Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-42vt5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-12T09:40:56.860Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 8 - The Childhood Roots of Vaccine Hesitance and Resistance

A Five-Decade Cohort Study

from Part III - Life-Course Development from Prenatal Environment through Childhood to Adulthood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 September 2025

Satoshi Kanazawa
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science
Get access

Summary

Jay Belsky’s career has always focused on important social and behavioral challenges, and Jay specialized in showing how childhood experience shapes people’s behavior in the face of such challenges. Here we describe a project that could have been done by Jay himself, and that drew on ideas shared with us by Jay over the years of our friendship. The social problem we studied emerged from the 2020–21 COVID-19 pandemic: resistance to the vaccines. We carried out the project in the five-decade Dunedin Study, where we compared groups who differed in their intentions toward the COVID-19 vaccine in the weeks before vaccines became available. We found that vaccine-resistant and vaccine-hesitant cohort members had histories of adverse childhood experiences that fostered mistrust of authority, early-life mental-health problems that fostered misinterpretation of health messages, and early-life personality styles including tendencies to have extreme negative emotions, shut down mentally under stress, to value being a nonconformist, and to be fatalistic about health. Making matters worse, many Vaccine-Resistant and Vaccine-Hesitant participants also had difficulty cognitively comprehending health information. We found that negative vaccine intentions are not short-term misunderstandings that can be readily cleared up by delivering more information to adults in the midst of a public-health crisis. Instead they are part of a person’s lifelong psychological style of misinterpreting information and making unhealthy decisions during stressful uncertain situations. The key contribution from our study is the appreciation that this style is laid down well before secondary school age. To prepare for pandemics of the future, education about viruses and vaccines in schools could reduce citizens’ level of uncertainty and fear during a pandemic and give people preexisting knowledge that prevents shutdown under emotional distress and enhances their capacity to hear health messages.

Information

Type
Chapter
Information
Genes, Environments, and Differential Susceptibility
Current Topics in Evolutionary Developmental Psychology
, pp. 176 - 192
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Book purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Adashi, E.Y., & Cohen, I.G. (2022). The pandemic preparedness program: Reimagining public health. JAMA. 327(3):219220. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.23656CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Gale, C. R., & Altschul, D. (2021). Pre-pandemic cognitive function and COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy: Cohort study. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 96, 100105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2021.05.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, N. T., Chapman, G. B., Rothman, A. J., Leask, J., & Kempe, A. (2017). Increasing vaccination: Putting psychological science into action. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 18(3), 149207. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100618760521CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browne, M., Thomson, P., Rockloff, M. J., & Pennycook, G. (2015). Going against the herd: Psychological and cultural factors underlying the ‘vaccination confidence gap’. PLoS One, 10(9), e0132562. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132562CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cascini, F., Pantovic, A., Al-Ajlouni, Y., Failla, G., & Ricciardi, W. (2021). Attitudes, acceptance and hesitancy among the general population worldwide to receive the COVID-19 vaccines and their contributing factors: A systematic review. EClinicalMedicine, 40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2021.101113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dai, H., Saccardo, S., Han, M. A., Roh, L., Raja, N., Vangala, S., … Croymans, D. M. (2021). Behavioural nudges increase COVID-19 vaccinations. Nature, 597(7876), 404409. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03843-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ecker, U. K. H., Lewandowsky, S., Cook, J., Schmid, P., Fazio, L. K., Brashier, N., Kendeou, P., Vraga, E. K., & Amazeen, M. A. (2022). The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 1329. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00006-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Loe, B. S., Chadwick, A., Vaccari, C., Waite, F., Rosebrock, L., … Lambe, S. (2022). COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: The Oxford coronavirus explanations, attitudes, and narratives survey (Oceans) II. Psychological Medicine, 52(14), 31273141. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720005188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galea, S. (2021). The contagion next time. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hunter, C. M., Chou, W. S., & Webb Hooper, M. (2021). Behavioral and social science in support of SARS-CoV-2 vaccination: National Institutes of Health initiatives. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 11(7), 13541358. https://doi.org/10.1093/tbm/ibab067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, N., & Oyserman, D. (2016). Using identity-based motivation to improve the nation’s health without breaking the bank. Behavioral Science & Policy, 2, 2538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milkman, K. L., Patel, M. S., Gandhi, L., Graci, H. N., Gromet, D. M., Ho, H., … Duckworth, A. L. (2021). A megastudy of text-based nudges encouraging patients to get vaccinated at an upcoming doctor’s appointment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(20), e2101165118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2101165118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, C., Ambler, B., Harrington, H., Houts, R., & Wood, P. (2022) Deep-seated psychological histories of COVID-19 vaccine hesitance and resistance, PNAS-Nexus. https://academic.oup.com/pnasnexus/advance-article/doi/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac034/6553423Google Scholar
Murphy, J., Vallières, F., Bentall, R.P. et al. (2021). Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Nature Communications 12, 29. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-20226-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, & Medicine. (2021a). Communication strategies for building confidence in COVID-19 vaccines: Addressing variants and childhood vaccinations. The National Academies Press. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, & Medicine.Google Scholar
Nuffield Council on Bioethics. (2021). Vaccine access and uptake. Nuffield Council on Bioethics.Google Scholar
Nuti, S. V., & Armstrong, K. (2021). Lay epidemiology and vaccine acceptance. JAMA, 326(4), 301302. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.11130CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuzzo, JB, Gostin, LO. (2022). The first 2 years of COVID-19: Lessons to improve preparedness for the next pandemic. JAMA, 327(3):217218. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.24394CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, E., Steptoe, A., & Fancourt, D. (2021). Attitudes towards vaccines and intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Implications for public health communications. The Lancet Regional Health–Europe, 1, 2666-7762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lanepe.2020.100012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulton, R., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (2015). The Dunedin multidisciplinary health and development study: Overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 50(5), 679693. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-015-1048-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, E. (2003). Diffusion of innovations, 5th edition. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Sgaier, S. (2021). Meet the kinds of people holding us back from full vaccination. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2021/05/18/opinion/covid-19-vaccine-hesitancy.htmlGoogle Scholar
Szilagyi, P. G., Thomas, K., Shah, M. D., Vizueta, N., Cui, Y., Vangala, S., & Kapteyn, A. (2021). Changes in COVID-19 vaccine intent from April/May to June/July 2021. JAMA, 326(19), 19711974. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.18761CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tibbetts, J. (2021). How to convince people to accept a Covid-19 vaccine. Knowable Magazine from the Annual Reviews. Retrieved from https://knowablemagazine.org/article/health-disease/2021/how-to-convince-people-to-accept-covid19-vaccineCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tufekci, Z. (2021a). 5 Pandemic mistakes we keep repeating. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/ideas/archive/2021/02/how-public-health-messaging-backfired/618147/Google Scholar
Tufekci, Z. (2021b). The unvaccinated may not be who you think. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2021/10/15/opinion/covid-vaccines-unvaccinated.html?referringSource=articleShareGoogle Scholar
Uscinski, J., & Parent, J. (2014). Conspiracy theories are for losers. In American Conspiracy Theories. Oxford University Press. https://oxford.universitypressscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199351800.001.0001/acprof-9780199351800-chapter-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0884-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volpp, K. G., Loewenstein, G., & Buttenheim, A. M. (2021). Behaviorally informed strategies for a national COVID-19 vaccine promotion program. JAMA, 325(2), 125126. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2020.24036Google ScholarPubMed
Wood, S. (2012). Prone to progress: Using personality to identify supporters of innovative social entrepreneurship. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 31(1), 129141. https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.11.060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, S., Pate, M. A., & Schulman, K. (2021). Novel strategies to support global promotion of COVID-19 vaccination. BMJ Global Health, 6(10), e006066. http://gh.bmj.com/content/6/10/e006066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, S., & Schulman, K. (2021a). Beyond politics – Promoting Covid-19 vaccination in the United States. New England Journal of Medicine, 384(7), e23. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMms2033790CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, S., & Schulman, K. (2021b). When vaccine apathy, not hesitancy, drives vaccine disinterest. JAMA, 325(24), 24352436. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.7707CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Accessibility standard: WCAG 2.0 A

The PDF of this book conforms to version 2.0 of the Web Content Accessibility Guidelines (WCAG), ensuring core accessibility principles are addressed and meets the basic (A) level of WCAG compliance, addressing essential accessibility barriers.

Content Navigation

Table of contents navigation
Allows you to navigate directly to chapters, sections, or non‐text items through a linked table of contents, reducing the need for extensive scrolling.
Index navigation
Provides an interactive index, letting you go straight to where a term or subject appears in the text without manual searching.

Reading Order & Textual Equivalents

Single logical reading order
You will encounter all content (including footnotes, captions, etc.) in a clear, sequential flow, making it easier to follow with assistive tools like screen readers.
Short alternative textual descriptions
You get concise descriptions (for images, charts, or media clips), ensuring you do not miss crucial information when visual or audio elements are not accessible.
Full alternative textual descriptions
You get more than just short alt text: you have comprehensive text equivalents, transcripts, captions, or audio descriptions for substantial non‐text content, which is especially helpful for complex visuals or multimedia.

Visual Accessibility

Use of colour is not sole means of conveying information
You will still understand key ideas or prompts without relying solely on colour, which is especially helpful if you have colour vision deficiencies.
Use of high contrast between text and background colour
You benefit from high‐contrast text, which improves legibility if you have low vision or if you are reading in less‐than‐ideal lighting conditions.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×