing 36(4), 669686. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijresmar.2019.05.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinterleitner, M, Kammermeier, V and Moffitt, B (2024) How the Populist Radical Right Exploits Crisis: Comparing the Role of Proximity in the COVID-19 and Refugee Crises in Germany. West European Politics 47(7), 15031528. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2023.2275892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobolt, SB, Leeper, TJ and Tilley, J (2021) Divided by the Vote: Affective Polarization in the Wake of the Brexit Referendum. British Journal of Political Science 51(4), 14761493. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123420000125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunger, S and Paxton, F (2022) What’s in a Buzzword? A Systematic Review of the State of Populism Research in Political Science. Political Science Research and Methods 10(3), 617633. https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2021.44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, RF and Norris, P (2016) Trump, Brexit, and the Rise of Populism: Economic Have-Nots and Cultural Backlash. HKS Faculty Research Working Paper No. RWP16-026. Harvard Kennedy School. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2818659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, S et al. (2019) The Origins and Consequences of Affective Polarization in the United States. Annual Review of Political Science 22, 129146. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-051117-073034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, JT, Baldassarri, DS and Druckman, JN (2022) Cognitive–Motivational Mechanisms of Political Polarization in Social-Communicative Contexts. Nature Reviews Psychology 1(10), 560576. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00093-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kao, F (2012) An Exploratory Study of Collective Nostalgia. In Gürhan-Canli, Z, Otnes, C and Zhu, RJ (eds), Advances in Consumer Research. Vol. 40, pp. 514518, Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research.Google Scholar
Kekkonen, A and Ylä-Anttila, T (2021) Affective Blocs: Understanding Affective Polarization in Multiparty Systems. Electoral Studies 72, 102367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.electstud.2021.102367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lammers, J and Baldwin, M (2018) Past-Focused Temporal Communication Overcomes Conservatives’ Resistance to Liberal Political Ideas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 114(4), 599. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchegiani, C and Phau, I (2013) Development and Validation of the Personal Nostalgia Scale. Journal of Marketing Communications 19(1), 2243. https://doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2010.542078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, L (2018) Ideologues without Issues: The Polarizing Consequences of Ideological Identities. Public Opinion Quarterly 82(S1), 866887. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfy005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, V (2017) Belonging from Afar: Nostalgia, Time and Memory. Sociological Review 65(2), 401415. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-954X.12402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mudde, C (2004) The Populist Zeitgeist. Government and Opposition: An International Journal of Comparative Politics 39(4), 541563. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1477-7053.2004.00135.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mudde, C and Rovira Kaltwasser, C (2013) Exclusionary vs. Inclusionary Populism: Comparing Contemporary Europe and Latin America. Government and Opposition: An International Journal of Comparative Politics 48(2), 147174. https://doi.org/10.1017/gov.2012.11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, S and Proksch, S-O (2024) Nostalgia in European Party Politics: A Text-Based Measurement Approach. British Journal of Political Science 54(3), 9931005. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123423000571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noury, A and Roland, G (2020) Identity Politics and Populism in Europe. Annual Review of Political Science 23, 421439. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-polisci-050718-033542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oshri, O, Yair, O and Huddy, L (2022) The Importance of Attachment to an Ideological Group in Multi-Party Systems: Evidence from Israel. Party Politics 28(6), 11641175. https://doi.org/10.1177/13540688211044475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M and Keightley, E (2006) The Modalities of Nostalgia. Current Sociology 54(6), 919941. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011392106068458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schumacher, G, Rooduijn, M and Bakker, BN (2022) Hot Populism? Affective Responses to Antiestablishment Rhetoric. Political Psychology 43(5), 851871. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shehaj, A, Shin, AJ and Inglehart, R (2021) Immigration and Right-Wing Populism: An Origin Story. Party Politics 27(2), 282293. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354068819849888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simón, P (2020) Two-Bloc Logic, Polarisation and Coalition Government: The November 2019 General Election in Spain. South European Society and Politics 25(3-4), 533563. https://doi.org/10.1080/13608746.2020.1857085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smeekes, A and Verkuyten, M (2017) The Presence of the Past: Identity Continuity and Group Dynamics. European Review of Social Psychology 28(1), 162202. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2017.1380264.Google Scholar
Smeekes, A, Verkuyten, M and Martinovic, B (2015) Longing for the Country’s Good Old Days: National Nostalgia, Autochthony Beliefs, and Opposition to Muslim Expressive Rights. British Journal of Social Psychology 54(3), 561580. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeekes, A, Wildschut, T and Sedikides, C (2021) Longing for the ‘Good Old Days’ of Our Country: National Nostalgia as a New Master-Frame of Populist Radical Right Parties. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology 5(2), 90102. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefaniak, A et al. (2021) Different Pasts for Different Political Folk: Political Orientation Predicts Collective Nostalgia Content. Frontiers in Political Science 3, 633688. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpos.2021.633688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taggart, P (2018) Populism and ‘Unpolitics. In Fitzi, G, Mackert, J and Turner, B (eds), Populism and the Crisis of Democracy: Volume 1: Concepts and Theory. pp. 7987. Routledge. London. https://doi.org/10.4324/97813151080706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H and Turner, J (1979) An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Conflict. In Austin, WG and Worchel, S (eds), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole, pp. 3347.Google Scholar
Tilley, J and Hobolt, SB (2023) Brexit as an Identity: Political Identities and Policy Norms. PS: Political Science & Politics 56(4), 546552. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1049096523000367.Google Scholar
Van der Velden, MACG et al. (2023) Do We All Long for the Past? Investigating Nostalgia as a Persuasive Political Rhetorical Strategy. OSF Preprint, 27 October, https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/3ka95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, J-W et al. (2022) Make It Great Again: The Relationship between Populist Attitudes and Nostalgia. Political Psychology 43(5), 951968. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Spanje, J (2011) The Wrong and the Right: A Comparative Analysis of ‘Anti-Immigration’ and ‘Far Right’ Parties. Government and Opposition: An International Journal of Comparative Politics 46(3), 293320. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1477-7053.2011.01340.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, M (2024) Affective Polarization in Europe. European Political Science Review 16(3), 378392. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755773923000383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, M and Eberl, J (2022) Divided by the Jab: On the Nature, Origins, and Consequences of COVID-19 Vaccination Identities. OSF Preprint, 20 April. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/zcas8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westheuser, L and Zollinger, D (2024) Cleavage Theory Meets Bourdieu: Studying the Role of Group Identities in Cleavage Formation. European Political Science Review 17(1), 110127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755773924000249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widmann, T (2021) How Emotional are Populists Really? Factors Explaining Emotional Appeals in the Communication of Political Parties. Political Psychology 42(1), 163181. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12693.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wodak, R (2015) The Politics of Fear: What Right-Wing Populist Discourses Mean. London: Sage.10.4135/9781446270073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zollinger, D (2024) Cleavage Identities in Voters’ Own Words: Harnessing Open-Ended Survey Responses. American Journal of Political Science 68(1), 139159. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Overview of Experimental Procedure

Figure 1

Figure 2. Baseline of Self-Placement into Ideological Positions

Figure 2

Figure 3. Descriptive Analysis of Social Groups

Figure 3

Figure 4. Affective Sentiment for Social Groups

Figure 4

Figure 5. Predicted Affective Sentiment for Social Groups

Supplementary material: File

van der Velden et al. supplementary material

van der Velden et al. supplementary material
Download van der Velden et al. supplementary material(File)
File 380.4 KB